Going on an Antedate

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Going on an Antedate Evolutionary Anthropology 16:204–209 (2007) CROTCHETS & QUIDDITIES Going on an Antedate A strange history of imperfect perfect proportions. KENNETH M. WEISS AND JEFFREY A. KURLAND In the mid-1700s, the English lan- tion from which leads to the changes explained the inheritance patterns of guage was rather out of control. In that define evolution. However, since quantitative traits like skin color. the absence of a satisfactory refer- it will be on the test, many students Mendel’s ideas were different. He ence, word meanings were unclear. (even those who are awake) see HW was trained to think that organic na- Even Shakespeare had already only as an algebraic threat to their ture follows the same laws as does become somewhat obscure to most grade-point average. As one student the physical world of fundamental readers. So the intellectual-about- griped, ‘‘Let me get this straight. particles.4–6 Inheritance must follow town, Samuel Johnson, convinced When nothing happens . nothing mathematical rules. He used hybrid- investors that an English dictionary changes? Duh.’’ ization experiments to deduce those based on improved principles would Over decades of textbooks, HW rules from the relative frequency of be commercially successful.1–3 Exist- appears to be taught largely because alternative traits such as green ver- ing dictionaries were based on ‘‘a it always has been taught. It’s part of sus yellow peas. He succeeded genealogy of sentiments,’’ as author our pedagogic heritage, but its mean- because he deliberately chose traits had copied author over time. Instead, ing and rationale have become hid- known to breed true in different Johnson argued for an approach that den in the dusty volumes of past strains of peas, and he showed that reflected ‘‘intellectual history,’’ a term journal articles. It seems unthinkable the rules were general by repeating he appears to have coined (Fig. 1). to omit it. But why? To see that, we his experiments and testing seven Appearing in 1755, ‘‘Johnson’s’’ be- have to go back to the origin of mod- unrelated traits. came the standard dictionary for ern genetics. Mendel avoided traits with blurry about a century. He showed a word’s intermediates, but he did explain a evolving meaning by using historical fact that also puzzled Darwin and examples of its usage. FIRST PRINCIPLES FIRST Galton. A trait could disappear in In science as well as literature, When Gregor Mendel did his fa- one generation, then reappear in the meaning can become blurred over mous experiments with peas, the na- next. If yellow and green peas were time. Perhaps even more problematic ture of inheritance was unknown. crossed, for example, all the off- and harder to trace than word defini- Following an ancient idea, Darwin spring plants bore yellow peas. But if tions are changes in concepts. An thought that each organ transmitted he then crossed those plants, the interesting example is the Hardy- a tiny miniature of itself to the green trait would reappear in the Weinberg principle (HW). After gonads and that the ‘‘gemmules’’ next generation and—a vital clue— teaching the essentials of Darwinian from each parent blended to form the proportions of the two types evolution, Mendelian inheritance, the offspring. It was quickly shown were predictable and repeatable. In molecular biology, and speciation, that blending didn’t work, but Dar- particular, in the offspring cross he students are routinely introduced to win’s cousin Francis Galton ad- found his famous 3:1 ratio that HW. We teach it as a stable genetic vanced a modified theory of Ances- always favored one trait. He inferred baseline, the HW equilibrium, devia- tral Heredity: You receive a set of that a plant inherited nonblending heritable units from your parents. factors from each parent, and was The fertilized egg uses up half of that equally likely to transmit either to its Ken Weiss and Jeff Kurland are in the material to construct you as an orga- offspring. In modern terms, if a yel- Department of Anthropology at Penn nism; the remaining half is saved to low-producing allele (variant state) A State University. be doled out to your children, who is dominant over the green a, the get half their material from you and original AA 3 aa (yellow 3 green) half from your spouse. Thus, your cross produces only yellow Aa off- direct contribution is cut by half in spring. Listing the maternally VC 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc. each succeeding generation, gradu- derived allele first, when you cross DOI 10.1002/evan20151 Published online in Wiley InterScience ally petering out until there is hardly these with each other, you get equal (www.interscience.wiley.com). any left. This ‘‘biometric’’ theory numbers of AA, aA, Aa, and aa CROTCHETS & QUIDDITIES Antedating Ourselves 205 Figure 1. Some defining wisdom from Samuel Johnson’s Preface, 1755.1 offspring. Since only the aas are In 1902, the English statistician stops, the then-current ratio of domi- green, that is Mendel’s famous 3:1 George Udny Yule asked what would nants to recessives, whatever it is, ratio. happen to Mendelian proportions if will remain indefinitely. So depend- you hybridized two strains (AA 3 ing on when selective breeding stops, aa) and let their descendants ran- there are any number of possible sta- TODAY’S STUDENTS NOT THE domly breed thereafter instead of ble proportions. Independently, in setting up controlled breeding for England, Karl Pearson proved that ONLY ONES CONFUSED each generation. Yule correctly the 3:1 ratio is stable in a random- Mendel’s work was recognized in inferred that under Mendel’s rules mating population. But, like Yule, he 1900, and ‘‘gene’’ was coined by the the classic 3:1 ratio would persist in started with equal allele frequencies Danish botanist Wilhelm Johannssen every future generation. Since the and never generalized these results in 1909 to refer to his inherited par- founders in Yule’s thought-experi- to all possible frequencies. The great ticles, whatever they are. As Samuel ment were AA and aa, hence the al- statistician should have done that, Johnson might find amusing, so lele frequencies were 0.5, this evolu- but he didn’t. many new functional aspects of the tionary result was a not terribly sur- By 1908, confusion was rampant. genome have been found that the prising extension of Mendel’s own A well-known Mendelian, Reginald definition has morphed to its latest experiments. But Yule then asked Punnett, presented a paper on Men- hardly-helpful incarnation of ‘‘a what would happen if only the domi- delism in humans, citing eye color union of genomic sequences encod- nant 75% of the population, rather and brachydactyly (short fingers) as ing a coherent set of potentially over- than everybody, were allowed to examples, which puzzled Yule. In a lapping functional products.’’7 breed? This would be the kind of ar- random-mating population like Eng- When we teach old, established, tificial selection that agricultural land, why didn’t Punnett find a 3:1 everybody-knows-that concepts, it’s breeders had done for centuries, and ratio of brown to blue eyes? Now it easy to forget that a century ago Darwin used as a model for evolu- was Punnett’s turn to be puzzled, these were new ideas that solved real tion by natural selection. Yule due to his inaccurate reading of scientific conundrums, and in ways wrongly calculated that in a few gen- some comments published by Yule. that were not always evident at the erations the dominants would Why did Yule think that Punnett time. approach a limiting proportion of thought that the population should Mendel’s 3:1 ratio refers to a single 85.355339%. This seemed to show increasingly be brown-eyed and bra- generation of peas from a breeding that Galton’s theory was right: The chydactylous? After all, whether experiment. But if this is a general wimpy recessive trait may brown eyes or short fingers were law of inheritance, what would hap- stay around, but diluted. But Mendel increasing or not, their inheritance pen over many generations—on the was right, too, because the recessive pattern was clearly Mendelian! evolutionary scale? This proved to be keeps reappearing. Yule concluded By this time, the relationship a surprisingly confusing question in that Mendel’s principles were a spe- among variation, heredity, breeding, which evolution, semantics, and the cial case of ancestral heredity for a selection, and evolution had been myopic eye of preconception entered dominant trait. mixed into a heady brew of concep- the drama. The effect of a genotype However, W. E. Castle, the leading tual confusion of heroic proportions. on producing a trait was confused American defender of Mendel’s Four people, Yule, Castle, Pearson, with its effect on the competitive fit- theory, was delighted to find a mis- and Punnett (Fig. 2) had independ- ness of the trait. Perhaps, in part, take in Yule’s calculation; a realiza- ently seen what the problem was, but Mendel’s word ‘‘dominirende,’’ or tion showed the failure of Galton’s each had solved only part of it. But ‘‘dominating,’’ suggested a trait’s abil- theory. If selection consistently elimi- what has all this to do with Hardy ity to bully its way to selective vic- nated the recessive every generation, and Weinberg, whoever they were? tory.4,8 Only later did the translation but all possible matings among the become ‘‘dominant’’ and thereby lose remaining dominants are calculated, any such evolutionary connotation. the dominant will very, very, very HARDY, WEINBERG, AND THEIR Johnson would be pleased. Two slowly approach 100%. There is no SILENT PARTNERS other concepts were confused, the stable proportion of recessives with descriptive word ‘‘proportion’’ and constant selection against them.
Recommended publications
  • 8Th International Workshop on the History of Human Genetics: the History of Eugenics and the Evolution of Techniques in Human Genetics
    8th International Workshop on the History of Human Genetics: The History of Eugenics and The Evolution of Techniques in Human Genetics Date: June 4-5, 2020 Place: Berlin Messe Cube Messedamm 26, 14055 Berlin, Germany https://www.messe-berlin.de/en/ Thursday June 4, 2020 History of Eugenics 14.00 - 14.30 15 Welcome and Heike Petermann: Otmar von Verschuer and Human Genetics in Germany Introduction 14.30 - 15.30 75 Session 1 1. Eugenics in Germany Bernd Gausemeier, Hannover (FRG): Exploring the formation of human genetics with Wilhelm Weinberg Susanne Doetz, Magdeburg (FRG): Eugenics in the GDR From Submitted abstracts. 15.30 - 16.00 30 Coffee Break 16.00 - 17.30 90 Session 2 2. Eugenics International Andrew Rushton, Flemington (USA): Bibliometrics and Eminence of British Geneticists. From submitted abstracts Evening Programme (separate registration required): 18.15 - 19.45 90 Guided Tour Jens P. Fürste, Berlin (FRG): Walk through University in Berlin-Dahlem 20.00 - 22.00 Dinner At the Harnack-Haus Friday June 5, 2020 Human Genetics in Berlin 09.00 - 09.15 15 Welcome Heike Petermann, Münster (FRG) 09.15 - 10.45 90 Session 3 3. Human Genetics after 1945 in Berlin Karl Sperling, Berlin (FRG): History of Human Genetics in Berlin-West. Olaf Riess, Tübingen (FRG), Regine Witkowski, Berlin (FRG): History of Human Genetics in Berlin East. Stefan Mundlos, Berlin (FRG): Human Genetics in Berlin today 11.00 - 11.30 30 Coffee Break 11.30 - 13.00 90 Session 4 History of Techniques in Human Genetics Didier Goidin, Paris (FRA): Evolution of Array-CGH. Nicole Fleischer, Boston, MA (USA): Evolution of Next-Generation- Phenotyping.
    [Show full text]
  • Perspectives
    Copyright 1999 by the Genetics Society of America Perspectives Anecdotal, Historical and Critical Commentaries on Genetics Edited by James F. Crow and William F. Dove Hardy, Weinberg and Language Impediments James F. Crow Genetics Department, University of Wisconsin, Madison, Wisconsin 53706 HE Hardy-Weinberg law is the cornerstone of dip- also, even more crucial, that Mendelism and Darwin's Tloid population genetics. Yet it seems trivially obvi- idea of continuous evolution were compatibleº (Pro- ous, a routine application of the binomial theorem. And vine 1971, p. 85). Yule's statement was a curious slip indeed it was so regarded by Hardy when he wrote his for a man who had introduced so much clarity into famous paper, a masterpiece of clarity: the rancorous debates between the mendelists and the To the Editor of Science: I am reluctant to intrude in biometricians. I suppose that even the greatest are enti- a discussion concerning matters of which I have no expert tled to one mental lapse. knowledge, and I should have expected the very simple When I began teaching genetics, this principle was point which I wish to make to have been familiar to called Hardy's law. Later, Stern (1943) called attention biologists. However, some remarks of Mr. Udny Yule, to to an article of Weinberg (1908), who showed the same which Mr. R. C. Punnett has called my attention, suggest that it may still be worth making.. principle at the same time (for an English translation of Suppose that Aa is a pair of Mendelian characters, A Weinberg's paper, see Boyer 1963, pp.
    [Show full text]
  • The Survival of Eugenics in 20Th-Century Germany HUMAN -MGENETICS Paul Weindling KEDUCATION
    Am. J. Hum. Genet. 52:643-649, 1993 The Survival of Eugenics in 20th-Century Germany HUMAN -MGENETICS Paul Weindling KEDUCATION Wellcome Unit for the History of Medicine, University of Oxford, Oxfor After the Second World War, a U.S. intelligence ser- genics. The process of the emancipation of human ge- vices officer investigating the Kaiser Wilhelm Institute netics from eugenics that Kevles (1985) dates as occur- for Anthropology and Human Heredity commented ring from the 1930s in Britain and the United States is that one German anthropologist was 1,000 times more much harder to date for Germany. Weingart sees its guilty than an ordinary SS man (Weindling 1989). Yet, origins as occurring in the early 1940s, but the comple- despite the Nuremberg medical trials and denazifica- tion of the breakthrough of a new discipline of human tion tribunals, there was remarkable continuity of eu- genetics in Germany as occurring only in the 1960s. genically minded geneticists, who, after 1945, were ap- The aim of this paper is to scrutinize this claim. I will pointed to newly founded chairs and to institutes of suggest that the history of human genetics in Germany human genetics (Koch 1985). At the same time certain did not begin in the 1960s, in 1945, or even in the late leading geneticists (notably, Hans Nachtsheim) were 1930s but that its origins were earlier-i.e., in the era of concerned that the reaction against Nazi racism should rediscovery of the Mendelian patterns of hereditary. not lead to the wholesale condemnation of eugenics, so Both before and after 1945 human geneticists were in- that eugenic prescriptions could continue to be derived volved in eugenics movements, and human genetics was from human genetics.
    [Show full text]
  • Wilhelm Weinberg's Early Contribution to Segregation Analysis
    PERSPECTIVES Wilhelm Weinberg’s Early Contribution to Segregation Analysis Alan Stark* and Eugene Seneta*,1 *School of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales 2006, Australia ABSTRACT Wilhelm Weinberg (1862–1937) is a largely forgotten pioneer of human and medical genetics. His name is linked with that of the English mathematician G. H. Hardy in the Hardy–Weinberg law, pervasive in textbooks on population genetics since it expresses stability over generations of zygote frequencies AA, Aa, aa under random mating. One of Weinberg’s signal contributions, in an article whose centenary we celebrate, was to verify that Mendel’s segregation law still held in the setting of human heredity, contrary to the then-prevailing view of William Bateson (1861–1926), the leading Mendelian geneticist of the time. Specifically, Weinberg verified that the proportion of recessive offspring genotypes aa in human parental crossings Aa · Aa (that is, the segregation ratio for such ¼ 1 a setting) was indeed p 4. We focus in a nontechnical way on his procedure, called the simple sib method, and on the heated controversy with Felix Bernstein (1878–1956) in the 1920s and 1930s over work stimulated by Weinberg’s article. ORE than a decade after the rediscovery of Mendelism, In the first decade of the 20th century when the im- Mthere was controversy over whether human inheritance plications of Mendel’s experiments were being realized, actually followed Mendel’s laws. Wilhelm Weinberg (1862– Weinberg had an established medical practice in Stuttgart. 1937) was a prominent member of the German “school” of At about that time he started to produce an impressive list of genetics in the first third of the 20th century.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hardy–Weinberg Law (1908)
    Chapter 11 The Hardy–Weinberg law (1908) Godfrey Harold Hardy was born in 1877 in Surrey, England. His parents were teach- ers. He studied mathematics at Trinity College, Cambridge University, from 1896, became a fellow of his college in 1900 and a lecturer in mathematics in 1906. After a first book on The Integration of Functions of a Single Variable (1905), he pub- lished in 1908 A Course of Pure Mathematics, which was reedited many times and translated to many foreign languages. Fig. 11.1 Hardy (1877–1947) At that time, the rediscovery of Mendel’s work had raised some doubts. Some biologists wondered why the dominant characters did not become more frequent from generation to generation. Reginald Punnett, who had written a book entitled Mendelism in 1905, asked the question to Hardy, with whom he played cricket in Cambridge. Hardy wrote his solution in an article on “Mendelian proportions in a mixed population”, which was published in 1908. To simplify the analysis, he imag- ined the situation of a large population where the choice of the sexual partner would be random. Moreover he restricted his attention to just two factors (or “alleles”) A and a, A being dominant and a recessive. For generation n,letpn be the frequency N. Bacaer,¨ A Short History of Mathematical Population Dynamics, 59 DOI 10.1007/978-0-85729-115-8 11, © Springer-Verlag London Limited 2011 60 11 The Hardy–Weinberg law (1908) of the “genotype” AA,2qn that of Aa and rn that of aa. Of course, pn +2qn +rn = 1. Hardy assumed also that none of these genotypes led to an excess of mortality or to a decrease in fertility when compared to the two other genotypes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Development, Rise, Fall, and Return of the Concept of Anticipation in Hereditary Disease
    Coming Full Circle: The Development, Rise, Fall, and Return of the Concept of Anticipation in Hereditary Disease by Judith Ellen Friedman B.Sc., University of Alberta, 1994 B.A. University of Alberta, 1995 M.A. University of Alberta, 1997 A Dissertation Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in the Department of History © Judith Ellen Friedman, 2008 University of Victoria All rights reserved. This dissertation may not be reproduced in whole or in part, by photocopying or other means, without the permission of the author. ISBN: 978-0-494-52946-1 ii Coming Full Circle: The Development, Rise, Fall, and Return of the Concept of Anticipation in Hereditary Disease by Judith Ellen Friedman B.Sc., University of Alberta, 1994 B.A. University of Alberta, 1995 M.A. University of Alberta, 1997 Supervisory Committee Dr. Gregory Blue, Supervisor (Department of History) Dr. Angus McLaren, Departmental Member (Department of History) Dr. David Zimmerman, Departmental Member (Department of History) Dr. Robert Reid, Outside Member (Department of Biology) Dr. Michael Ashwood-Smith, Outside Member (Department of Biology) Dr. Robert Olby, External Examiner (Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh) iii Supervisory Committee Dr. Gregory Blue, Supervisor (Department of History) Dr. Angus McLaren, Departmental Member (Department of History) Dr. David Zimmerman, Departmental Member (Department of History) Dr. Robert Reid, Outside Member (Department of Biology) Dr. Michael Ashwood-Smith, Outside Member (Department of Biology) Dr. Robert Olby, External Examiner (Department of History and Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh) ABSTRACT This dissertation examines the history of the creation and development of the concept of anticipation, a pattern of heredity found in several diseases (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cultural History of Heredity IV: Heredity in the Century of the Gene
    MAX-PLANCK-INSTITUT FÜR WISSENSCHAFTSGESCHICHTE Max Planck Institute for the History of Science 2008 PREPRINT 343 Conference A Cultural History of Heredity IV: Heredity in the Century of the Gene Table of Contents Introduction Staffan Müller-Wille, Hans-Jörg Rheinberger and John Dupré 3 Leaving Inheritance behind: Wilhelm Johannsen and the Politics of Mendelism Staffan Müller-Wille 7 Mendelian Factors and Human Disease: A Conversation Jean Paul Gaudillière & Ilana Löwy 19 Heredity without Mendelism: Theory and Practice of Dairy Cattle Breeding in the Netherlands 1900-1950 Bert Theunissen 27 Innovation and Ownership in Living Products: Animals and Fruits in the United States, the 1870s to 1930 Daniel J. Kevles 51 Coalition and Opposition: Heredity, Culture, and the Boundaries of Anthropology in the Work of Alfred L. Kroeber Maria E. Kronfeldner 61 Comments on Daniel Kevles and Maria Kronfeldner’s Papers Edna Suárez 77 Producing Identity, Industrializing Purity: Elements for a Cultural History of Genetics Christophe Bonneuil 81 Mendelism and Agriculture in the First Decades of the XXth Century in Mexico Ana Barahona 111 Herbert Spencer Jennings, Heredity, and Protozoa as Model Organisms, 1908-1918 Judy Johns Schloegel 129 Clones, Pure Lines, and Heredity: The Work of Victor Jollos Christina Brandt 139 Pedigree vs. Mendelism. Concepts of Heredity in Psychiatry before and after 1900 Bernd Gausemeier 149 Pedigree Charts as Tools to Visualize Inherited Disease in Progressive Era America Philip Wilson 163 Biohistorical Narratives of Jewish history. Contextualizing the Studies of Wil- helm Nussbaum (1896-1985) Veronika Lipphardt 191 William Bateson’s Pre- and Post-Mendelian Research Program in ‘Heredity and Development’ Marsha L.
    [Show full text]
  • A Century of Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium
    A Century of Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium Oliver Mayo CSIRO Livestock Industries, Adelaide, Australia ardy–Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) is the state of essentially stable in genetical terms: a system, like Hthe genotypic frequency of two alleles of one the genome of a population, is at equilibrium in autosomal gene locus after one discrete generation time when no net change occurs or is expected to of random mating in an indefinitely large population: occur from its state at that time. Furthermore, equi- if the alleles are A and a with frequencies p and libria can be stable, meaning that when a small q (=1-p), then the equilibrium gene frequencies are displacement occurs, the system is expected to simply p and q and the equilibrium genotypic fre- return to the equilibrium. 2 2 quencies for AA, Aa and aa are p , 2pq and q . It was Soon after the rediscovery of Mendel’s remarkable independently identified in 1908 by G. H. Hardy and work in 1900, interest arose in the properties of W. Weinberg after earlier attempts by W. E. Castle Mendelian genes in populations; this was the dawn of and K. Pearson. Weinberg, well known for pioneer- population genetics. Castle (1903) and Pearson ing studies of twins, made many important (1903a, 1903b) were among the first to investigate contributions to genetics, especially human genet- these. As Edwards (in press) has pointed out, Castle ics. Existence of this equilibrium provides a did not derive a generalization equivalent to reference point against which the effects of selec- Pearson’s, and will be considered no further in this tion, linkage, mutation, inbreeding and chance can paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Genetics in the Madhouse
    © Copyright, Princeton University Press. No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by digital or mechanical means without prior written permission of the publisher. Data-Heredity-MadnessIntroduction INTRODUCTION Data-Heredity-Madness A Medical-Social Dream The plan of the institution, the budget, the rules for its administration were not calculated merely to pursue cures for the mentally ill; science itself was also to be advanced. —Report of a Rhine Asylum Committee (1830) Heredity has an undeniably great importance for mental illness and psychical deficiencies. So it is no accident that attention was focused earlier and more intensely on the inheritance question in psychiatry than in any other area of medicine. —Wilhelm Schallmayer (1918) Genetics has been supported by compelling images. We think first of DNA, whose helical structure, announced in 1953, is still often exalted as the secret of life. For half a century before that, the sci- ence of heredity was identified with neat diagrams of green and yellow or smooth and wrinkled peas bred by Gregor Mendel in the garden of an Augustinian monastery.1 The mutant eyes or wings of the fruit fly also assumed an iconic form. Images like these distract us from a science of mass reproduction. The agricultural breeding factories that already had sprung up before anyone cared about Mendel, and the industrialized laboratories of recombinant DNA, have never been appealing in the way of a ladder swirling heaven- ward. Graceful curves can only be part of the story. Let the reader cast an eye over the great filing cabinets of data from armies, pris- ons, immigration offices, census bureaus, and insurance offices that have been brought to bear on the topic of human heredity.
    [Show full text]
  • CURT STERN August 30, 1902-October 23, 1981
    NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES C U R T S TERN 1902—1981 A Biographical Memoir by J AMES V. NEEL Any opinions expressed in this memoir are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Academy of Sciences. Biographical Memoir COPYRIGHT 1987 NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES WASHINGTON D.C. CURT STERN August 30, 1902-October 23, 1981 BY JAMES V. NEEL HERE HAVE BEEN three remarkable periods in the his- Ttory of modern genetics. The first of these, in the early years of this century, encompasses the rediscovery and confirmation of Mendel's findings and the enunciation of the chromosomal theory of inheritance. The second period is characterized by a concatenation of discoveries regarding the more precise mechanisms of chromosomal behavior, largely based on the use of Drosophila and emanating from the "fly room" at Columbia University. The third is the period in which we now find ourselves, initiated in the 1940s by the seminal observations of Avery and collaborators and the later phage work demonstrating that the essential genetic material was DNA and the demonstration, using Neurospora, that genes have essential and specific roles in the synthesis of pro- teins. This was followed by the elucidation of the structure of DNA, leading to a cascade of discoveries concerning DNA fine structure and how it can be manipulated. Each of these flowerings dominated the conceptual biological thinking of the time. NOTE: This memoir is an expanded version of a manuscript published in the An- nual Review of Genetics. I am deeply indebted to the many people who have contrib- uted their remembrances and perspective to the writing of this memoir.
    [Show full text]
  • Plant Introduction and Breeding
    United States Department of Agriculture Agricultural Research Service 150 Years of Research June 2013 at the United States Department of Agriculture: Plant Introduction and Breeding i Cover photo: The stately building that once housed the U.S. Department of Agriculture in Washington, D.C., ca. 1890. (This photo is preserved in the USDA History Collection, Special Collections, National Agricultural Library.) ii Abstract Griesbach, R.J. 2013. 150 Years of Research at the While supplies last, single copies of this publication United States Department of Agriculture: Plant can be obtained at no cost from Robert J. Griesbach, Introduction and Breeding. U.S. Department USDA-ARS, Office of Technology Transfer, 5601 of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, Sunnyside Avenue, Room 4-1159, Beltsville, MD Washington, DC. 20705; or by email at [email protected]. The U.S. Department of Agriculture celebrated Copies of this publication may be purchased in various its 150th anniversary in 2012. One of the primary formats (microfiche, photocopy, CD, print on demand) functions of the USDA when it was established in 1862 from the National Technical Information Service, 5285 was “to procure, propagate, and distribute among the people new Port Royal Road, Springfield, VA 22161, (800) 553- and valuable seeds and plants.” The U.S. Government first 6847, www.ntis.gov. became involved in new plant introductions in 1825 when President John Quincy Adams directed U.S. The U.S. Department of Agriculture (USDA) prohibits Consuls to forward rare plants and seeds to the State discrimination in all its programs and activities on the Department for propagation and distribution.
    [Show full text]
  • A Reality Check on Hardy–Weinberg
    Twin Research and Human Genetics Volume 16 Number 4 pp. 782–789 C The Authors 2013 doi:10.1017/thg.2013.40 A Reality Check on Hardy–Weinberg Alan E. Stark and Eugene Seneta School of Mathematics and Statistics FO7, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia G. H. Hardy (1877–1947) and Wilhelm Weinberg (1862–1937) had very different lives, but in the minds of geneticists, the two are inextricably linked through the ownership of an apparently simple law called the Hardy–Weinberg law. We demonstrate that the simplicity is more apparent than real. Hardy derived the well-known trio of frequencies {q2, 2pq, p2} with a concise demonstration, whereas for Weinberg it was the prelude to an ingenious examination of the inheritance of twinning in man. Hardy’s recourse to an identity relating to the distribution of types among offspring following random mating, rather than an identity relating to the mating matrix, may be the reason why he did not realize that Hardy–Weinberg equi- librium can be reached and sustained with non-random mating. The phrase ‘random mating’ always comes up in reference to the law. The elusive nature of this phrase is part of the reason for the misunderstandings that occur but also because, as we explain, mathematicians are able to use it in a different way from the man-in-the-street. We question the unthinking appeal to random mating as a model and explanation of the distribution of genotypes even when they are close to Hardy–Weinberg proportions. Such sustained proportions are possible under non-random mating.
    [Show full text]