On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 23 Issue 1 Proceedings of the 40th Annual Penn Article 2 Linguistics Conference 2017 On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic Faruk Akkuş University of Pennsylvania Balkız Öztürk Boğaziçi University Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Akkuş, Faruk and Öztürk, Balkız (2017) "On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 23 : Iss. 1 , Article 2. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss1/2 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss1/2 For more information, please contact [email protected]. On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic Abstract This paper investigates the patterns of cognate objects (COs) associated with unergatives and unaccusatives in Sason Arabic. We propose that COs of both unergatives and unaccusatives are not true arguments, as evinced by their highly productive and unrestricted use, but constitute rhematic complements in the sense of Ramchand (2008), therefore cannot be used as diagnostics for unergative-unaccusative distinction in the language. This working paper is available in University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss1/2 On Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic Faruk Akkuş and Balkız Öztürk 1 Introduction This paper investigates the patterns of cognate objects (COs) associated with unergatives and unac- cusatives in Sason Arabic, an endangered Arabic dialect spoken in eastern Turkey (Jastrow 2006, Akkuş 2016). We propose that COs of both unergatives and unaccusatives are not true arguments, but consti- tute rhematic complements in the sense of Ramchand (2008), therefore cannot be used as a diagnos- tics for unergative-unaccusative distinction in the language. 2 Previous Literature on Cognate Objects (1) and (2) illustrate examples of COs from the familiar languages such as English and French. (1) a. John danced a (slow) dance. b. Mary sang a (beautiful) song. (2) a. Il a danse une grande danse. he has danced a grand dance. ‘He danced a grand dance.’ (Pereltsvaig 2002, (2)) b. Elle a chante une (belle) chanson. she has sung a beautiful song ‘She sang a beautiful song.’ As can be seen from the above examples, COs are noun phrases containing a noun which is morphologically related to the verb. In English, this noun can sometimes be the exact copy of the verb (as in the case of smile a smile, laugh a laugh, and dance a dance). Two central questions that have been the focus of the previous research on COs are (i) what can they tell us about the predicate- types? and (ii) are they arguments or adjuncts? The widely-held generalization about the occurrence of cognate objects is stated in Kuno and Takami (2004:107) as the Unergative Restriction on the Cognate Object Construction: (3) Unergative Restriction on the Cognate Object Construction Only unergative verbs can appear in the cognate object construction. No unaccusative verbs can. This generalization has been used as a diagnostics to differentiate between unergative and un- accusative verbs. It has been argued that intransitives which can take a cognate object are typically unergative verbs, i.e., verbs whose subjects are (non-)volitional initiators, but not unaccusatives with undergoer subjects. (See furthermore Keyser and Roeper 1984, Larson 1988, Massam 1992, Levin and Rappaport Hovav 1995, and Hale and Keyser 1993, among others.) (4) a. Malinda smiled her most enigmatic smile. (Levin and Rappaport Hovav, 1995:40) b. The baby slept a sound sleep. (Nakajima 2006:677) However, Kuno and Takami (2004:116, also in Nakajima 2006) observe that some unaccusative We would like to thank the audience at LAEL’15 at Boğaziçi University, Istanbul for their constructive comments on the earlier version of this paper. U. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics, Volume 23.1, 2017 2 FARUK AKKUŞ AND BALKIZ ÖZTÜRK verbs can occur with cognate objects. (5) a. The tree grew a century’s growth within only ten years. b. The stock market dropped its largest drop in three years today. c. Stanley watched as the ball bounced a funny little bounce right into the shortstop’s glove. The empirical facts raise certain questions regarding Perlmutter’s (1978) Unaccusative Hypoth- esis (UH), shown in (6). For instance, what are the implications of the possibility of unaccusative verbs allowing cognate objects for the UH? (6) The Unaccusative Hypothesis Unergative and unaccusative verbs are syntactically differentiated; while unergative verbs have nonderived subjects (i.e., surface subjects are generated as subjects at D-structure), sur- face subjects of unaccusative verbs originate as direct objects. In fact, this point is closely related to the property of cognates, that is whether they are argu- mental or adverbial (Pereltsvaig 1999, 2002). Crucially, if the COs occupy the object position, rather than the adjunct position, this would contradict the UH, since the object position of unaccusatives would be underlyingly occupied by the surface subject. The literature on COs is not unanimous, and the previous studies on the nature of COs can be divided into two camps: i. Some claim that COs are thematic and/or underlying arguments of their predicates (cf. Hale & Keyser (1993) for English, Macfarland (1995) for English and French, Massam (1990) for English, Matsumoto (1996) for English and Japanese) ii. Others maintain that COs are adjuncts (cf. Jones (1988) for English, Moltmann (1989) for English and German, Zubizarreta (1987) for English). There are also studies like Pereltsvaig (1999, 2002), Nakajima (2006) which argue for both types. For instance, Nakajima makes a distinction between argumental and adverbial COs and points out that argumental COs are only possible with unergatives, whereas the COs certain unaccusatives take are adverbial, still arguing for the role of COs in the unaccusative-unergative split. 3 Cognate Objects in Sason Arabic The first striking fact about COs in Sason Arabic is that they can occur with a wide range of predi- cates. In Sason, COs can occur not only with unergative verbs, as in (7), but also with transitive verbs that have an overt direct object (italicized in (8)).1 (7) a. zake-ma kotti zak. laugh-a bad laughed.3m He laughed a bad laugh. b. sabi bayu ibki boy crying cry.3m The boy is crying a cry. (8) a. ali ams kitab qaru qara. ali yesterday book reading read.3m ‘Ali read book(s) yesterday.’ b. axpeys akıl ayale. bread eating ate.3f ‘She ate bread.’ 1Cognate objects and predicates they are associated with are boldfaced throughout. COGNATE OBJECTS IN SASON ARABIC 3 Moreover, in Sason Arabic unaccusative verbs can also productively take COs, as illustrated in (9). (9) a. badıncanad pat-ma gıze kotti patto. tomatoes rottening-a such bad rottened.3pl ‘The tomatoes rottened such a bad rottening.’ b. çiçak ubs-ma boş kotti ubes. flower fading-a very bad faded.3m ‘The flower faded a bad fading.’ c. nahar talu-ma koys tala ala sari sun appearing-a beautiful appeared.3m this morning ‘The sun appeared a beautiful appearance this morning.’ Likewise, COs can appear with predicates from all aspectual classes: activities (10a), accom- plishments (10b) and achievements (10c): (10) a. faqəz le sari faqaze. running of morning ran.3f ‘She ran a morning run.’ b. ene addil-ma imbala diqqat adlu-a. room building-a without care made.3pl-it ‘They built the room carelessly.’ c. mot-ma xəfef mat. death-a quick died.3m ‘He died a quick death.’ The fact that COs can occur with almost any type of predicate is problematic for the hypothesis that all COs are arguments of a verb. Thus, it seems like at least some COs in Sason are not selected, and there is no restriction on the occurrence of COs in terms of the argument structures of the pred- icates. With respect to the aspectual properties of predicates, Kuno and Takami (2004) argue that COs are acceptable if they denote a resultant object/product of an activity/process that the predicate de- notes. If the predicate denotes only the result without involving a process, as in the case of break, occur, appear, or only the manner of the process, then COs are not possible. However, in Sason there is no such restriction, as not only the predicates denoting processes, but also the ones denoting only results (11) or manner (12) are compatible with COs: (11) a. şuşa qarf ınqaraf. glass breaking broke.3m ‘The glass broke a breaking.’ b. nahar talu-ma koys tala ala sari. sun appearing-a beautiful appeared.3m this morning ‘The sun appeared a beautiful appearance this morning.’ c. dave say sare, hama boş nes ma-ca. wedding occurring occurred.3f but many person neg.3m ‘The wedding occurred an occuring, but not many people came.’ (12) a. babe fadu-ma hedi ınfada. door opening-a slow opened.3m ‘The door opened a slow opening.’ b. John maju-ma xıfef ca. John coming-a quick came.3m ‘John came a quick coming.’ c. şelç zabu-ma hedi zab. snow melting-a slow melt ‘The snow melted a slow melting.’ 4 FARUK AKKUŞ AND BALKIZ ÖZTÜRK This section has shown that COs in Sason can freely occur with a range of predicates, including unaccusatives, thus pose a problem for the hypothesis that all COs are arguments of a verb. The next section investigates the nature of COs in terms of their argumenthood vs. adjuncthood status and concludes that they are adverbial.2 4 Are Sason Cognate Objects Arguments or Adverbials? In order to address the question of whether COs are arguments or adverbials in SA, we need to define the criteria that distinguish between the two kinds of phrases (see Pereltsvaig (2002).
Recommended publications
  • Interdisciplinary Approaches to Stratifying the Peopling of Madagascar
    INTERDISCIPLINARY APPROACHES TO STRATIFYING THE PEOPLING OF MADAGASCAR Paper submitted for the proceedings of the Indian Ocean Conference, Madison, Wisconsin 23-24th October, 2015 Roger Blench McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research University of Cambridge Correspondence to: 8, Guest Road Cambridge CB1 2AL United Kingdom Voice/ Ans (00-44)-(0)1223-560687 Mobile worldwide (00-44)-(0)7847-495590 E-mail [email protected] http://www.rogerblench.info/RBOP.htm This version: Makurdi, 1 April, 2016 1 Malagasy - Sulawesi lexical connections Roger Blench Submission version TABLE OF CONTENTS TABLE OF CONTENTS................................................................................................................................. i ACRONYMS ...................................................................................................................................................ii 1. Introduction................................................................................................................................................. 1 2. Models for the settlement of Madagascar ................................................................................................. 2 3. Linguistic evidence...................................................................................................................................... 2 3.1 Overview 2 3.2 Connections with Sulawesi languages 3 3.2.1 Nouns..............................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Cognate Object Constructions In
    COGNATE OBJECT CONSTRUCTIONS IN MSA A thesis submitted to the University of Manchester for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Faculty of Humanities 2017 Safiah A. Y. Madkhali SCHOOL OF ARTS, LANGUAGES AND CULTURES CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES………………………………………………………………………….7 LIST OF FIGURES………………………………………………………………………...7 ABSTRACT………………………………………………………………………………...8 DECLARATION……………………………………………………………..……………9 COPYRIGHT STATEMENT……………………………………………………………10 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS……………………………………………………………...11 GLOSSING CONVENTIONS…………………………………………………………..12 TRANSLITERATION SYMBOLS………………………………………………………13 Chapter 1: INTRODUCTION ....................................................................................... 14 1.1. Introduction…………………………………………………………………….14 1.2. Modern Standard Arabic……………………………………………………...17 1.3. The Data………………………………………………………………………...18 1.4. Cognate Object Constructions………………………………………………..20 1.4.1. Delimitation of the Concept .............................................................................. 20 1.4.1.1 Morphological Criterion ..................................................................... 20 1.4.1.2 Semantic Criteria .................................................................................. 21 1.4.1.3 Syntactic Criteria .................................................................................. 25 1.4.1.4 Categories of Cognate Objects ........................................................... 28 1.4.1.5 Delimitation of the Concept in MSA ................................................. 33 1.4.2.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognate Words in Mehri and Hadhrami Arabic
    Cognate Words in Mehri and Hadhrami Arabic Hassan Obeid Alfadly* Khaled Awadh Bin Mukhashin** Received: 18/3/2019 Accepted: 2/5/2019 Abstract The lexicon is one important source of information to establish genealogical relations between languages. This paper is an attempt to describe the lexical similarities between Mehri and Hadhrami Arabic and to show the extent of relatedness between them, a very little explored and described topic. The researchers are native speakers of Hadhrami Arabic and they paid many field visits to the area where Mehri is spoken. They used the Swadesh list to elicit their data from more than 20 Mehri informants and from Johnston's (1987) dictionary "The Mehri Lexicon and English- Mehri Word-list". The researchers employed lexicostatistical techniques to analyse their data and they found out that Mehri and Hadhrmi Arabic have so many cognate words. This finding confirms Watson (2011) claims that Arabic may not have replaced all the ancient languages in the South-Western Arabian Peninsula and that dialects of Arabic in this area including Hadhrami Arabic are tinged, to a greater or lesser degree, with substrate features of the Pre- Islamic Ancient and Modern South Arabian languages. Introduction: three branches including Central Semitic, Historically speaking, the Semitic language Ethiopian and Modern south Arabian languages family from which both of Arabic and Mehri (henceforth MSAL). Though Arabic and Mehri descend belong to a larger family of languages belong to the West Semitic, Arabic descends called Afro-Asiatic or Hamito-Semitic that from the Central Semitic and Mehri from includes Semitic, Egyptian, Cushitic, Omotic, (MSAL) which consists of two branches; the Berber and Chadic (Rubin, 2010).
    [Show full text]
  • Comparing the Cognate Effect in Spoken and Written Second Language Word Production
    1 Short title: Cognate effect in spoken and written word production Comparing the cognate effect in spoken and written second language word production 1 2 1 Merel Muylle , Eva Van Assche & Robert J. Hartsuiker 1Department of Experimental Psychology, Ghent University, Ghent, Belgium 2 Thomas More University of Applied Sciences, Antwerp, Belgium Address for correspondence: Merel Muylle Department of Experimental Psychology Ghent University Henri Dunantlaan 2 B-9000 Gent (Belgium) E-mail: [email protected] 2 Abstract Cognates – words that share form and meaning between languages – are processed faster than control words. However, it is unclear whether this effect is merely lexical (i.e., central) in nature, or whether it cascades to phonological/orthographic (i.e., peripheral) processes. This study compared the cognate effect in spoken and typewritten production, which share central, but not peripheral processes. We inquired whether this effect is present in typewriting, and if so, whether its magnitude is similar to spoken production. Dutch-English bilinguals performed either a spoken or written picture naming task in English; picture names were either Dutch-English cognates or control words. Cognates were named faster than controls and there was no cognate-by-modality interaction. Additionally, there was a similar error pattern in both modalities. These results suggest that common underlying processes are responsible for the cognate effect in spoken and written language production, and thus a central locus of the cognate effect. Keywords: bilingualism, word production, cognate effect, writing 3 Converging evidence suggests that bilinguals activate both their mother tongue (L1) and their second language (L2) simultaneously when processing linguistic information (e.g., Dijkstra & Van Heuven, 2002; Van Hell & Dijkstra, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Roger Schwarzschild Rutgers University 18 Seminary Place New
    Roger Schwarzschild Rutgers University 18 Seminary Place New Brunswick, NJ 08904 [email protected] to appear in: Recherches Linguistiques de Vincennes November 30, 2004 ABSTRACT In some languages, measure phrases can appear with non-compared adjectives: 5 feet tall. I address three questions about this construction: (a) Is the measure phrase an argument of the adjective or an adjunct? (b) What are we to make of the markedness of this construction *142lbs heavy? (c) Why is it that the markedness disappears once the adjective is put in the comparative (2 inches taller alongside 2lbs heavier)? I claim that because degree arguments are ‘functional’, the measure phrase has to be an adjunct and not a syntactic argument of the adjective. Like event modifiers in extended NP’s and in VPs, the measure phrase predicates of a degree argument of the adjective. But given the kind of meaning a measure phrase must have to do its job in comparatives and elsewhere, it is not of the right type to directly predicate of a degree argument. I propose a lexically governed type-shift which applies to some adjectives allowing them to combine with a measure phrase. KEY WORDS adjective, measure phrase, degree, functional category, lexical, adjunct, argument, antonymy. Measure Phrases as Modifiers of Adjectives1 1. Introduction There is a widely accepted account of expressions like five feet tall according to which the adjective tall denotes a relation between individuals and degrees of height and the measure phrase, five feet, serves as an argument of the adjective, saturating the degree-place in the relation2.
    [Show full text]
  • Palatalization/Velar Softening: What It Is and What It Tells Us About the Nature of Language Morris Halle
    Palatalization/Velar Softening: What It Is and What It Tells Us about the Nature of Language Morris Halle This study proposes an account both of the consonantal changes in- volved in palatalization/velar softening and of the fact that this change is encountered before front vowels. The change is a straightforward case of feature assimilation provided that segments/phonemes are viewed as complexes of features organized into the ‘‘bottle brush’’ model illustrated in example (4) and elsewhere in the text, and that the universal set of features includes, in addition to the familiar binary features, six unary features, which specify the designated articulator(s) for every segment (not only for consonants). Keywords: phonetics, palatalization, velar softening, features, articula- tors For Ken Stevens, friend and colleague, on his 80th birthday My purpose in this study is to present an account of the very common alternation between dorsal and coronal consonants often referred to as palatalization or velar softening. This alternation, exemplified by English electri[k] ϳ electri[s]ity, occurs most often before front vowels. In spite of its extremely common occurrence in the languages of the world, to this time there has been no proper account of palatalization that would relate it to the other properties of language, in particular, to the fact that it is found most commonly before front vowels. In the course of working on these remarks, it became clear to me that palatalization raises numerous theoretical questions about which there is at present no agreement among phonologists. Since these include matters of the most fundamental importance for phonology, I start by exten- sively discussing the main issues and the views on them that seem to me most persuasive at this time (section 1).
    [Show full text]
  • CLIPP Christiani Lehmanni Inedita, Publicanda, Publicata Pleonasm and Hypercharacterization
    CLIPP Christiani Lehmanni inedita, publicanda, publicata titulus Pleonasm and hypercharacterization huius textus situs retis mundialis http://www.uni-erfurt.de/ sprachwissenschaft/personal/lehmann/CL_Publ/ Hypercharacterization.pdf dies manuscripti postremum modificati 23.02.2006 occasio orationis habitae 11. Internationale Morphologietagung Wien, 14.-17.02.2004 volumen publicationem continens Booij, Geert E. & van Marle, Jaap (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 2005. Heidelberg: Springer annus publicationis 2005 paginae 119-154 Pleonasm and hypercharacterization Christian Lehmann University of Erfurt Abstract Hypercharacterization is understood as pleonasm at the level of grammar. A scale of strength of pleonasm is set up by the criteria of entailment, usualness and contrast. Hy- percharacterized constructions in the areas of syntax, inflection and derivation are ana- lyzed by these criteria. The theoretical basis of a satisfactory account is sought in a holis- tic, rather than analytic, approach to linguistic structure, where an operator-operand struc- ture is formed by considering the nature of the result, not of the operand. Data are drawn from German, English and a couple of other languages. The most thorough in a number of more or less sketchy case studies is concerned with German abstract nouns derived in -ierung (section 3.3.1). This process is currently so productive that it is also used to hy- percharacterize nouns that are already marked as nominalizations. 1 1. Introduction Hypercharacterization 2 (German Übercharakterisierung) may be introduced per ostensionem: it is visible in expressions such as those of the second column of T1. T1. Stock examples of hypercharacterization language hypercharacterized basic surplus element German der einzigste ‘the most only’ der einzige ‘the only’ superlative suffix –st Old English children , brethren childer , brether plural suffix –en While it is easy, with the help of such examples, to understand the term and get a feeling for the concept ‘hypercharacterization’, a precise definition is not so easy.
    [Show full text]
  • Jicaque As a Hokan Language Author(S): Joseph H
    Jicaque as a Hokan Language Author(s): Joseph H. Greenberg and Morris Swadesh Source: International Journal of American Linguistics, Vol. 19, No. 3 (Jul., 1953), pp. 216- 222 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1263010 Accessed: 11-07-2017 15:04 UTC REFERENCES Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1263010?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://about.jstor.org/terms The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to International Journal of American Linguistics This content downloaded from 12.14.13.130 on Tue, 11 Jul 2017 15:04:26 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms JICAQUE AS A HOKAN LANGUAGE JOSEPH H. GREENBERG AND MORRIS SWADESH COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 1. The problem 2. The phonological equivalences in Hokan 2. Phonological note have been largely established by Edward 3. Cognate list Sapir's work.3 The Jicaque agreements are 4. Use of lexical statistics generally obvious. A special point is that 5.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognate Accusative in the Holy Quran and the Methods of Compensation* المفعول المطلق في القرآن الكريم وأساليب اإلحاطة بالشكل والمضمون
    An - Najah Univ. J. Res. (Humanities). Vol. 30(10), 2016 The Cognate Accusative in the Holy Quran and the Methods of Compensation* المفعول المطلق في القرآن الكريم وأساليب اﻹحاطة بالشكل والمضمون Wala’a Ya’aqbah وﻻء يعاقبة Language Center, Arab American University, Jenin, Palestine E-mail: [email protected] Received: (20/11/2015), Accepted: (12/5/2016) Abstract This study discusses the problem of translating the cognate accusative (CA) from Arabic into English. It also studies whether the translations studied achieve the optimal pragmatic meaning and force. It attempts to discover how cognate accusatives in the Holy Qur'an have been dealt with in translations done by a number of professional translators. In this study, the researcher will compare the different translations done by different translators, as Mohammad Muhsin Khan, Yusuf Ali, Muhammad Marmaduke Pickthall and Zaid Shakir. The aim is to discover how the cognate accusative has been dealt with; hence, the study will be involved in the basis of direct and indirect translation approaches to show how the translators manage to semantically and pragmatically render the source cognate accusative in the target language, the ensuing loss of meaning and ways of compensation. The study is to be empirical, analytical, and comparative, in the sense that it intends to observe the translators' decision-making in rendering the source text in the target language, whether they follow the direct or indirect translations to render the meaning in the target language. It also * This paper is submitted to the conference titled “Translation a Tool for Connection or Separation”. ”...... The Cognate Accusative in the“ ــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــــ 2058 studies whether the translations achieve the optimal meaning and force, both linguistically and pragmatically.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognet: a Large-Scale Cognate Database
    CogNet: a Large-Scale Cognate Database Khuyagbaatar Batsuren† Gábor Bella† Fausto Giunchiglia†§ DISI, University of Trento, Trento, Italy† Jilin University, Changchun, China§ {k.batsuren; gabor.bella; fausto.giunchiglia}@unitn.it Abstract IELex2, or ABVD (Greenhill et al., 2008), cover only the small set of 225 Swadesh basic concepts, This paper introduces CogNet, a new, although with an extremely wide coverage of up to large-scale lexical database that provides 4000 languages. Secondly, in these databases, lex- cognates—words of common origin and ical entries that belong to scripts other than Latin meaning—across languages. The database or Cyrillic mostly appear in phonetic transcription currently contains 3.1 million cognate pairs instead of using their actual orthographies in their across 338 languages using 35 writing sys- tems. The paper also describes the automated original scripts. These limitations prevent such re- method by which cognates were computed sources from being used in real-world computa- from publicly available wordnets, with an tional tasks on written language. accuracy evaluated to 94%. Finally, statistics This paper describes CogNet, a new large-scale, and early insights about the cognate data high-precision, multilingual cognate database, as are presented, hinting at a possible future well as the method used to build it. Our main exploitation of the resource1 by various fields technical contributions are (1) a general method of lingustics. to detect cognates from multilingual lexical re- sources, with precision and recall parametrable ac- 1 Introduction cording to usage needs; (2) a large-scale cognate database containing 3.1 million word pairs across Cognates are words in different languages that 338 languages, generated with the method above; share a common origin and the same meaning, (3) WikTra, a multilingual transliteration dictio- such as the English letter and the French lettre.
    [Show full text]
  • Cross-Linguistic Variation in Object Marking
    Cross-linguistic Variation in Object Marking Published by LOT phone: +31 30 253 6006 Janskerkhof 13 fax: +31 30 253 6406 3512 BL Utrecht e-mail: [email protected] The Netherlands http://www.lotschool.nl Cover illustration: detail of Polderland by Hennie de Swart (2004) ISBN 978-90-78328-39-1 NUR 616 Copyright c 2007: Peter de Swart. All rights reserved. The work in this dissertation has been funded by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO), grant 220-70-003. Cross-linguistic Variation in Object Marking een wetenschappelijke proeve op het gebied van de Letteren Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de Rector Magnificus prof. mr. S.C.J.J. Kortmann, volgens besluit van het College van Decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op woensdag 21 november 2007 om 10.30 uur precies door Petrus Jacobus Franciscus de Swart geboren op 23 augustus 1981 te Nijmegen Promotor: Prof. dr. P.C. Muysken Copromotor: Mw. dr. H. de Hoop Manuscriptcommissie: Mw. prof. dr. J. Aissen (University of California at Santa Cruz) Mw. prof. dr. A.C.M. van Kemenade Mw. prof. dr. B. Primus (Universit¨atzu K¨oln) Acknowledgements Being a fan of the genre of acknowledgements, I am hesitant to add my contribution to it. Although the work of a single person, this thesis could not have been written without the support and friendship of others. I would like to single out a few of them. This project could not have been carried out without the financial support of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) to the PIONIER Project Case Cross-linguistically [grant 220-70-003] which is gratefully acknowledged.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonetics in Phonology
    Phonetics in Phonology John J. Ohala University of California, Berkeley At least since Trubetzkoy (1933, 1939) many have thought of phonology and phonetics as separate, largely autonomous, disciplines with distinct goals and distinct methodologies. Some linguists even seem to doubt whether phonetics is properly part of linguistics at all (Sommerstein 1977:1). The commonly encountered expression ‘the interface between phonology and phonetics’ implies that the two domains are largely separate and interact only at specific, proscribed points (Ohala 1990a). In this paper I will attempt to make the case that phonetics is one of the essential areas of study for phonology. Without phonetics, I would maintain, (and allied empirical disciplines such as psycholinguistics and sociolinguistics) phonology runs the risk of being a sterile, purely descriptive and taxonomic, discipline; with phonetics it can achieve a high level of explanation and prediction as well as finding applications in areas such as language teaching, communication disorders, and speech technology (Ohala 1991). 1. Introduction The central task within phonology (as well as in speech technology, etc.) is to explain the variability and the patterning -- the “behavior” -- of speech sounds. What are regarded as functionally the ‘same’ units, whether word, syllable, or phoneme, show considerable physical variation depending on context and style of speaking, not to mention speaker-specific factors. Documenting and explaining this variation constitutes a major challenge. Variability is evident in several domains: in everyday speech where the same word shows different phonetic shapes in different contexts, e.g., the release of the /t/ in tea has more noise than that in toe when spoken in isolation.
    [Show full text]