The Eusociality Continuum

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Eusociality Continuum Forum The eusociality continuum ing vertebrates can be regarded as eusocial, just as eu- social invertebrates are cooperative breeders. We believe Pud W. Sherman this integrated approach will foster potentially revealing Section of Ncurobiology and Behavior, cross-taxon comparisons, which are essential to under- Cornell University, standing social evolution in birds, mammals, and in- Ithaca, NY 14853. USA sects. Key words: avion eusociality, cooperative breed- ing, eusociality, mammalian eusociality, reproductive Eileen A. Lacey skews, social system convergence. [Behav Ecol 6:102- Animal Behavior Croup, 108 (1995)] University of California, Davis, CA 95616. USA The evolution of eusociality has been an important Hudson K. Reeve puzzle ever since Darwin (1859: 268) identified Section of Neurobiology and Behavior, worker ants as presenting "one special difficulty, Cornell University, which at first appeared to me insuperable, and ac- Ithaca, NY 14853, USA tually fatal to the whole theory." In 1966, Batra coined the term eusocial (meaning truly social) to Laurent Keller Zoological Institute, describe halictine bees in which "the nest founding Bern University, parent survives to cooperate with a group of her Ethologische Station Hasli, mature daughters, with division of labor" (p. 375). CH-3052 Hinterkappelen, Subsequently, Michener (1969: 305) referred to Switzerland bees as eusocial if they lived in "matrifilial family and groups consisting of.. mothers and daughters. .. Institute of Zoology and Animal Ecology, [showing] division of labor with more or less rec- University of Lausanne, ognizable castes (egg layers and workers)." Bitiment de Biologic, In 1971, Wilson broadened these criteria to in- CH-I0I5 Lausanne, Switzerland clude other insects. Following his lead, Holldobler and Wilson (1990: 638) denned eusocial species as those exhibiting "cooperation in caring for the Eusocial societies are traditionally characterized by a young; reproductive division of labor, with more reproductive division of labor, an overlap of generations, or less sterile individuals working on behalf of in- and cooperative care of the breeders' young. Eusociality dividuals engaged in reproduction; and overlap of | downloaded: 2.10.2021 was once thought to occur only in termites, ants, and at least two generations of life stages capable of some bee and wasp species, but striking evolutionary contributing to colony labor." Once thought to convergences have recently become apparent between the occur only in the orders Hymenoptera (ants, bees, societies of these insects and those of cooperatively breed- and wasps) and Isoptera (termites), eusociality has ing birds and mammals. These parallels have blurred now been reported in Japanese aphids (Homop- distinctions between cooperative breeding and eusocial- tera: Aoki, 1982; I to, 1989), Australian weevils (Co- ity, leading to calls for either drastically restricting or leoptera: Kent and Simpson, 1992), Australian expanding usage of these terms. We favor the latter thrips (Thysanoptera: Crespi, 1992), and African approach. Cooperative breeding and eusociality are not mole-rats (Rodentia: Burda and Kawalika, 1993; discrete phenomena, but rather form a continuum of Jarvis and Bennett, 1993;JarvisetaI., 1991,1994). fundamentally similar social systems whose main dif- As detailed information has accumulated on the ferences lie in the distribution of lifetime reproductive reproductive and social behavior of vertebrates and success among group members. Therefore we propose to invertebrates, distinctions between eusociality and array vertebrate and invertebrate cooperative breeders other social systems have become blurred. Indeed, along a common axis, representing a standardized mea- a number of authors have identified striking evo- sure of reproductive variance, and to drop such (loaded) lutionary parallels between the social systems of terms as "primitive" and "advanced" eusociality. The cooperatively breeding birds and mammals and terminology we propose unites all occurrences of olio- those of social insects (e.g., Alexander etal., 1991; parental helping of kin under a single theoretical um- Andersson, 1984; Emlen et al., 1991; Lacey and brella (e.g., Hamilton's rule). Thus, cooperatively breed- Sherman, 1991; Reeve and Sherman, 1991; Veh- https://doi.org/10.7892/boris.116135 102 Behavioral Ecology Vol. 6 No. 1 source: rencamp, 1979). Further, as Seger (1991: 346) not- the distinguishing attributes (e.g., morphological ed, detailed studies of insects have revealed a broad differentiation of colony members) is continuous, spectrum of social organizations among species tra- rather than discrete, both widiin and among taxa. ditionally characterized as eusocial (e.g., see Keller, Finally, the terms "primitive" and "advanced" are 1993; Michener, 1985; Ross and Matthews, 1991). both value-laden and ambiguous, as they may refer Not surprisingly, therefore, several authors (e.g., either to social complexity (sensu Michener, 1969) Crespi and Yanega, 1994; Tsuji, 1992) have re- or similarity to presumed ancestral forms (sensu cently questioned the adequacy of traditional def- Carpenter, 1991). initions of eusociality. Problems have arisen pri- To resolve these ambiguities, we propose using marily because the key denning characteristic— variation in lifetime reproductive success (LRS) "reproductive division of labor, with more or less among members of cooperatively breeding social sterile individuals working"—is vague and thus am- groups to quantify "reproductive division of la- biguous in its application. One solution is to define bor." Reproductive differences are central to all eusociality more narrowly. This approach has been definitions of eusociality, and they underlie much adopted by Tsuji (1992) and Crespi and Yanega of die diversity among vertebrate and invertebrate (1994), who argue that the term should be applied societies (see Bourke, 1991; Vehrencamp, 1979). to only a subset of the insects currendy recognized Such differences result from social competition and as eusocial. Alternatively, definitional problems suppression within groups as well as ecological fac- could be reduced by expanding the eusociality con- tors that preclude reproduction by some group cept to include all vertebrate and invertebrate so- members. Differences in LRS provide an evolu- cieties with helpers. tionarih/ relevant basis for interspecific compari- We favor the latter approach. It seems more pro- sons because it is through such differences that ductive to recognize that similar social systems oc- natural selection shapes the morphology, physiol- cur in birds, mammals, and insects than to debate ogy, and behavior of eusocial species. whedier particular insects are eusocial (e.g., Furey, One could standardize LRS variation in numer- 1992 versus Tsuji, 1992). Behavioral convergences ous ways. One possibility is the index of reproduc- between eusocial insects and cooperatively breed- tive skew (5) developed by Reeve and Ratnieks ing vertebrates should long ago have focused our (1993) and Keller and Vargo (1993): attention on common selective factors favoring so- ciality and alloparental care in these taxa (see Strassmann and Queller, 1989). Research on these N> + N. groups has proceeded largely independently, how- where Nm is the number of nonbreeding alloparents ever, and as a result there is currendy one set of (helpers) in a group, Nt is the number of breeders evolutionary explanations for cooperative breeding in the group (some of which may also behave as in birds and mammals (e.g.. Brown, 1987; Emlen, alloparents), and v is a measure of the variation in 1991; Jennions and Macdonald, 1994) and a par- reproductive success among breeders. In groups allel, but distinct, set of explanations for sociality containing a single breeder, v is defined as 1.0; in in insects (e.g., Seger, 1991; Trivers, 1985). We groups widi multiple breeders, v is the variance suggest that the evolution of sociality in both groups among breeders in their proportion of die summed will be best understood if these explanations are LRS of the group divided by the maximum possible merged. value for diis variance. Thus, v — N^s*, where J1 is As a first step toward this unification it would be the sample variance in die proportion of total off- useful to have a quantitative way to compare social spring produced by breeders: systems across diverse taxa. Current schemes for comparing insect societies are qualitative, however, and emphasize traits that result only secondarily from reproductive differences among colony mem- bers. For example, some authors (e.g., Cowan, 1991; (N»- 1) Eickwort, 1981; Michener, 1974) distinguish "ad- (in diis expression, p, is die proportion of offspring vanced" from "primitive" eusociality. Advanced produced by die idi breeder). eusocial species inhabit large, long-lived colonies Using S, one can begin to compare die degree containing worker* that typically are unable to mate of reproductive skew widiin and among social spe- and that are well-differentiated morphologically cies on a common scale diat ranges from 0 to 1. from queens, whereas primitively eusocial species When LRS is equal among group members, S — 0; live in small, often annual colonies containing work- when reproduction is restricted to a single individ- ers that are morphologically similar to queens and, ual and odier group members never breed, S =• 1. usually, capable of mating. If, as seems likely, skews vary considerably among The advanced-primitive dichotomy was
Recommended publications
  • How to Design Experiments in Animal Behaviour∗ 8
    SERIES ARTICLE How to Design Experiments in Animal Behaviour∗ 8. How Do Wasps Decide Who Would Be the Queen? Part 2 Raghavendra Gadagkar Continuing to explore the fascinating world of the Indian pa- per wasp Ropalidia marginata, in this article, we will ask how wasps choose their queens in another context. In the previ- ous article in this series, we saw how a simple experiment re- vealed that wasps fight, i.e., indulge in dominance-subordinate interactions, and the winner becomes the queen and the loser becomes the worker. This was in the context of new nestfoundation. But contextmatters. Whenthe same wasps once again have to decide who will be their next queen Raghavendra Gadagkar is if the first one dies or is experimentally removed, the same DST Year of Science Chair Professor at the Centre for rules do not hold. The wasps in a mature colony continue Ecological Sciences, Indian to show dominance-subordinate interactions and can even be Institute of Science, arranged in a dominance hierarchy, but the dominance ranks Bangalore, Honorary of the wasps do not predict who their next queen will be. Professor at JNCASR, and Non-Resident Permanent How they choose their next queen in this context continues Fellow of the to be an enduring mystery. In this article, I will describe four Wissenschaftskolleg (Institute simple experiments that have helped us come close to nail- for Advanced Study), Berlin. ing the culprit, although I must confess that we have not yet During the past 40 years he has established an active found the smoking gun—the chase is on, and we are hot on school of research in the area the trail—please join in! of animal behaviour, ecology and evolution.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Methods Offer Powerful Insights Into Social Evolution in Bees Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton
    Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton To cite this version: Sarah Kocher, Robert Paxton. Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees. Apidologie, Springer Verlag, 2014, 45 (3), pp.289-305. 10.1007/s13592-014-0268-3. hal- 01234748 HAL Id: hal-01234748 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01234748 Submitted on 27 Nov 2015 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Apidologie (2014) 45:289–305 Review article * INRA, DIB and Springer-Verlag France, 2014 DOI: 10.1007/s13592-014-0268-3 Comparative methods offer powerful insights into social evolution in bees 1 2 Sarah D. KOCHER , Robert J. PAXTON 1Department of Organismic and Evolutionary Biology, Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA 2Institute for Biology, Martin-Luther-University Halle-Wittenberg, Halle, Germany Received 9 September 2013 – Revised 8 December 2013 – Accepted 2 January 2014 Abstract – Bees are excellent models for studying the evolution of sociality. While most species are solitary, many form social groups. The most complex form of social behavior, eusociality, has arisen independently four times within the bees.
    [Show full text]
  • Eusociality: Origin and Consequences
    Corrections CELL BIOLOGY. For the article ‘‘The NF1 tumor suppressor criti- BIOCHEMISTRY. For the article ‘‘Engineered single-chain dimeric cally regulates TSC2 and mTOR,’’ by Cory M. Johannessen, streptavidins with an unexpected strong preference for biotin- Elizabeth E. Reczek, Marianne F. James, Hilde Brems, Eric 4-fluorescein,’’ by Filiz M. Aslan, Yong Yu, Scott C. Mohr, and Legius, and Karen Cichowski, which appeared in issue 24, June Charles R. Cantor, which appeared in issue 24, June 14, 2005, of 14, 2005, of Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA (102, 8573–8578; first Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA (102, 8507–8512; first published June published June 3, 2005; 10.1073͞pnas.0503224102), the authors 6, 2005; 10.1073͞pnas.0503112102), the footnotes appeared in note the following. On page 8574, the last sentence of the first the wrong order, due to a printer’s error. The ** footnote on paragraph in the left column, ‘‘Clarified lysates were normalized page 8507 should have read: ‘‘Throughout this paper, we use the for protein levels and analyzed by Western blotting with the terms monomer(ic), dimer(ic), and tetramer(ic) to refer to the following antibodies: phospho-p70S6K (T-389), phospho- number of biotin-binding domains present in a molecule, re- tuberin (S-939), phospho-tuberin (T-1462), phospho-Akt (S- gardless of the number of polypeptide chains it has.’’ In addition, 473), tuberin (C-20) from Santa Cruz Biotechnology, and the †† footnote on page 8512 should have read: ‘‘We note, protein kinase B␣͞AKT1, actin, and ␤-tubulin from Sigma- however,
    [Show full text]
  • Finding a Mate with Eusocial Skills
    Finding a Mate With Eusocial Skills Chris Marriott1 and Jobran Chebib2 1University of Washington, Tacoma, WA, USA 98402 2University of Zurich,¨ Zurich,¨ Switzerland 8057 [email protected] Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/isal/proceedings-pdf/alif2016/28/298/1904330/978-0-262-33936-0-ch052.pdf by guest on 30 September 2021 Abstract mutual response of the individuals in the herd with no need for social awareness or exchange of information. Sexual reproductive behavior has a necessary social coordina- Prior work (from now on when we reference the prior tion component as willing and capable partners must both be work we mean the work in Marriott and Chebib (2015a,b)) in the right place at the right time. It has recently been demon- strated that many social organizations that support sexual re- showed that herding, philopatry, and assortative mating production can evolve in the absence of social coordination arose through non-social mechanisms of convergence and between agents (e.g. herding, assortative mating, and natal common descent. This work raised a few questions regard- philopatry). In this paper we explore these results by includ- ing the role that social interaction plays in many of these ob- ing social transfer mechanisms to our agents and contrasting served behaviors. These mating behaviors can be explained their reproductive behavior with a control group without so- cial transfer mechanisms. We conclude that similar behaviors by both non-social and social mechanisms. In many cases emerge in our social learning agents as those that emerged in the non-social solution is the simpler one, though it is likely the non-social learning agents.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture Outline: 1
    Bio342_ Animals_Behavior_F2018 LECTURE GOALS: £ Consider different ways of classifying questions in Animal Behavior. LECTURE OUTLINE: 1. Ethology cleaves to the “4 questions” delineated by Niko Tinbergen including: a. Causation ~ mechanism b. Ontogeny ~ development c. Adaptive Value ~ function d. Phylogeny ~ evolution 2. These 4 questions can be described in dichotomous distinctions: a. proximate vs. ultimate b. contemporary (snapshot) vs. chronicle (story) 3. Tinbergen’s 4 Questions resemble Aristotle’s theoretical framework for a general analytical scheme known as “the 4 causes”. 4. Dewsburry presents an alternate classification scheme that avoid the problems of the proximate/ultimate distinction. a. Genesis: What is the influence of past events as a dynamic interpretation of history on 3 timescales. i. Evolution: relates past natural selection & drift etc. ii. Culture: covers cross generational, non-genetic transmission. iii. Development: begins with conception and includes genetic factors as well as environmental factors. - dynamic. b. Control: Short term regulation of behavior. i. external (outside the skin: including biotic and abiotic environment) ii. internal (physiology & endocrinology) factors. c. Consequences: The full range of effects that are contingent on behavior i. consequences for the organism: behaviors change the organism itself ii. consequences for the environment : The extended phenotype, both abiotic and social iii. consequences for differential reproduction : inclusive fitness 5. Example of behaviors that are difficult to put into Tinberbgen’s 4 Questions. a. Genetic Assimilation – the role of plasticity past and present. b. Epigenetic inheritance – Michael Meaney’s work with maternal care in rats. i. This is a maternal influence at a critical period ii. The mechanism involves methylation of the Cortisol (Glucocorticoid) Receptor during a critical period that alters the function of the HPA.
    [Show full text]
  • Brief Contents
    B r i e f C o n t e n t s Chapter 1 The Science of Animal Behavior 2 Chapter 2 Evolution and the Study of Animal Behavior 20 Chapter 3 Methods for Studying Animal Behavior 38 Chapter 4 Behavioral Genetics 56 Chapter 5 Learning and Cognition 78 Chapter 6 Communication 112 Chapter 7 Foraging Behavior 142 Chapter 8 Antipredator Behavior 170 Chapter 9 Dispersal and Migration 196 Chapter 10 Habitat Selection, Territoriality, and Aggression 226 Chapter 11 Mating Behavior 254 Chapter 12 Mating Systems 286 Chapter 13 Parental Care 312 Chapter 14 Social Behavior 338 A p p e n d i x 1 Scientific Literature 372 Appendix 2 Writing a Scientific Paper 374 Appendix 3 Animal Care Information 377 C o n t e n t s P r e f a c e x x i i i Chapter 1 The Science of Animal Behavior 2 1.1 Animals and their behavior are an integral part of human society 4 Recognizing and defi ning behavior 5 Measuring behavior: elephant ethograms 5 1.2 Th e scientifi c method is a formalized way of knowing about the natural world 6 Th e importance of hypotheses 7 Th e scientifi c method 7 Correlation and causality 10 Hypotheses and theories 11 Social sciences and the natural sciences 11 1.3 Animal behavior scientists test hypotheses to answer research questions about behavior 12 Hypothesis testing in wolf spiders 12 Negative results and directional hypotheses 13 Generating hypoth eses 14 Hypotheses from mathematical models 14 1.4 Anthropomorphic explanations of behavior assign human emotions to animals and can be diffi cult to test 15 1.5 Scientifi c knowledge is generated and
    [Show full text]
  • Genetic Accommodation and the Role of Ancestral Plasticity in the Evolution of Insect Eusociality Beryl M
    © 2018. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2018) 221, jeb153163. doi:10.1242/jeb.153163 COMMENTARY Genetic accommodation and the role of ancestral plasticity in the evolution of insect eusociality Beryl M. Jones1,* and Gene E. Robinson1,2,3,4 ABSTRACT novel genetic combinations and phenotypes (Carroll, 2008). ‘ ’ For over a century, biologists have proposed a role for phenotypic Mutation-first evolution (see Glossary), where a new mutation ‘ ’ plasticity in evolution, providing an avenue for adaptation in addition provides novel phenotypes that can be screened by natural to ‘mutation-first’ models of evolutionary change. According to the selection, is easily studied when the mutation can be directly linked various versions of this idea, the ability of organisms to respond to the phenotype. Even without knowledge of the phenotypic adaptively to their environment through phenotypic plasticity may consequences of alleles, mutation-first evolution studies can be lead to novel phenotypes that can be screened by natural selection. If initiated in both natural populations and laboratories simply by these initially environmentally induced phenotypes increase fitness, documenting changes in allele frequencies over time. then genetic accommodation can lead to allele frequency change, However, novel traits are also suggested to originate independent influencing the expression of those phenotypes. Despite the long of new mutations, via the environmental and developmental history of ‘plasticity-first’ models, the importance of genetic induction of phenotypes. One of the first biologists to emphasize accommodation in shaping evolutionary change has remained this was Baldwin, who at the turn of the 20th century suggested a ‘ ’ controversial – it is neither fully embraced nor completely discarded process of organic selection by which fitness differences arising by most evolutionary biologists.
    [Show full text]
  • Revisiting Stigmergy in Light of Multi-Functional, Biogenic, Termite Structures As Communication Channel ⇑ Sebastian Oberst A,B, , Joseph C.S
    Computational and Structural Biotechnology Journal 18 (2020) 2522–2534 journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/csbj Revisiting stigmergy in light of multi-functional, biogenic, termite structures as communication channel ⇑ Sebastian Oberst a,b, , Joseph C.S. Lai b, Richard Martin a, Benjamin J. Halkon a, Mohammad Saadatfar c, Theodore A. Evans d a Centre for Audio, Acoustics and Vibration, Faculty of Engineering and IT, University of Technology Sydney, 15 Broadway, Ultimo, NSW 2007, Australia b School of Engineering and IT, University of New South Wales Canberra, Northcott Dr, Campbell ACT 2612, Australia c Department of Applied Mathematics, Australian National University, 58-60 Mills Road, Canberra, ACT 2601, Australia d School of Biological Sciences, The University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling Hwy, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia article info abstract Article history: Termite mounds are fascinating because of their intriguing composition of numerous geometric shapes Received 2 March 2020 and materials. However, little is known about these structures, or of their functionalities. Most research Received in revised form 4 August 2020 has been on the basic composition of mounds compared with surrounding soils. There has been some tar- Accepted 5 August 2020 geted research on the thermoregulation and ventilation of the mounds of a few species of fungi-growing Available online 19 August 2020 termites, which has generated considerable interest from human architecture. Otherwise, research on termite mounds has been scattered, with little work on their explicit properties. Keywords: This review is focused on how termites design and build functional structures as nest, nursery and food Termite structures storage; for thermoregulation and climatisation; as defence, shelter and refuge; as a foraging tool or Complexity Superorganism building material; and for colony communication, either as in indirect communication (stigmergy) or Vibrational communication as an information channel essential for direct communication through vibrations (biotremology).
    [Show full text]
  • The Secret Lives of Ants
    The Secret Lives of Ants By Sandeep Ravindran on Wed, 15 Mar 2017 The tiny brown clone walks alone, far away from its brethren. Its solitary path takes it round and round the edge of the small plastic dish as it completely ignores its siblings. It’s very unusual for an ant to be this anti-social. But it’s not the ant’s fault that it’s a loner, rather that of Daniel Kronauer. The ant is unable to detect the others’ social cues, the result of years of work by Kronauer and his team of researchers at Rockefeller University. Their seemingly esoteric work could shed light on eusociality, one of the more unusual social structures in the animal world. Eusocial animals raise their young cooperatively while adhering to a strict division of labor—one class reproduces while others may tend to the young or forage for food. Deciphering eusociality could lead to a broader understanding of how complex social systems evolved. Already, Kronauer’s lab has elucidated some of the first genetic underpinnings of ant sociality, and the tools they used to do that could transform how researchers study social insects. Video: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/next/wp-content/uploads/2017/03/ant-tracking.mp4 Two ants, PP in pink and GG in green, have had a crucial olfactory receptor knocked out, leaving them unable to pick up on the others' social signals. Scientists have long been fascinated by the complex societies and behaviors of eusocial insects such as ants and honeybees. Some ants collaborate to construct towering edifices, others undertake complex foraging expeditions or herd aphids and farm fungi for food.
    [Show full text]
  • Eusociality: Origin and Consequences
    PERSPECTIVE Eusociality: Origin and consequences Edward O. Wilson*† and Bert Ho¨ lldobler‡§ *Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford Street, Cambridge, MA 02138-2902; ‡School of Life Sciences-LSC 274, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-4501; and §Theodor-Boveri-Institut fu¨ r Biowissenschaften (Biozentrum), Universita¨t Wu¨ rzburg, Lehrstuhl fu¨ r Zoologie II, Am Hubland, D97074 Wu¨ rzburg, Germany Contributed by Edward O. Wilson, July 12, 2005 In this new assessment of the empirical evidence, an alternative to the standard model is proposed: group selection is the strong binding force in eusocial evolution; individual selection, the strong dissolutive force; and kin selection (narrowly defined), either a weak binding or weak dissolutive force, according to circumstance. Close kinship may be more a consequence of eusociality than a factor promoting its origin. A point of no return to the solitary state exists, as a rule when workers become anatomically differenti- ated. Eusociality has been rare in evolution, evidently due to the scarcity of environmental pressures adequate to tip the balance among countervailing forces in favor of group selection. Eusociality in ants and termites in the irreversible stage is the key to their ecological dominance and has (at least in ants) shaped some features of internal phylogeny. Their colonies are consistently superior to solitary and preeusocial competitors, due to the altruistic behavior among nestmates and their ability to organize coordinated ac- tion by pheromonal communication. n eusociality, an evolutionarily ad- of groups will spread if the positive in- theory are difficult to relate to the com- vanced level of colonial existence, tergroup component of the altruists’ fit- plexities of tangible social phenomena.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Eusociality
    Vol 466j26 August 2010jdoi:10.1038/nature09205 ANALYSIS The evolution of eusociality Martin A. Nowak1, Corina E. Tarnita1 & Edward O. Wilson2 Eusociality, in which some individuals reduce their own lifetime reproductive potential to raise the offspring of others, underlies the most advanced forms of social organization and the ecologically dominant role of social insects and humans. For the past four decades kin selection theory, based on the concept of inclusive fitness, has been the major theoretical attempt to explain the evolution of eusociality. Here we show the limitations of this approach. We argue that standard natural selection theory in the context of precise models of population structure represents a simpler and superior approach, allows the evaluation of multiple competing hypotheses, and provides an exact framework for interpreting empirical observations. or most of the past half century, much of sociobiological greater than two times the cost to the altruist (R 5 1/2) or eight times theory has focused on the phenomenon called eusociality, in the case of a first cousin (R 5 1/8). where adult members are divided into reproductive and (par- Due to its originality and seeming explanatory power, kin selection F tially) non-reproductive castes and the latter care for the came to be widely accepted as a cornerstone of sociobiological theory. young. How can genetically prescribed selfless behaviour arise by Yet it was not the concept itself in its abstract form that first earned natural selection, which is seemingly its antithesis? This problem favour, but the consequence suggested by Hamilton that came to has vexed biologists since Darwin, who in The Origin of Species be called the ‘‘haplodiploid hypothesis.’’ Haplodiploidy is the sex- declared the paradox—in particular displayed by ants—to be the determining mechanism in which fertilized eggs become females, and most important challenge to his theory.
    [Show full text]
  • The Basic Principles of Kin Sociality and Eusociality: Human Evolution
    Natural Science, 2016, 8, 8-19 Published Online January 2016 in SciRes. http://www.scirp.org/journal/ns http://dx.doi.org/10.4236/ns.2016.81002 The Basic Principles of Kin Sociality and Eusociality: Human Evolution Ding-Yu Chung Utica, MI, USA Received 17 December 2015; accepted 25 January 2016; published 28 January 2016 Copyright © 2016 by author and Scientific Research Publishing Inc. This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution International License (CC BY). http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ Abstract The paper posits that kin sociality and eusociality are derived from the handicap-care principles based on the need-based care to the handicappers from the caregivers for the self-interest of the caregivers. In this paper, handicap is defined as the difficulty to survive and reproduce indepen- dently. Kin sociality is derived from the childhood handicap-care principle where the children are the handicapped children who receive the care from the kin caregivers in the inclusive kin group to survive. The caregiver gives care for its self-interest to reproduce its gene. The individual’s gene of kin sociality contains the handicapped childhood and the caregiving adulthood. Eusociality is derived from the adulthood handicap-care principle where responsible adults are the handi- capped adults who give care and receive care at the same time in the interdependent eusocial group to survive and reproduce its gene. Queen bees reproduce, but must receive care from worker bees that work but must rely on queen bees to reproduce. A caregiver gives care for its self-interest to survive and reproduce its gene.
    [Show full text]