Intergroup Encounters in Grey-Cheeked Mangabeys (Lophocebus Albigena) and Redtail Monkeys (Cercopithecus Ascanius): Form and Function

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Intergroup Encounters in Grey-Cheeked Mangabeys (Lophocebus Albigena) and Redtail Monkeys (Cercopithecus Ascanius): Form and Function Intergroup Encounters in Grey-Cheeked Mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) and Redtail Monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius): Form and Function Michelle Brown Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2011 © 2011 Michelle Brown All rights reserved ABSTRACT Intergroup Encounters in Grey-Cheeked Mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) and Redtail Monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius): Form and Function Michelle Brown Across species and populations, encounters between neighboring social groups take a variety of forms. In particular, intergroup encounters (IGEs) may or may not be aggressive and may include the participation of males and/or females. The proximate causes of aggressive participation by each sex, particularly among primates, is generally thought to be the availability of mates and food. However, existing hypotheses of resource defense have rarely been explicitly tested through identification of the proximate causes of male and female aggression. In this dissertation, I sought to test the existing hypotheses by determining whether female food defense, male food defense, and male mate defense occur in grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) and redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius). With a team of field assistants, I observed six mangabey groups for 15 months and four redtail groups for 12 months at the Ngogo site in Kibale National Park, Uganda. We observed naturally-occurring IGEs in both species, simulated IGEs among mangabey groups using playback experiments, and measured the availability of food resources in botanical plots. I evaluated multiple aspects of intergroup relations, including initiation of encounters, occurrence of intense aggression, encounter outcomes, overall encounter rates, and the effect of neighbors’ long-distance calls on group movements. For each IGE aspect, I determined the effects of group size and resource value. I found strong evidence for female food defense by redtails and male food defense by both species, weak evidence of male mate defense by redtails and no evidence for mangabeys, and no evidence of female food defense by mangabeys. In addition, the specific conditions under which food defense is expected to occur in primates were appropriate for male mangabeys, but not for female or male redtails. This pattern of results indicates that existing hypotheses cannot accurately predict which populations will exhibit food or mate defense by males or females, or the specific social and ecological conditions that elicit defense. I also found that mangabeys exhibited two types of IGE: whole group encounters, where the majority of two groups were in visual contact, and subgroup encounters, where one or a few individuals left their group and interacted aggressively with a neighboring group. Whereas whole group IGEs appeared to function as defense of specific feeding sites, subgroup IGEs did not; instead, they appeared to be a means of defending the core of the home range. Compared to earlier studies, the mangabey groups in this study exhibited higher encounter rates and more pronounced aggression. Higher group densities and more intense feeding competition have given rise to a dramatically different pattern of mangabey intergroup relations. This study demonstrates the importance of considering multiple IGE aspects, hypotheses, and food characteristics when evaluating the role of intergroup relations in the lives of social animals. TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Tables and Figures vi Acknowledgements xiii Dedication xviii Preface xix Chapter 1: Intergroup Encounters in Group-Living Birds and Mammals, with a Focus on Non-Human Primates Introduction 1 I. Defining intergroup encounters 5 II. General principles 7 II. A. Functions 7 II. B. Behaviors 10 II. C. Determinants of success 26 II. D. Spatial patterns 30 II. E. Factors modifying spacing systems 31 III. Differences among primates, other mammals, and birds 35 IV. A model of primate intergroup encounters 37 IV. A. Hypotheses 37 IV. B. Evaluating the model 44 Future directions 48 References 49 Tables and figures 63 i Chapter 2: Approach and avoidance among grey-cheeked mangabey (Lophocebus albigena) groups Abstract 72 Introduction 73 Methods 78 Data collection 78 Data analysis: long-range encounters 85 Data analysis: short-range encounters 91 Results 95 Habitat quality 95 Long-range encounters 95 Short-range encounters 98 Discussion 104 Long-range encounters 105 Short-range encounters 109 General conclusions 117 References 119 Tables and figures 125 ii Chapter 3: Proximate causes of male and female aggression during intergroup encounters in grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) and redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius) Abstract 167 Introduction 168 Methods 172 Results 184 Discussion 191 References 204 Tables and figures 211 Chapter 4: The effects of between-group contest competition on grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) and redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius) Abstract 232 Introduction 233 Methods 237 Results 245 Discussion 249 References 256 Tables and figures 261 iii Chapter 5: Intergroup encounter rates in redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius) and grey- cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus albigena) Abstract 274 Introduction 275 Methods 279 Results 286 Discussion 290 References 297 Tables and figures 301 Chapter 6: An integrated approach to intergroup encounters Introduction 319 Review of results 321 Mangabey patterns 322 Redtail patterns 328 Future directions for research on between-group contests 329 Economics of food defense 329 Evolutionary game theory 333 Generalizability of the results 335 Conclusions 337 References 339 Tables and figures 344 iv Appendix 1: Top 20 diet species for three Kibale mangabey studies: (A) 348 this study, (B) Barrett (1995), and (C) Waser (1977). Appendix 2: Cumulative home range area for each study group, 351 calculated using the “data area curve” function in Biotas. Appendix 3: Spearman rank correlations among food-related variables. 357 Appendix 4: Measuring food abundance: the disconnect between 361 phenology census results and primate feeding behavior. Appendix 5: Frequencies of female and male participation in aggressive 373 intergroup encounters in relation to home range defendability. Appendix 6: Maps of four redtail and seven mangabey ranges, as well as 384 encounter outcomes. v LIST OF TABLES AND FIGURES CHAPTER 1 Figure 1. The components of intergroup encounters. 63 Figure 2. A hypothetical relationship between energetic balance and the 64 frequency of aggressive intergroup encounters. Figure 3. A spectrum of spacing behaviors, ranging from site-independent to 65 -dependent dominance. Figure 4. A spectrum of spacing behaviors in relation to population density. 66 Table 1. Combinations of variable states necessary for each function of IGE 67 participation. Table 2. Predicted and observed sex-specific functions of participation in 68 IGEs for some group-living primates. CHAPTER 2 Table 1. The observation period and hours per group and the composition 125 of each group. Table 2. Second generation models considered in the selection process, 126 each of which includes the best control variables. Table 3. Comparison of the mangabey population density, number of food 127 tree stems, and food basal area in three mangabey studies in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Table 4. Number of whoop gobble bouts recorded in each home range 128 zone. Figure 1. Change in group movement at different intergroup distances after 129 a neighbor’s long-distance call. Figure 2. Change in group movement within core/exclusive and 130 periphery/shared areas after a neighbor’s long-distance call. Table 5. Pearson χ2 tests of the frequency of withdrawal across core 131 (exclusive) and periphery (shared) areas, per study group. vi Figure 3. Group movements in response to neighbors’ whoop-gobbles by six 132 Ngogo study groups in a high density population. Table 6. The number (and percentage) of post-call travel direction 133 responses, sorted by pre-call travel direction. Table 7. The best model of listener deflection angles (when the pre-call 134 travel direction was ‘toward’ the caller). Figure 4. Listener’s deflection angle as a function of the degree of intrusion 135 by the calling group into the listener’s home range. Figure 5. Listener’s deflection angle as a function of the number of adult 136 males in the group . Figure 6. Listener’s deflection angle as a function of the short term feeding 137 value of its location at the time of the call. Table 8. Control variables assessed during the first step of the model 138 selection process. Table 9. Models consisting of different combinations of important control 139 variables. Table 10. Models assessed during the final step of the model selection 140 process, containing the best control model and different food- related independent variables Table 11. The number of experimental playback trials conducted per group, 141 per condition. Table 12. The number of experimental playback trials per condition, pooled 142 across groups. Table 13. The number of IGEs per group, per condition. 143 Table 14. The number of IGEs per condition, pooled across groups. 144 Table 15. Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney tests of the differences in the conditions 145 associated with naturally-occurring intergroup encounters (IGEs) and experimental playback trials (PBs). Table 16. Differences in social and ecological predictors (median and 146 interquartile range) across group movement responses in experimental
Recommended publications
  • Initial Research Question • M
    4/30/2008 Macaques Human-Macaque ► Old World monkey Interactions and Related ► Body size: Medium to Large bodied ► Omnivorous: fruit, leaves, invertebrates Effects on Macaque ► Arboreal/terrestrial ► Social groups of 20-50 of both sexes and all ages Behavior and Conservation ► Alpha males usually fission to start own groups ► Native to Asia and Africa Denae Crowl ► Order = Primates ► Family = Cercopithecidae Department of ► Genus = Macaca Anthropology, SIUE Phyletic Species-Groups Within the Genus Macaca (A. F. Richard, et. al. 1989) 1. M. fascicularis group • M. cyclopic - Formosan rock, or Taiwan macaque • M. fascicularis - Crab-eating, or long-tailed macaque • M. fuscata - Japanese macaque • M. mulatta - Rhesus macaque 2. M. silenus-sylvanus group • M. nemestrina - Pigtail macaque • M. silenus - Lion-tailed macaque • M. sylvanus - Barbary macaque • M. brunnescens - Mana-Butung macaque Initial Research Question • M. hecki - Heck’s macaque • H. maura - Moor macaque • M. nigra - Celebes black macaque • M. nigrescens - Gorontalo macaque • M. ochreata - Booted macaque Human Perceptions vs. Macaque • M. tonkeana - Tonkean macaque 3. M. sinica group Behavior • M. assamensis - Assamese macaque • M. radiata - Bonnet macaque • M. sinica - Toque macaque • M. thibetana - Tibetan macaque 4. M. arctoides group • M. arctoides - Stumptail, or bear macaque Both human perceptions and macaques behaviors have been My Perception an issue due to the increasing amount of human-macaque interaction ► Bali, Indonesia ► Yap, Micronesian Island ►What is meant by ‘natural’ environment? ► . Revered . Pests What forms of conservation are beneficial to . Provisioned . Disease transmitters macaques and humans? . Welcomed tourist . Illegal transport to main ►What are the issues surrounding established attraction island conservation methods? . Illegal as pets . Crop-raiders 1 4/30/2008 Offerings in form of a flowers/fruits Methods = non-deliberate provisioning ►Reviewed published literature ►Variables .
    [Show full text]
  • The Taxonomy of Primates in the Laboratory Context
    P0800261_01 7/14/05 8:00 AM Page 3 C HAPTER 1 The Taxonomy of Primates T HE T in the Laboratory Context AXONOMY OF P Colin Groves RIMATES School of Archaeology and Anthropology, Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia 3 What are species? D Taxonomy: EFINITION OF THE The biological Organizing nature species concept Taxonomy means classifying organisms. It is nowadays commonly used as a synonym for systematics, though Disagreement as to what precisely constitutes a species P strictly speaking systematics is a much broader sphere is to be expected, given that the concept serves so many RIMATE of interest – interrelationships, and biodiversity. At the functions (Vane-Wright, 1992). We may be interested basis of taxonomy lies that much-debated concept, the in classification as such, or in the evolutionary implica- species. tions of species; in the theory of species, or in simply M ODEL Because there is so much misunderstanding about how to recognize them; or in their reproductive, phys- what a species is, it is necessary to give some space to iological, or husbandry status. discussion of the concept. The importance of what we Most non-specialists probably have some vague mean by the word “species” goes way beyond taxonomy idea that species are defined by not interbreeding with as such: it affects such diverse fields as genetics, biogeog- each other; usually, that hybrids between different species raphy, population biology, ecology, ethology, and bio- are sterile, or that they are incapable of hybridizing at diversity; in an era in which threats to the natural all. Such an impression ultimately derives from the def- world and its biodiversity are accelerating, it affects inition by Mayr (1940), whereby species are “groups of conservation strategies (Rojas, 1992).
    [Show full text]
  • The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque
    Fascinating Life Sciences Jin-Hua Li · Lixing Sun Peter M. Kappeler Editors The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque Fascinating Life Sciences This interdisciplinary series brings together the most essential and captivating topics in the life sciences. They range from the plant sciences to zoology, from the microbiome to macrobiome, and from basic biology to biotechnology. The series not only highlights fascinating research; it also discusses major challenges associ- ated with the life sciences and related disciplines and outlines future research directions. Individual volumes provide in-depth information, are richly illustrated with photographs, illustrations, and maps, and feature suggestions for further reading or glossaries where appropriate. Interested researchers in all areas of the life sciences, as well as biology enthu- siasts, will find the series’ interdisciplinary focus and highly readable volumes especially appealing. More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/15408 Jin-Hua Li • Lixing Sun • Peter M. Kappeler Editors The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque Editors Jin-Hua Li Lixing Sun School of Resources Department of Biological Sciences, Primate and Environmental Engineering Behavior and Ecology Program Anhui University Central Washington University Hefei, Anhui, China Ellensburg, WA, USA International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology Anhui, China School of Life Sciences Hefei Normal University Hefei, Anhui, China Peter M. Kappeler Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology Unit, German Primate Center Leibniz Institute for Primate Research Göttingen, Germany Department of Anthropology/Sociobiology University of Göttingen Göttingen, Germany ISSN 2509-6745 ISSN 2509-6753 (electronic) Fascinating Life Sciences ISBN 978-3-030-27919-6 ISBN 978-3-030-27920-2 (eBook) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27920-2 This book is an open access publication.
    [Show full text]
  • Sex-Specific Variation of Social Play in Wild Immature Tibetan Macaques
    animals Article Sex-Specific Variation of Social Play in Wild Immature Tibetan Macaques, Macaca thibetana Tong Wang 1,2, Xi Wang 2,3, Paul A. Garber 4,5, Bing-Hua Sun 2,3, Lixing Sun 6, Dong-Po Xia 1,2,* and Jin-Hua Li 2,3,* 1 School of Life Sciences, Anhui University, Hefei 230601, China; [email protected] 2 International Collaborative Research Center for Huangshan Biodiversity and Tibetan Macaque Behavioral Ecology, Hefei 230601, China; [email protected] (X.W.); [email protected] (B.-H.S.) 3 School of Resource and Environmental Engineering, Anhui University, Hefei 230601, China 4 Department of Anthropology and Program in Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA; [email protected] 5 International Centre of Biodiversity and Primate Conservation, Dali University, Dali 671000, China 6 Department of Biology, Central Washington University, Ellensburg, WA 98926, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] (D.-P.X.); [email protected] (J.-H.L.); Tel.: +86-551-63861723 (D.-P.X. & J.-H.L.) Simple Summary: Social play among immature individuals has been well-documented across a wide range of mammalian species. It represents a substantial part of the daily behavioral repertoire during immature periods, and it is essential for acquiring an appropriate set of motor, cognitive, and social skills. In this study, we found that infant Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana) exhibited similar patterns of social play between males and females, juvenile males engaged more aggressive play than juvenile females, and juvenile females engaged more affiliative play than juvenile males.
    [Show full text]
  • The Japan Monkey Centre Primates Brain Imaging Repository for Comparative Neuroscience: an Archive of Digital Records Including Records for Endangered Species
    Primates (2018) 59:553–570 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-018-0694-3 ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Japan Monkey Centre Primates Brain Imaging Repository for comparative neuroscience: an archive of digital records including records for endangered species Tomoko Sakai1,2,3,4 · Junichi Hata2,3,5 · Hiroki Ohta3 · Yuta Shintaku6,7 · Naoto Kimura7 · Yuki Ogawa3 · Kazumi Sogabe8 · Susumu Mori1,9 · Hirotaka James Okano3 · Yuzuru Hamada10 · Shinsuke Shibata2 · Hideyuki Okano2,5 · Kenichi Oishi1 Received: 26 February 2018 / Accepted: 9 October 2018 / Published online: 24 October 2018 © The Author(s) 2018 Abstract Advances in magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and computational analysis technology have enabled comparisons among various primate brains in a three-dimensional electronic format. Results from comparative studies provide information about common features across primates and species-specifc features of neuroanatomy. Investigation of various species of non- human primates is important for understanding such features, but the majority of comparative MRI studies have been based on experimental primates, such as common marmoset, macaques, and chimpanzee. A major obstacle has been the lack of a database that includes non-experimental primates’ brain MRIs. To facilitate scientifc discoveries in the feld of comparative neuroanatomy and brain evolution, we launched a collaborative project to develop an open-resource repository of non-human primate brain images obtained using ex vivo MRI. As an initial open resource, here we release a collection of structural MRI and difusion tensor images obtained from 12 species: pygmy marmoset, owl monkey, white-fronted capuchin, crab-eating macaque, Japanese macaque, bonnet macaque, toque macaque, Sykes’ monkey, red-tailed monkey, Schmidt’s guenon, de Brazza’s guenon, and lar gibbon.
    [Show full text]
  • Macaca Thibetana)
    ZOOLOGICAL RESEARCH Interchange between grooming and infant handling in female Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana) Qi Jiang1, Dong-Po Xia2, Xi Wang1, Dao Zhang1, Bing-Hua Sun1, Jin-Hua Li1,3,* 1 School of Resource and Environmental Engineering, Anhui University, Hefei Anhui 230601, China 2 School of Life Science, Anhui University, Hefei Anhui 230601, China 3 School of Life Science, Hefei Normal University, Hefei Anhui 241000, China ABSTRACT Keywords: Tibetan macaques (Macaca thibetana); Interchange; Infant handling; Grooming; Biological In some non-human primates, infants function as market theory a social tool that can bridge relationships among INTRODUCTION group members. Infants are a desired commodity for group members, and mothers control access to While female attraction to infants represents a common feature them. The biological market theory suggests that in non-human primate species, maternal responses to infant handling show a certain degree of variability (Maestripieri, grooming is widespread and represents a commodity 1994; Nicolson, 1987). In some species, such as Asian that can be exchanged for infant handling. As colobines, females allow their newborn infants to be held a limited resource, however, the extent to which frequently and carried for long durations by other group infants are interchanged between mothers (females members (infant caretaking) (Nicolson, 1987; Stanford, 1992). with an infant) and non-mothers (potential handlers, In other species, such as baboons and macaques, mothers are much more restrictive in allowing access their young females without an infant) remains unclear. In this offspring (Altmann, 2002; Nicolson, 1987), despite persistent study, we collected behavioral data to investigate the attempts by group members to interact with their infant (Frank relationship between grooming and infant handling in & Silk, 2009; Gumert, 2007a; Henzi & Barrett, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • Laboratory Primate Newsletter
    LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 45, No. 3 July 2006 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR, CONSULTING EDITOR ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Psychology Department, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and re- lated matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) dis- seminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of indi- vidual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research prob- lems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special con- sideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $5.00 each.
    [Show full text]
  • Population Dynamics of the Toque Monkey, Macaca Sinica
    /?7_f-P'^KS Population Dynamics of the Toque Monkey, Macaca sinica 1975. In: "Socioecology and Psychology of Primates". ii. II. 'l\ittle, ed., I out on xtiblishers: 'Dhe Hague, pp. 125-151 WOLFGANG P. J. DITTUS INTRODUCTION Fundamental to any understanding of the adaptation of animals to their natural environment is the knowledge of their numbers and life and death characteristics. The data on population dynamics to be presented here is based on a three and one-half year field study of the toque macaque, Macaca sinica (Linnaeus 1771) of Sri Lanka (Ceylon). Publications deal- ing with aspects of ecology and behavior are in preparation. The toque macaque, a member of the family Cercopithecidae, shares the subgenus Zati (Reichenbach 1862) with the bonnet macaque, M. radiata (Geoffroy 1812), of southern India and possibly M. assamensis (McClelland 1839) of northeastern India and Burma (Hill and Bernstein 1969). In its range M. sinica is confined to the forested regions of Ceylon, and three subspecies have been recognized; M. s. sinica (Linnaeus 1771), M. s. aurijrons(Pocock 1931), andM. s. op'tsthomelas(Hill 1942), Research was supported, in part, by National Science Foundation grant GB-3545, awarded to Dr. John F. Eisenberg; National Institute of Mental Health grant RolMH15673-01; -02; research grant 686 from the National Geographic Society; and Smithsonian Institution Foreign Currency Program grant SFC-7004, awarded to Dr. I. F. Eisenberg and Dr. Suzanne Ripley. It is a pleasure to express my gratitude to Dr. John F. Eisenberg whose support, advice and kind encouragement made this research a reality. I thank especially Messrs.
    [Show full text]
  • Mandrillus Leucophaeus Poensis)
    Ecology and Behavior of the Bioko Island Drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus poensis) A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Drexel University by Jacob Robert Owens in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2013 i © Copyright 2013 Jacob Robert Owens. All Rights Reserved ii Dedications To my wife, Jen. iii Acknowledgments The research presented herein was made possible by the financial support provided by Primate Conservation Inc., ExxonMobil Foundation, Mobil Equatorial Guinea, Inc., Margo Marsh Biodiversity Fund, and the Los Angeles Zoo. I would also like to express my gratitude to Dr. Teck-Kah Lim and the Drexel University Office of Graduate Studies for the Dissertation Fellowship and the invaluable time it provided me during the writing process. I thank the Government of Equatorial Guinea, the Ministry of Fisheries and the Environment, Ministry of Information, Press, and Radio, and the Ministry of Culture and Tourism for the opportunity to work and live in one of the most beautiful and unique places in the world. I am grateful to the faculty and staff of the National University of Equatorial Guinea who helped me navigate the geographic and bureaucratic landscape of Bioko Island. I would especially like to thank Jose Manuel Esara Echube, Claudio Posa Bohome, Maximilliano Fero Meñe, Eusebio Ondo Nguema, and Mariano Obama Bibang. The journey to my Ph.D. has been considerably more taxing than I expected, and I would not have been able to complete it without the assistance of an expansive list of people. I would like to thank all of you who have helped me through this process, many of whom I lack the space to do so specifically here.
    [Show full text]
  • Primate Occurrence Across a Human- Impacted Landscape In
    Primate occurrence across a human- impacted landscape in Guinea-Bissau and neighbouring regions in West Africa: using a systematic literature review to highlight the next conservation steps Elena Bersacola1,2, Joana Bessa1,3, Amélia Frazão-Moreira1,4, Dora Biro3, Cláudia Sousa1,4,† and Kimberley Jane Hockings1,4,5 1 Centre for Research in Anthropology (CRIA/NOVA FCSH), Lisbon, Portugal 2 Anthropological Centre for Conservation, the Environment and Development (ACCEND), Department of Humanities and Social Sciences, Oxford Brookes University, Oxford, United Kingdom 3 Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom 4 Department of Anthropology, Faculty of Social Sciences and Humanities, Universidade NOVA de Lisboa, Lisbon, Portugal 5 Centre for Ecology and Conservation, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Cornwall, United Kingdom † Deceased. ABSTRACT Background. West African landscapes are largely characterised by complex agroforest mosaics. Although the West African forests are considered a nonhuman primate hotspot, knowledge on the distribution of many species is often lacking and out- of-date. Considering the fast-changing nature of the landscapes in this region, up- to-date information on primate occurrence is urgently needed, particularly of taxa such as colobines, which may be more sensitive to habitat modification than others. Understanding wildlife occurrence and mechanisms of persistence in these human- dominated landscapes is fundamental for developing effective conservation strategies. Submitted 2 March 2018 Accepted 6 May 2018 Methods. In this paper, we aim to review current knowledge on the distribution of Published 23 May 2018 three threatened primates in Guinea-Bissau and neighbouring regions, highlighting Corresponding author research gaps and identifying priority research and conservation action.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduced Primate Species, Chlorocebus Sabaeus, in Dania
    AN INTRODUCED PRIMATE SPECIES, CHLOROCEBUS SABAEUS, IN DANIA BEACH, FLORIDA: INVESTIGATING ORIGINS, DEMOGRAPHICS, AND ANTHROPOGENIC IMPLICATIONS OF AN ESTABLISHED POPULATION by Deborah M. Williams A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of The Charles E. Schmidt College of Science In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton, FL May 2019 Copyright 2019 by Deborah M. Williams ii AN INTRODUCED PRIMATE SPECIES, CHLOROCEBUS SABAEUS, IN DANIA BEACH, FLORIDA: INVESTIGATING ORIGINS, DEMOGRAPHICS, AND ANTHROPOGENIC IMPLICATIONS OF AN ESTABLISHED POPULATION by Deborah M. Williams This dissertation was prepared under the direction of the candidate's dissertation advisor, Dr. Kate Detwiler, Department of Biological Sciences, and has been approved by all members of the supervisory committee. It was submitted to the faculty of the Charles E. Schmidt College of Science and was accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. SUPERVISORY COMMITTEE: ~ ~,'£-____ Colin Hughes, Ph.D. ~~ Marianne Porter, P6.D. I Sciences arajedini, Ph.D. Dean, Charles E. Schmidt College of Science ~__5~141'~ Khaled Sobhan, Ph.D. Interim Dean, Graduate College iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are so many people who made this possible. It truly takes a village. A big thank you to my husband, Roy, who was my rock during this journey. He offered a shoulder to lean on, an ear to listen, and a hand to hold. Also, thank you to my son, Blake, for tolerating the late pick-ups from school and always knew when a hug was needed. I could not have done it without them.
    [Show full text]
  • Chewing and Sucking Lice As Parasites of Iviammals and Birds
    c.^,y ^r-^ 1 Ag84te DA Chewing and Sucking United States Lice as Parasites of Department of Agriculture IVIammals and Birds Agricultural Research Service Technical Bulletin Number 1849 July 1997 0 jc: United States Department of Agriculture Chewing and Sucking Agricultural Research Service Lice as Parasites of Technical Bulletin Number IVIammals and Birds 1849 July 1997 Manning A. Price and O.H. Graham U3DA, National Agrioultur«! Libmry NAL BIdg 10301 Baltimore Blvd Beltsvjlle, MD 20705-2351 Price (deceased) was professor of entomoiogy, Department of Ento- moiogy, Texas A&iVI University, College Station. Graham (retired) was research leader, USDA-ARS Screwworm Research Laboratory, Tuxtia Gutiérrez, Chiapas, Mexico. ABSTRACT Price, Manning A., and O.H. Graham. 1996. Chewing This publication reports research involving pesticides. It and Sucking Lice as Parasites of Mammals and Birds. does not recommend their use or imply that the uses U.S. Department of Agriculture, Technical Bulletin No. discussed here have been registered. All uses of pesti- 1849, 309 pp. cides must be registered by appropriate state or Federal agencies or both before they can be recommended. In all stages of their development, about 2,500 species of chewing lice are parasites of mammals or birds. While supplies last, single copies of this publication More than 500 species of blood-sucking lice attack may be obtained at no cost from Dr. O.H. Graham, only mammals. This publication emphasizes the most USDA-ARS, P.O. Box 969, Mission, TX 78572. Copies frequently seen genera and species of these lice, of this publication may be purchased from the National including geographic distribution, life history, habitats, Technical Information Service, 5285 Port Royal Road, ecology, host-parasite relationships, and economic Springfield, VA 22161.
    [Show full text]