Defensive and Exploratory Reactions of Black and Golden-Headed Lion Tamarins Exposed to a Mirror

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Defensive and Exploratory Reactions of Black and Golden-Headed Lion Tamarins Exposed to a Mirror Revista de Etologia 2001, Vol.3, N°1, 15-23 Defensive and exploratory responses to mirrors in lion tamarins The Image Is Another Tamarin: Defensive and Exploratory Reactions of Black and Golden-Headed Lion Tamarins Exposed to a Mirror CÉSAR ADES, LUCIANO TOMÉ AND VÂNIA HADDAD DIEGO Universidade de São Paulo To determine the nature of the social behavior of black (Leontopithecus chrysopygus) and golden-faced (L. chrysomelas) lion tamarins toward their mirror-image, the responses of lion tamarins of both species (defense, exploration, observation of mirror and stay in proximity of the mirror) were assessed in their home cages during two sessions of exposure to mirrors and compared to baseline levels. Mirrors elicited, on first and second presentation, strong defensive responses such as vocalizations, scent marking, increased locomotion, as well as exploration and observation directed to the mirror. On the second exposure, there was a decrease in defensive performance for black but not for golden-headed lion tamarins, while exploratory and looking to the mirror levels remained at their high, former levels. Results indicate that lion tamarins, contrary to some indication of non-aggressive responsiveness to mirror-image in cotton-top tamarins, reacts defensively to mirror-images as though they were interacting with an intruder. Permanence of observing responses to the mirror-image, at least in black lion tamarins, may indicate the existence of a social exploration tendency, independent from aggressiveness toward strange conspecifics. Index terms: Mirrors, Defensive behavior. Lion tamarins. Leontopithecus chrysopygus, L. chrysomelas. A imagem é outro mico: reações defensivas e exploratórias de micos-leões-pretos e micos-leões-de-cara- dourada diante do espelho. A fim de determinar a natureza do comportamento social de micos-leões-pretos (Leontopithecus chrysopygus) e micos-leões-de-cara-dourada (L. chrysomelas) diante de sua imagem ao espelho, fo- ram registradas por duas sessões, nos seus recintos, as respostas ao espelho de indivíduos de ambas as espécies – defesa, exploração, olhar para o espelho e permanência próximo ao espelho – comparando os níveis encontra- dos aos da linha de base. O espelho provocou respostas defensivas intensas – vocalizações, marcação de cheiro, locomoção, etc. - em ambas as apresentações, assim como comportamentos de exploração e de observação dirigidos ao espelho. Os micos-leões-pretos, mas não os micos-leões-de-cara-dourada, diminuiram seu compor- tamento defensivo na segunda sessão, mantendo contudo altos a exploração e a observação do espelho. Os resultados indicam que, contrariamente a relatos sobre a pouca agressividade de sagüis-de-topete diante do espelho, os micos-leões reagem defensivamente à sua imagem, como se ela representasse um indivíduo estra- nho de sua espécie. A permanência das respostas de observação do espelho, pelo menos em micos-leões-pretos, pode ser uma indicação da existência de uma tendência para a exploração social, independente da agressividade. Descritores: Espelhos. Comportamento defensivo. Micos-leões. Leontopithecus chrysopygus. L. chrysomelas. Many animals ignore their image seen in (MacLean, 1964), pygmy marmosets Cebuella a mirror, but in some species with well- pygmea (Eglash & Snowdon, 1983) and Cerra- developed vision, particularly primates, mirrors may elicitate species-typical exploratory or so- Thanks are due to the chrysopygus (Elisabete C. Lopes, cial responses. Among the New World monkeys, Karin A. Onodera e Úrsula M. Liemert) and to the chrysomelas (Mariana M. Kroll, Lucas R. Napolitano, capuchins Cebus apella (Anderson & Roeder, Gigliana S. Lima, Liana L. de Souza and Marcos Takahashi) 1989) squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus teams for cooperation and enthusiasm during data collection, to the Fundação Parque Zoológico de São Pau- César Ades, Instituto de Psicologia, USP. Av. Prof. Mello lo for the permission to perform our experiment with lion Moraes, 1721, 05508-900. E-mail:[email protected] tamarins and to FAPESP and CNPq for research grants. 15 César Ades, Luciano Tomé and Vânia Haddad Diego do marmosets Callithrix penicillata (Perin & Guer- touching the place on their head where hair had ra, 2000) have been observed to react with been dyed. Again, tamarins “stared into the increased curiosity and/or aggressiveness to mirror peacefully and for long durations, but their mirror-image, at least during the first did not vocalize or act aggressively” (p. 10813). exposures, in a manner which suggest that they They did so for longer periods than they had may take the image for a conspecific. done before their hair changed colors. Hauser, Mirror tests have also been used as a Miller, Liu, and Gupta (2001) recently technique for assessing self-recognition in performed a replication of Hauser et al. (1995) primates. Chimpanzees may react to their image experiment, as well as a new one designed to with threatening displays but on repeated follow closely the technique used with exposure, they engage in a variety of self- chimpanzees, and failed, in both, to find any directed behaviors when looking at themselves evidence of self-recognition as indicated by (such as watching inside their own mouth), and touches to the dye-marked area. Animals may inspect and touch parts of their face however tended to look longer at the mirror – marked with dye which they detect through the relatively to habituation sessions – when first mirror-image (Gallup, 1977; Povinelli, Rulf, exposed to their new, colored visual Landau, & Bierschwale, 1993). Self directed appearance. Hauser et al. (2001) again mention behavior at the mirror, reported in chimpanzees that during familiarization to the mirror, in and orangutans but not in gorillas or in Experiment 2, no head touching and no monkeys, has been interpreted by Gallup (1998) aggressive displays toward the mirror were as indicating that, in some apes at least, a fun- recorded. During test sessions, animals did not damental evolutionary step towards self- direct prolonged looks to the mirror, nor did consciousness and towards attribution of they display aggressively. consciousness to conspecifics was taken (see This lack or low frequency of agonistic Ades, 1998; Pereira Jr., 1999, and de Veer e van responses, in both studies of Hauser et al. (1995, den Bos, 1999 for critical discussions of such 2001) is somewhat surprising once, in many interpretations). species of primates, mirror images elicit intense Our interest in testing lion tamarins in a and prolonged threat and defensive displays. mirror context was aroused by the experiment The purpose of the present research, performed on self-recognition Hauser, Kralik, Botto- with captive black (Leonthopithecus chrysopygus) Mahan, Garrett, and Oser (1995) conducted on and golden-headed lion tamarins (L. cotton-tops tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Mirrors chrysomelas), was not to find an evidence of self- were mounted in the cages of tamarins for a knowledge through a Gallup mark test, but to period before experimental manipulations were investigate the nature of the reactions of lion performed. First contact with mirrors was tamarins to mirrors and to discover whether generally not aggressive. As Hauser et al. (1995) they are basically of the self or conspecific- put it: “tamarins with access to mirrors showed directed type. Our hypothesis was that the initial interest, evidenced by brief stares, visual monkeys would react with threat and other and manual searches behind the mirror, and defensive responses to their appearance in the occasional threats … mirror stares appeared mirror, as they would react to an intruder generally nonaggressive, with no accompanying conspecific. vocalizations and only five threat displays” (p. Lion tamarins, the larger of callithrichids, 10811). A mark test, analogous to the one are endangered species which live in swamp and performed on chimpanzees, was performed by dryland forests (Albernaz, 1997), in nuclear or dying the white hair on the tamarins’ head with extended families. While there may be ingroup bright colours. Only individuals with long aggressiveness, threats and aggressive categories previous exposures to the mirror were observed of behavior are mostly directed towards 16 Defensive and exploratory responses to mirrors in lion tamarins members of other groups. “Visual contact often with a front wire mesh partition, a small shelter leads to high arousal with considerable in its rear part and several tree trunks, two of locomotion, piloerection, and vocalizing. In our which served as bridges between shelter and the experience, injuries often result when two fore part of the enclosure. captive groups have had the opportunity to At the beginning of the sessions, a 50 x make physical contact through small openings 30 cm metallic, horizontal tray with a 50 x 55 between enclosures” (Kleiman, 1984, p. 39). cm vertical mirror above it, were mounted on Intergroup aggressive encounters in the field the wire mesh front partition, at approximately which have to do with feeding resource 1,5 m from the floor. Flour beetle larvae and competition involve visual contact, alarm calls, gum candies were spread on the tray, in all scent marking and sequences of chase and sessions, to attract tamarins. Animals were retreats, as in other callithrichid species. In his subjected to a 15-min baseline session with the research with wild golden lion tamarins L. rosalia mirror in a control position, opaque surface at the Poço das Antas Biological Reserve, Peres facing the enclosure,
Recommended publications
  • Foraging Behavior and Microhabitats Used by Black Lion Tamarins, Leontopithecus Chrysopyqus (Mikan) (Primates, Callitrichidae)
    Foraging behavior and microhabitats used by black lion tamarins, Leontopithecus chrysopyqus (Mikan) (Primates, Callitrichidae) Fernando de Camargo Passos 2 Alexine Keuroghlian 3 ABSTRACT. Foraging in the Black Lion Tamarin (L. chrysopygus Mikan, 1823) was observed in the Caetetus Ecological Station, São Paulo, southeastern Brazil, during 83 days between November 1988 to October 1990. These tamarins use manipuJative, specitic-site foraging behavior. When searching for animal prey items, they examine a variety ofmicrohabitats (dry palm leaves, twigs, under loose bark, in tree cavities). These microhabitats were spatially dispersed among different forest macrohabitats such as swamp torests and dry forested areas. These data indicated that the prey foraging behavior of L. chrysopygus was quite variable, and they used a wide variety ofmicrohabitats, different ofthe other lion tall1arin species. KEY WORDS. Callitrichidae, Leontopithecus chrysopygus, black lion tamarin, ani­ mai prey, foraging behavior, diet, microhabitats Lion tamarins, Leontopithecus Lesson, 1840, are considered primarily in­ sectivores and frugivores (COIMBRA-FILHO & MITTERMEIER 1973), or omnivores (KLElMAN et aI. 1988) because of the diversity of their diet. ln the wild, they consume mostly fruits, exudates, nectar, and animal prey. ln comparison to fruits, animal prey make up a relatively small proportion ofthe diet and are costly to obtain, but its nutritional vai ue make it an essential component of their diet. The prey of black lion tamarins (L. chrysopygus) may include a variety ofinvertebrates (insects, spiders, and other arthropods) and small vertebrates, such as anuran frogs (CARVA­ LHO et aI. 1989; PASSOS 1999). ln this note, we present our observations on prey foraging. We then compare them with studies of other lion tamarin species and discuss some of the unique aspects ofblack lion tamarin foraging in re\ation to the microhabitats they use.
    [Show full text]
  • Lion Tamarin PHVA 3.Pdf
    Lion Tamarin PHVA WORKSHOP FINAL REPORT Escola de Administração Fazendária (ESAF) Brasília, Distrito Federal, Brazil 7-11 JUNE 2005 Institutional Support IBAMA - Instituto Brasileiro do Meio Ambiente e dos Recursos Naturais Renováveis IPÊ - Instituto de Pesquisas Ecológicas AMLD - Associação Mico-Leão-Dourado IESB - Instituto de Estudos Sócio-Ambientais do Sul da Bahia Planning Commitee General Organization Onildo João Marini Filho, IBAMA Cláudio Valladares-Padua, IPÊ Denise Rambaldi, AMLD Jonathan Ballou, NZP Devra Kleiman, ICCM Patrícia Medici, IPE, IUCN/SSC CBSG Brasil Bengt Holst, ICCM, IUCN/SSC CBSG Europe PHVA Workshop Design and Report Patrícia Medici, IPE, IUCN/SSC CBSG Brasil Bengt Holst, IUCN/SSC CBSG Europe Layout and cover graphics: Mikkel Stelvig, Copenhagen Zoo A contribution of the Conservation Breeding Specialist Group. © Copyright CBSG 2006 Holst, B., E.P. Medici, O.J. Marino-Filho, D. Kleiman, K. Leus, A. Pissinatti, G. Vivekananda, J.D. Bal- lou, K. Traylor-Holzer, B. Raboy, F. Passos, K. Vleeschouwer and M.M. Montenegro (eds.). 2006. Lion Tamarin Population and Habitat Viability Assessment Workshop 2005, final report. IUCN/SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group, Apple Valley, MN, USA. Third Lion Tamarin Population and Habitat Viability Assessment (PHVA) Escola de Administração Fazendária (ESAF) Brasília, Distrito Federal, Brazil 7-11 June 2005 IUCN disclaimer: • with respect to content: IUCN encourages meetings, workshops and other fora for the consideration and analysis of issues related to conservation, and believes that reports of these meetings are most useful when broadly disseminated. The opinions and views expressed by the authors may not necessarily reflect the formal policies of IUCN, its Commissions, its Secretariat or its members.
    [Show full text]
  • The Black Lion Tamarin Leontopithecus Chrysopygus - Its Conservation and Management
    ORYX VOL 32 NO 4 OCTOBER 1998 The black lion tamarin Leontopithecus chrysopygus - its conservation and management Ana Carolina Mamede-Costa and Nivar Gobbi The black lion tamarin Leontopithecus chrysopygus originally occurred throughout a large part of the Atlantic forest in the west of the state of Sao Paulo, Brazil. Today, however, it is restricted to a few isolated forest fragments as a result of deforestation caused by cattle ranching, and urban and agricultural expansion, especially in this century. One of its last strongholds is a small gallery forest at Lengois Paulista in the west-central part of the state. The authors report on a long-term study of this small and isolated population, aimed particularly at providing a basis for the intensive management and conservation of the species and its habitat. Introduction Paulo, and in 1905 O. Hume donated a speci- men to the museum, collected from Bauru All four species of lion tamarins are threat- (Coimbra-Filho, 1970, 1976). These were the ened with extinction as a result of the wide- only records for the species until Adelmar F. spread and almost total destruction of the Coimbra-Filho discovered two specimens Atlantic forest: c. 7 per cent remains of the mounted by a taxidermist in the town of forests that once covered more than 1,200,000 Presidente Wenceslau in 1970. They were re- sq km of eastern and southern Brazil. All 18 of ported to have been obtained from a forest the Atlantic forest's endemic primates were patch on a nearby ranch, the Fazenda ranked as Critically Endangered, Endangered Kitayama, which, however, had already been or Vulnerable in the World Conservation destroyed.
    [Show full text]
  • WILDLIFE in a CHANGING WORLD an Analysis of the 2008 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species™
    WILDLIFE IN A CHANGING WORLD An analysis of the 2008 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species™ Edited by Jean-Christophe Vié, Craig Hilton-Taylor and Simon N. Stuart coberta.indd 1 07/07/2009 9:02:47 WILDLIFE IN A CHANGING WORLD An analysis of the 2008 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species™ first_pages.indd I 13/07/2009 11:27:01 first_pages.indd II 13/07/2009 11:27:07 WILDLIFE IN A CHANGING WORLD An analysis of the 2008 IUCN Red List of Threatened Species™ Edited by Jean-Christophe Vié, Craig Hilton-Taylor and Simon N. Stuart first_pages.indd III 13/07/2009 11:27:07 The designation of geographical entities in this book, and the presentation of the material, do not imply the expressions of any opinion whatsoever on the part of IUCN concerning the legal status of any country, territory, or area, or of its authorities, or concerning the delimitation of its frontiers or boundaries. The views expressed in this publication do not necessarily refl ect those of IUCN. This publication has been made possible in part by funding from the French Ministry of Foreign and European Affairs. Published by: IUCN, Gland, Switzerland Red List logo: © 2008 Copyright: © 2009 International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources Reproduction of this publication for educational or other non-commercial purposes is authorized without prior written permission from the copyright holder provided the source is fully acknowledged. Reproduction of this publication for resale or other commercial purposes is prohibited without prior written permission of the copyright holder. Citation: Vié, J.-C., Hilton-Taylor, C.
    [Show full text]
  • PREY FORAGING BEHAVIOR, SEASONALITY and TIME-BUDGETS in BLACK LION TAMARINS, Leontopithecus Chrysopygus (MIKAN 1823) (MAMMALIA, CALLITRICHIDAE)
    FORAGING BEHAVIOR IN Leontopithecus chrysopygus 455 PREY FORAGING BEHAVIOR, SEASONALITY AND TIME-BUDGETS IN BLACK LION TAMARINS, Leontopithecus chrysopygus (MIKAN 1823) (MAMMALIA, CALLITRICHIDAE) KEUROGHLIAN, A.1 and PASSOS, F. C.2 1Department of Wildlife Management, West Virginia University, Morgantown, West Virginia, 26505-6125, USA 2Departamento de Zoologia, Universidade Estadual de Campinas, Campinas, Brazil Correspondence to: Fernando de Camargo Passos, Departamento de Zoologia, Universidade Federal do Paraná, C.P. 19020, CEP 81531-990, Curitiba, PR, Brazil, e-mail: [email protected] Received May 13, 1999 – Accepted July 18, 2000 – Distributed August 31, 2001 ABSTRACT Foraging behavior, seasonality and time-budgets in the Black Lion Tamarin (L. chrysopygus) was obser- ved in the Caetetus Ecological Station, South-eastern Brazil, during 83 days between November 1988 to October 1990. For the full dry season we found that animal prey represented 11.2% of the black lion tamarin diet, while during the wet season they represented 1.9%. Foraging behavior made up 19.8% of their total activity in the dry season and only 12.8% in the wet season. These results point out that animal prey are relatively more important during the dry season, due to reduced availability of other resources, e.g. fruits, and that a greater foraging effort is required when a larger proportion of the diet is animal prey. Key words: Leontopithecus chrysopygus, foraging behavior, seasonality, time-budgets, Callitrichidae. RESUMO Comportamento de forrageio por presas, sazonalidade e orçamento temporário das atividades do mico-leão-preto, Leontopithecus chrysopygus (Mikan 1823) (Mammalia, Callitrichidae) O comportamento de forrageio por presas, a sazonalidade e o orçamento temporal das atividades no mico-leão-preto (L.
    [Show full text]
  • Avaliação Histológica Da População De Folículos Pré
    UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL FLUMINENSE ALLINE FERREIRA BRASIL AVALIAÇÃO HISTOLÓGICA DA POPULAÇÃO DE FOLÍCULOS PRÉ- ANTRAIS OVARIANOS E DA TAXA DE ATRESIA FOLICULAR EM FÊMEAS DE DIFERENTES ESPÉCIES E POSIÇÕES HIERÁRQUICAS DE Leontopithecus (CALLITRICHIDAE – PRIMATES) MANTIDAS EM CATIVEIRO NITERÓI 2008 ALLINE FERREIRA BRASIL AVALIAÇÃO HISTOLÓGICA DA POPULAÇÃO DE FOLÍCULOS PRÉ-ANTRAIS OVARIANOS E DA TAXA DE ATRESIA FOLICULAR EM FÊMEAS DE DIFERENTES ESPÉCIES E POSIÇÕES HIERÁRQUICAS DE Leontopithecus (CALLITRICHIDAE – PRIMATES) MANTIDAS EM CATIVEIRO Te se apresentada ao Curso de Pós-Graduação em Patologia da Universidade Fe deral Fluminense, como requisito parcial pa ra obtenção do Grau de Doutor. Área de concentração: Anatomia Patológica Humana e Veterinária. Orientador: Dra. Ana Maria Reis Ferreira Niterói 2008 Brasil, Alline Ferreira Avaliação Histológica d a População de Folículos Pré-Antrais Ovarianos e da Taxa de Atresia Folicular em Fêmeas de diferentes espécies e Posições Hierárqu icas de Leontopithecus Sp. (Callitrichidae – Primates) mantidas em Cativeiro / Alline Ferreira Brasil.- Niterói, 2008. 101 f. Tese de Doutorado (An atomia Patológica Humana e Veterinária- Programa de Pós- graduação em Patologia)- Universidade Federal Fluminense Orientador: Dra. Ana Maria Re is Ferreira Bibliografia: f 90-101 1. Leontopithecus. 2 . DEGENERAÇÃO 3. FOLÍCULOS PRÉ- ANTRAIS. I. UNIVERSIDADE FEDERAL FLUMINENSE. II. Título CDD ALLINE FERREIRA BRASIL AVALIAÇÃO HISTOLÓGICA DA POPULAÇÃO DE FOLÍCULOS PRÉ-ANTRAIS OVARIANOS E DA TAXA DE ATRESIA FOLICULAR EM FÊMEAS DE DIFERENTES ESPÉCIES E POSIÇÕES HIERÁRQUICAS DE Leontopithecus (CALLITRICHIDAE – PRIMATES) MANTIDAS EM CATIVEIRO Tese apresentada ao Curso de Pós-Graduação em Patologia da Universidade Federal Fluminense, como requisito parcial para obtenção do Grau de Doutor. Área de concentração: Anatomia Patológica Humana e Veterinária.
    [Show full text]
  • Gorilla World and Jungle Trails PRIMATE EVOLUTION
    ALL ABOUT PRIMATES! Gorilla World and Jungle Trails PRIMATE EVOLUTION The ancestors of primates show up in the fossil record around 85 to 65 million years ago. The first true primates fossil was discovered in China and dates back 55 million years! The idea of the “missing link” is very misleading. Evolution is not a linear chain, but more like a complicated tree with many branches. THE MODERN PRIMATE Primates are a taxonomical Order of related species that fall under the Class Mammalia Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata Class: Mammalia Order: Primates From here primates tend to fall into 3 major categories THE THREE PRIMATE CATEGORIES Prosimians Monkeys Apes PROSIMIANS Prosimians represent the more “primitive” of primates General Characteristics: Small Size Nocturnal Relatively Solitary Grooming Claws and Tooth Combs Well-developed sense of smell Vertical Clingers and Leapers This group includes all lemurs, galagos, lorises, and tarsiers MONKEYS Monkeys are the most geographically diverse category of primates, spanning throughout South and Central America, Africa, Asia, and even one location in Europe General Characteristics Long Tails Diurnal (one exception) Increased sense of sight More complex social structures Increased Intelligence Quadrupedal Monkeys are classified as either New World or Old World NEW WORLD VS. OLD WORLD MONKEYS New World Monkeys span Old World Monkeys span throughout Central and throughout Europe, Africa, and South America. Asia. Characteristics: Round, flat Characteristics: Narrow, nostrils. Smaller in size. downward
    [Show full text]
  • 2008 Survey on Mixed Species Housing of Neotropical Primates
    2008 SURVEY ON MIXED SPECIES HOUSING OF NEOTROPICAL PRIMATES INTRODUCTION In early 2008, on behalf of the New World Monkey Taxon Advisory Group (TAG), a short questionnaire was sent to 185 AZA institutions to attempt to gather information pertaining to current trends in mixed species housing and exhibition of neotropical primates. A total of 65 institutions responded to the survey. Of these, forty-five institutions reported experiences with mixed species exhibits. The remaining 20 institutions responded that they had had no experience with mixed species exhibits. The questionnaire was identical to one that had been sent to AZA institutions in 1995. The results of the 1995 survey became the basis for a chapter on Housing and Exhibiting Mixed Species of Neotropical Primates (Sodaro, 1999) in the Callitrichid Husbandry Manual (Sodaro and Saunders, eds., 1999). The questionnaire consisted of the following four questions: 1. List the neotropical primate species that you have successfully mixed with each other in exhibits or off exhibit holding enclosures. Please describe the number of animals of each species as well as the age and sex composition of each species’ group. Briefly describe the method of introduction. 2. List the neotropical primate species that were not successfully mixed with each other. Please describe the number of animals of each species as well as the age and sex composition of each species’ group. Briefly explain why the combination was not successful, as well as the method of introduction. 3. List other species of animals that you have successfully mixed with neotropical primates. Briefly describe the method of introduction.
    [Show full text]
  • Callitrichines 85
    PIPC02b 11/7/05 17:20 Page 85 Callitrichines 85 Niemitz, C. (1984). Biology of Tarsiers. Gustav Fischer, Stuttgart. Schultz, A. (1948). The number of young at a birth and the number Niemitz, C., Nietsch, A., Warter, S., and Rumpler, Y. (1991). of nipples in primates. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 6:1–23. Tarsius dianae: a new primate species from central Sulawesi Shekelle, M. (2003). Taxonomy and biogeography of eastern (Indonesia). Folia Primatol. 56:105–116. Tarsiers [PhD thesis]. Washington University, St. Louis. Nietsch, A. (1999). Duet vocalizations among different popu- Shekelle, M., Leksono, S., Ischwan, L., and Masala, Y. (1997). lations of Sulawesi tarsiers. Int. J. Primatol. 20:567–583. The natural history of the tarsiers of north and central Sulawesi. Nietsch, A., and Kopp, M. (1998). The role of vocalization in species Sulawesi Prim. News. 4:4–11. differentiation of Sulawesi tarsiers. Folia Primatol. 69:371–378. Sherman, P., Braude, S., and Jarvis, J. (1999). Litter sizes and Nietsch, A., and Niemitz, C. (1992). Indication for facultative poly- mammary numbers of naked mole-rats: breaking the one-half gamy in free-ranging Tarsius spectrum, supported by morpho- rule. J. Mammal. 80:720–733. metric data. In: International Primatological Society Abstracts. Simons, E., and Bown, T. (1985). Afrotarsius chatrathi, the first International Primatological Society, Strasbourg. p. 318. tarsiiform primate (Tarsiidae) from Africa. Nature 313:4750– Pallas, P. S. (1778). Novae Species quad e Glirium Ordinae cum 477. Illustrationibus Variis Complurium ex hoc Ordinae Animalium, Simpson, G. G. (1945). The principles of classification and a W. Walther, Erlangen. classification of mammals.
    [Show full text]
  • Leontopithecus Chrysopygus in Capão Bonito, SP
    40 Neotropical Primates 23(1), August 2016 References DEMOGRAPHY OF THE BLACK LION TAMARIN Ankel-Simons, F. 2007. Primate Anatomy: An introduction. (LEONTOPITHECUS CHRYSOPYGUS, MIKAN) IN 3rd ed. Durham, North Carolina: Elsevier. CAPÃO BONITO NATIONAL FOREST (STATE OF Canizo, R. 2012. Ecologia e comportamento do leãozinho, SÃO PAULO) Cebuella pygmaea (Spix, 1823) (Primates, Callitrichidae) em um fragmento florestal.Dissertação de mestrado. Uni- Lucas Tadeu Pelagio Caldano versidade Federal do Acre, Rio Branco, Acre. Cauê Monticelli Canizo, R. and Calouro, A. M. 2011. Observação do com- Pedro Manoel Galetti Jr. portamento agonístico de Cebuella pygmaea sobre Sciurus spadiceus em um fragmento florestal no Estado do Acre, Introduction Brasil. Neotrop. Primates 18: 60–62. Messias, M. R.; Coragem, J. T.; Gomes, I. S. R.; Oliveira, The black lion tamarin is known to inhabit 11 Atlantic M. A. Bonavigo, P. H.; Nienow, S. S. and Souza, E. S. Forest fragments, with a total estimated population of 2011. Southern extension of the geographical range of 1,000 individuals in the wild (Kierulff et al., 2008). Its the pygmy marmoset Cebuella pygmaea niveiventris (Lön- conservation status went from Critically Endangered to nberg, 1940) in the southwestern Amazon basin, State of Endangered in recent years (IUCN, 2015) due to success- Rondônia, Brazil. Neotrop. Primates 18: 30–31. ful conservation efforts (Kierulff et al., 2008). The largest Moynihan, M. 1976. Notes on the ecology and behavior population of black lion tamarins (~820 animals) inhabits of the pygmy marmoset (Cebuella pygmaea) in Amazo- Morro do Diabo State Park (Valladares-Padua and Cullen nian Colombia. In: Neotropical primates: Field studies Jr.
    [Show full text]
  • (Leontopithecus Chrysopygus) in the Middle and Upper Paranapanema
    32 Neotropical Primates 22(1), June 2015 NEW RECORDS, RECONFIRMED SITES AND PROPOSALS FOR THE CONSERVATION OF BLACK LION TAMARIN (LEONTOPITHECUS CHRYSOPYGUS) IN THE MIDDLE AND UPPER PARANAPANEMA Laurence Culot1, Juliana Griese2, Christoph Knogge3, Marcelo Tonini4, Mayara Mulato dos Santos1, Camila Genaro Estevam1, Bruna Pinto Lopes1, Bruno Mantovani da Cunha1, Aline Bertanha Silva2, Bárbara Heliodora Soares do Prado5, Francini de Oliveira Garcia2, Renata Cristina Batista Fonseca6, Marcio Port-Carvalho7 1 Universidade Estadual Paulista, Campus Rio Claro, Departamento de Zoologia, Laboratório de Primatologia, Avenida 24ª, 1515, 13506-900 Rio Claro, SP, Brazil, [email protected] 2 Instituto Itapoty, Programa Nós e o Mico-leão, Juntos pela Conservação, Rua Franklin Gutierrez, 315, Centro, Itatinga, SP, 18690-000, Brazil, www.itapoty.org.br 3 Rua Esmeralda 43, Nazaré Paulista, SP, Brazil 4 Rua França 38, 18406430, Jardim Europa, Itapeva, SP, Brazil 5 Estação Ecológica de Angatuba, Seção Estação Experimental de Itapetininga – IF, CP 143, 18200-000, Itapetininga, SP, Brazil 6 Universidade Estadual Paulista, Campus Botucatu - Fazenda Lageado, Faculdade de Ciências Agronômicas, Departamento de Ciência Florestal, Avenida José Barbosa de Barros, 1780, Botucatu, SP, Brazil 7 Seção de Animais Silvestres, Instituto Florestal, Rua do Horto, 931, São Paulo, SP, Brazil Abstract Te Black Lion Tamarin (BLT, Leontopithecus chrysopygus) is an endangered species, endemic to the State of Sao Paulo, Brazil. Te conservation of the species depends on the efective management of the populations and forest conservation but also on the updated knowledge of its areas of occurrence. Here, we report the record of a new site of BLT occurrence in the municipality of Guareí and confrm the presence of this species in two municipalities, Taquarivaí and Borebi, which (Borebi) is the frst record since the last two decades in the Middle Paranapanema basin.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Monogamy in Primates: a Phylogenetic Approach
    THE EVOLUTION OF MONOGAMY IN PRIMATES: A PHYLOGENETIC APPROACH A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Alana Hope Muhlberger May, 2011 Thesis written by Alana Hope Muhlberger B.A., Ohio University, 2009 M.A., Kent State University, 2011 Approved by _________________________________, Advisor Marilyn A. Norconk _________________________________, Chair, Department of Anthropology Richard S. Meindl _________________________________, Associate Dean, College of Arts and Sciences John Stalvey ii TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES ............................................................................................................ v LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................. vi ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .............................................................................................. vii CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ....................................................................................... 8 1.1 Social System Terminology .................................................................................. 11 1.2 Monogamy ............................................................................................................ 12 1.3 Previous Work ...................................................................................................... 19 1.4 Research Questions ..............................................................................................
    [Show full text]