26. Musical Syntax II: Empirical Perspectives

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

26. Musical Syntax II: Empirical Perspectives 487 26.Musical Musical Syntax II: Empirical Synt Perspectives a Marcus Pearce, Martin Rohrmeier 26.1 Computational Research.................... 487 Efforts to develop a formal characterization of mu- 26.1.1 Foundations ..................................... 487 sical structure are often framed in syntactic terms, 26.1.2 Early Approaches: Pattern Processing... 488 sometimes but not always with direct inspiration 26.1.3 Markov Modeling............................... 489 from research on language. In Chap. 25,wepresent 26.1.4 Beyond Simple Markov Models: Hidden syntactic approaches to characterizing musical Markov Models structure and survey a range of theoretical issues and Dynamic Bayesian Networks ........ 490 involved in developing formal syntactic theories 26.1.5 Hierarchical Models ........................... 491 of sequential structure in music. Such theories 26.1.6 Neural Networks................................ 493 are often computational in nature, lending them- 26.2 Psychological Research...................... 494 selves to implementation and our first goal here 26.2.1 Perception of Local Dependencies ....... 494 is to review empirical research on computational 26.2.2 Perception of Nonlocal Dependencies.. 495 modeling of musical structure from a syntactic point of view. We ask about the motivations for 26.3 Neuroscientific Research.................... 496 implementing a model and assess the range of 26.3.1 Introduction ..................................... 496 approaches that have been taken to date. It is im- 26.3.2 Neural Basis of Syntactic Processing portant to note that while a computational model in Music............................................ 496 may be capable of deriving an optimal structural 26.3.3 Syntax in Music and Language ............ 497 description of a piece of music, human cognitive 26.3.4 Grouping Structure ............................ 497 processing may not achieve this optimal perfor- 26.4 Implications and Issues..................... 498 mance, or may even process syntax in a different 26.4.1 Convergence Between Approaches ...... 498 way. Therefore we emphasize the difference be- 26.4.2 Syntax, Semantics and Emotion .......... 498 tween developing an optimal model of syntactic References................................................... 499 processing and developing a model that simu- lates human syntactic processing. Furthermore, we argue that, while optimal models (e.g., op- musical syntax from the perspective of compu- Part C | 26.1 timal compression or prediction) can be useful tational modeling, experimental psychology and as a benchmark or yardstick for assessing human cognitive neuroscience. There exists a large num- performance, if we wish to understand human ber of computational models of musical syntax, but cognition then simulating human performance we limit ourselves to those that are explicitly cog- (including aspects that are nonoptimal or even nitively motivated, assessing them in the context erroneous) should be the priority. Following this of theoretical, psychological and neuroscientific principle, we survey research on processing of research. 26.1 Computational Research 26.1.1 Foundations of representing. Therefore, there is a direct link between the theoretical characterization of musical syntax (dis- Different approaches to building computer models of cussedinChap.25) and the implementation and testing musical structure can be characterized, and distin- of these theories as computational models of cogni- guished, in terms of how expressive they are in terms tion, discussed here. Implementing a theory of musical of the degree of structural complexity they are capable syntax and processing has several potential advantages, © Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2018 R. Bader (Ed.), Springer Handbook of Systematic Musicology, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-662-55004-5_26 488 Part C Music Psychology – Physiology following well-known examples in cognitive science: ers form. Nonetheless, it is often useful to identify theoretical bounds on structural complexity using an 1. Implementing a theory as a computer program optimal model. Furthermore, theoretical models of mu- (which must run, generating output from the data sical structure can help us understand the hypothesis and parameters supplied as input) ensures that it space that human learners are faced with when they takes as little as possible for granted and any as- acquire the syntactic structure of a musical style. In sumptions are explicitly stated [26.1–3]. artificial intelligence research, we distinguish between 2. Experiments can be run to evaluate the implemented the representation defining the search space and algo- theory by comparison of its behavior with the output rithms for traversing the space. Similarly in machine expected from theoretical accounts or directly with learning, we distinguish between the hypothesis space human behavior given the same inputs [26.3, 4]. and learning mechanisms for traversing the space. In the case of musical syntax, the hypotheses correspond It is important to distinguish between models whose to potential stylistic grammars generating structural de- knowledge is provided to them by a human expert (in scriptions of music and we can think of the learning as the style of good, old-fashioned artificial intelligence, traversing the space of possible grammars, specifying GOFAI) and those that acquire knowledge about musi- the parameters distinguishing those grammars along the cal structure by learning (either supervised or unsuper- way. vised) from experience of music, given some predefined The following sections illustrate these points, using structural representation, the parameters of which are different kinds of computational models that have been learned (in the tradition of machine learning), be it proposed for understanding musical syntax. a neural network, a Markov model or grammatical in- ference. The appropriate approach depends on the goal 26.1.2 Early Approaches: Pattern Processing of the modeling. However, in many cases the two ap- proaches are complementary in that successfully learn- We begin with a review of two early approaches that ing complex representations (e.g., context-free rules) is are of historical importance because they laid the extremely challenging but the alternative approach can foundations for symbolic models that subsequently be- result in models that do not generalize far beyond the came influential. One early approach was based on musical domain for which they were developed (e.g., the assumption that listeners use pattern induction pro- Steedman’s [26.5, 6] grammars for blues harmony). In cesses to develop predictions for successive events in the past, tasks that required context-free representa- melodies [26.10, 11]. These models attempt to define tions have usually been hand coded while tasks that formal languages for describing the patterns perceived require simpler representational relationships have been by humans in temporal sequences (such as music) and able to benefit from the flexibility of machine learning. use them to explain how these patterns are applied for However, methods have now been developed in compu- prediction. Simon and Sumner [26.11], for example, be- Part C | 26.1 tational linguistics to learn certain kinds of context-free gin with ordered alphabets for representing the range representation [26.7]. Note that if we are interested in of possible values for a particular musical dimension cognitive representations of music, there is the addi- (e.g., note names, note durations). Simon and Sum- tional issue of the extent to which the representations in ner restrict their attention to the dimensions of melody, question are actually learned or inherited (i. e., innately harmony, rhythm and form and use alphabets for dia- specified) by human beings ([26.8] as, e.g., discussed in tonic notes, triads, duration, stress and formal structure. the poverty of the stimulus debate [26.9]). The operations they consider are same (when the subse- As mentioned above, we must also emphasize the quent symbol is identical to the previous one) and NEXT distinction between finding an optimal structural de- (the subsequent symbol is obtained by taking the next scription of a piece of music and modeling the cognitive symbol in the specified alphabet a specified number of representation of that piece in the mind of a lis- times). Sequences of symbols may then be described tener. The former bounds the latter but listeners are more compactly as a sequence of these operations. likely to be subject to constraints of perception and Deutsch and Feroe [26.10] extended the model of cognition (e.g., limitations of working memory load), Simon and Sumner in several ways, in particular defin- which would prevent them reaching an optimal struc- ing structures as sequences of elementary operators and tural description. Note also that it is problematic to sequence operators such as prime, retrograde, inver- assume the existence of an average listener without sion and alternation. They apply their pattern language understanding all the factors (e.g., musical training, to various alphabets, corresponding to collections of environmental context, degree of attention etc.) that pitches, such as the major and (natural, harmonic and could influence the structural descriptions that listen- melodic) minor scales, major, minor and diminished tri- Musical Syntax II: Empirical Perspectives 26.1 Computational Research 489 ads, and seventh chords. They argue that the pattern how unexpected the next
Recommended publications
  • Beyond Music and Beyond Words: a Psychoanalytic Inquiry
    STUDIJE O MUZIČKOJ UMETNOSTI/STUDIES ON MUSIC Miloš Zatkalik UDC 78.01 Univerzitet umetnosti u Beogradu doi:10.5937/ZbAkUm1801089Z Fakultet muzičke umetnosti Original scientific paper Aleksandar Kontić Visoka škola likovnih i primenjenih umetnosti u Beogradu. Beyond music and beyond words: A psychoanalytic inquiry Abstract. Relationships between language and music have always been lively and dynamic, from their syncretic unity in rituals, to setting text to music, verbal accounts of musical works, musicalization of literature, to abundance of linguistic and literary metaphors in discourse about music. Both language and music unfold in time; both possess a hierarchy of elements that are combined according to a set of rules. This paper will first indicate some linguistic (primarily syntactic) concepts that inform music theory. An analogy can also be established between the Chomskyan concept of the deep structure of language and Schenkerian Ursatz. Music, however, is not always organized along the lines of syntactic hierarchy. Music is also capable of simultaneity in a way inaccessible to language. In order to negotiate this ambiguous relationship between music and language, we invoke the psychoanalytic model of the mind. It postulates that the archaic experience of the world is associated with the auditory sphere. This auditory, pre-verbal mode of conceiving the world will gradually yield to subsequent developmental phases: the visual, and then verbal communication. The deep structure of music is necessarily analogous to verbal structures. However, owing to its pre-verbal origin, music is also organized according to the more archaic modes of mental functioning. For instance: the precepts of logic do not apply; music allows condensation much more readily than language.
    [Show full text]
  • ISSN -2347-856X ISSN -2348-0653 International Journal of Business and Administrati
    Research Paper IJBARR Impact Factor: 3.072 E- ISSN -2347-856X ISSN -2348-0653 MUSIC IS AN ART AND SCIENCE Prof. P.Thenmozhi Associate Professor and Head, Department of Home Science, Seethalakshmi Ramaswami College, Tiruchirappalli,,India. Music is an art form whose medium is sound and silence. Its common elements are pitch (science which governs melody and harmony), rhythm (and its associated concepts tempo, meter, and articulation), dynamics, and the sonic qualities of timbre and texture. The word derives from Greek μουσική (mousike; "art of the Muses").The creation, performance, significance, and even the definition of music vary according to culture and social context. Music ranges from strictly organized compositions (and their recreation in performance), through improvisational music to aleatoric forms. Within the arts, music may be classified as a performing art, a fine art, and auditory art. It may also be divided among art music and folk music. There is also a strong connection between music and mathematics (Talas-counts). Music may be played and heard live, may be part of a dramatic work or film, or may be recorded. Music is a miniature of the harmony of the whole universe, for the harmony of the universe is life itself, and humans, being a miniature of the universe, show harmonious and inharmonious chords in their pulsations, in the beat of their hearts, in their vibration, rhythm and tone. Their health or illness, their joy or discomfort, all show the music or lack of music in their life.Tanjore is to Carnatic Music where Germany is to Western Music. Music is the language of our soul and the soul is the residence of our spirituality.
    [Show full text]
  • The Creation of Consonance: How Musical Context Influences Chord Perception
    The Creation of Consonance: How Musical Context Influences Chord Perception Yuko Arthurs A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Music The University of Sheffield August 2015 Abstract This PhD study investigates how our perception of musical chords, both in isolation and in musical context, is influenced and shaped by our knowledge of the tonal hierarchy and tonal syntax in terms of consonance/dissonance, pleasantness/unpleasantness, stability/instability, and relaxation/tension. Six experiments were conducted to gather behavioural data on the perception of chords from listeners with varying levels of musical training and experience. The first study is principally concerned with the influence of frequency of occurrence on the perception of twelve types of chord in isolation, including both triads and tetrads. It also examines to what extent factors besides frequency of occurrence, namely listener familiarity with the timbre in which chords are played and the acoustic features of chords, predict listener perception. The second and third studies concern the perception of chords in musical context. The second study focuses on musical contexts in which diminished and augmented chords appear, and on the harmonic functions of chords in short sequences of IV-V-I. Using sequences containing an augmented chord, the third study investigates the ways in which a non-diatonic tone can be anchored by its succeeding tone, and considers how the perception of these sequences is influenced by the harmonic function of its succeeding chord. These studies all reveal that the way in which chords and chord sequences are perceived is not completely predetermined by their acoustic, physical dimension.
    [Show full text]
  • Relative Pitch and L2 Lexical Tone Perception/Tone Language Comprehension by Adult Tone
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 9-2017 Is It All Relative? Relative Pitch and L2 Lexical Tone Perception/ Tone Language Comprehension by Adult Tone and Non-Tone Language Speakers Sloane C. von Wertz The Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/2247 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] IS IT ALL RELATIVE? RELATIVE PITCH AND L2 LEXICAL TONE PERCEPTION/TONE LANGUAGE COMPREHENSION BY ADULT TONE AND NON-TONE LANGUAGE SPEAKERS by SLOANE CELESTE VON WERTZ A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, The City University of New York 2017 © 2017 SLOANE CELESTE VON WERTZ All Rights Reserved ii Is It All Relative? Relative Pitch and L2 Lexical Tone Perception/Tone Language Comprehension by Adult Tone and Non-Tone Language Speakers by Sloane Celeste von Wertz This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in satisfaction of the dissertation requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Date Gita Martohardjono Chair of Examining Committee Date Gita Martohardjono Executive Officer Supervisory Committee: Gita Martohardjono Andrew Rosenberg Joseph Straus THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK iii ABSTRACT Is It All Relative? Relative Pitch and L2 Lexical Tone Perception/Tone Language Comprehension by Adult Tone and Non-Tone Language Speakers by Sloane Celeste von Wertz Advisor: Professor Gita Martohardjono Languages generally use musical pitch variation of the voice as part of their sound systems (Maddieson, 2011)—pitch variations that can be somewhat reminiscent of music.
    [Show full text]
  • The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music
    THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Isabelle Peretz Robert J. Zatorre Editors OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page i THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC This page intentionally left blank THE COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE OF MUSIC Edited by ISABELLE PERETZ Départment de Psychologie, Université de Montréal, C.P. 6128, Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, Québec, H3C 3J7, Canada and ROBERT J. ZATORRE Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada 1 Zat-fm.qxd 6/5/03 11:16 PM Page iv 1 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide in Oxford New York Auckland Bangkok Buenos Aires Cape Town Chennai Dar es Salaam Delhi Hong Kong Istanbul Karachi Kolkata Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Mumbai Nairobi São Paulo Shanghai Taipei Tokyo Toronto Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries Published in the United States by Oxford University Press Inc., New York © The New York Academy of Sciences, Chapters 1–7, 9–20, and 22–8, and Oxford University Press, Chapters 8 and 21. Most of the materials in this book originally appeared in The Biological Foundations of Music, published as Volume 930 of the Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, June 2001 (ISBN 1-57331-306-8). This book is an expanded version of the original Annals volume. The moral rights of the author have been asserted Database right Oxford University Press (maker) First published 2003 All rights reserved.
    [Show full text]
  • Musical Syntax Processing in Broca's Aphasia
    Lynn Eekhof 33 Musical syntax processing in Broca’s aphasia A proposal for an information theory-based experiment L.S. (Lynn) Eekhof Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands. Manuscript written during their RMA Linguistics, Utrecht University, Utrecht, Netherlands. Keywords Abstract music The Shared Syntactic Integration Resource Hypothesis (SSIRH; Patel, 2003) syntax argues that resources for syntactic processing are shared by language and music. Broca’s aphasia As previous models of Broca’s aphasia, most notably Avrutin (2006), have claimed entropy that syntactic processing is weakened in Broca’s aphasics, this together predicts information theory that Broca’s aphasics should not only exhibit problems with the processing of language syntax, but also with musical syntax. In this paper, I propose a new approach for testing this claim based on concepts from information theory (Shannon, 1948). The goal of the proposed experiment is to quantify the severity of the musical deficit of Broca’s aphasics with the use of entropy, and find out whether this is correlated with their weakened performance on linguistic syntactic tests. As such, the proposed research could provide a more fine-grained picture of the syntactical deficit of Broca’s aphasics in the domain of music and language, and ultimately shed light on the fundamental debate about the modularity of mind. 1. Introduction Of the many ways human beings can express themselves, two abilities stand out for their striking similarity: language and music. Both are universal cognitive capacities (Patel, 2003); like language, the production (e.g. humming “well-formed”, natural sounding me lodies) and comprehension (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • 25. Musical Syntax I: Theoretical Perspectives
    473 25.Musical Musical Syntax I: Theoretical Synt Perspectives a Martin Rohrmeier, Marcus Pearce 25.1 Outline............................................. 473 The understanding of musical syntax is a topic of fundamental importance for systematic musicol- 25.2 Theories of Musical Syntax................. 474 ogy and lies at the core intersection of music theory 25.2.1The Concept of Musical Syntax ............ 474 and analysis, music psychology, and computa- 25.2.2Foundations of Musical Syntax............ 475 tional modeling. This chapter discusses the notion 25.3 Models of Musical Syntax................... 477 of musical syntax and its potential foundations 25.3.1Building Blocks ................................. 477 based on notions such as sequence grammat- 25.3.2Structure Building ............................. 478 icality, expressive unboundedness, generative capacity, sequence compression and stability. Sub- 25.4 Syntactic Models of Diferent Complexity ..................... 478 sequently, it discusses problems concerning the 25.4.1Finite-Context Models........................ 478 choice of musical building blocks to be modeled 25.4.2Finite-State Models ........................... 479 as well as the underlying principles of sequential 25.4.3Context-Free or Equivalent Models...... 479 structure building. The remainder of the chapter 25.4.4Beyond Context-Free Complexity ........ 481 reviews the main theoretical proposals that can be characterized under diferent mechanisms of 25.5 Discussion ........................................ 482 structure building, in particular approaches using 25.A Appendix: The Chomsky Hierarchy ..... 483 fnite-context or fnite-state models as well as 25.A.1Type 3(Regular)................................. 483 tree-based models of context-free complexity (in- 25.A.2Type 2(Context Free) .......................... 483 Part C | cluding the Generative Theory of Tonal Music) and 25.A.3Type 1(Context Sensitive).................... 483 beyond.
    [Show full text]
  • Recent Publications in Music 2012
    1 of 149 RECENT PUBLICATIONS IN MUSIC 2012 Compiled and edited by Geraldine E. Ostrove and David Sommerfield This list contains citations to literature about music in print and other media, emphasizing reference materials and works of research interest that appeared in 2011. Reporters who contribute regularly provide citations mainly or only from the year preceding the year this list is published in conjuction with Fontes artis musicae. However, reporters may also submit retrospective lists cumulating publications from up to the previous five years. In the hope that geographic coverage of this list can be expanded, the compilers welcome inquiries from bibliographers in countries not presently represented. CONTRIBUTORS. Argentina: Estela Escalada Japan: SEKINE Toshiko Australia: Julia Mitford Kenya: Santie De Jongh Austria: Thomas Leibnitz Malawi: Santie De Jongh Belgium: Johan Eeckeloo Mexico: Daniel Villanueva Rivas, María Del China: Katie Lai Consuelo García Martínez Croatia: Žeiljka Radovinović The Netherlands: Joost van Gemert Denmark: Anne Ørbæk Jensen New Zealand: Marilyn Portman Estonia: Katre Rissalu Nigeria: Santie De Jongh Finland: Tuomas Tyyri Russia: Lyudmila Dedyukina France: Élisabeth Missaoui Serbia: Radmila Milinković Germany: Susanne Hein South Africa: Santie De Jongh Ghana: Santie De Jongh Spain: José Ignacio Cano, Maria José Greece: Alexandros Charkiolakis González Ribot Greenland: Anne Ørbæk Jensen Taiwan: Katie Lai Hong Kong: Katie Lai Turkey: Senem Acar, Paul Alister Whitehead Hungary: SZEPESI Zsuzsanna Uganda: Santie De Jongh Iceland: Bryndis Vilbergsdóttir United Kingdom: Rupert Ridgewell Ireland: Roy Stanley United States: Karen Little, Lindsay Hansen Italy: Federica Biancheri Uruguay: Estela Escalada With thanks for assistance with translations and transcriptions to Kersti Blumenthal, Ana Cristán, Paul Frank, Irina Kirchik, Everette Larson, Miroslava Nezar, Joan Weeks, and Thompson A.
    [Show full text]
  • Contagious Heterophony: a New Theory About the Origins of Music
    Musicæ Scientiæ/XI-1/RR 16/03/07 13:56 Page 3 Musicae Scientiae © 2007 by ESCOM European Society Spring 2007, Vol XI, n° 1, 3-26 for the Cognitive Sciences of Music Contagious heterophony: A new theory about the origins of music STEVEN BROWN Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser University •ABSTRACT Two of the most salient features of music are the blending of pitch and the match- ing of time. I propose here a possible evolutionary precursor of human music based on a process I call “contagious heterophony”. Heterophony is a form of pitch blending in which individuals generate similar musical lines but in which these lines are poorly synchronized. A wonderful example can be found in the howling of wolves. Each wolf makes a similar call but the resultant chorus is poorly blended in time. The other major feature of the current hypothesis is contagion. Once one ani- mal starts calling, other members of the group join in through a spreading process. While this type of heterophonic calling is well-represented in nature, synchronized polyphony is not. In this article, I discuss evolutionary scenarios by which the human capacity to integrate musical parts in pitch-space and in time may have emerged in music. In doing so, I make mention of neuroimaging findings that shed light on the neural mechanisms of vocal imitation and metric entrainment in humans, two key processes underlying musical integration. While discussions of the origins of music were commonplace during the 18th and 19th centuries (Condillac, 1746; Rousseau, 1781; Spencer, 1857, 1890; Darwin, 1871, 1872), the topic seemed to fall out of favour during the 20th century.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution 29
    03.MUSIC.23_029-060.qxd 03/10/2005 15:20 Page 29 The Origins of Music: Innateness, Uniqueness, and Evolution 29 THE ORIGINS OF MUSIC: INNATENESS, UNIQUENESS, AND EVOLUTION JOSH MCDERMOTT past and present. It is incorporated into a vast array of Perceptual Science Group, Department of Brain and cultural events, including weddings and funerals, reli- Cognitive Sciences, MIT gious services, dances, and sporting events, as well as solitary listening sessions. It can make people feel happy MARC HAUSER or sad, so much so that music is central to modern Department of Psychology and Program in advertising campaigns. And people throughout the Neurosciences, Harvard world spend billions of dollars annually on the music and clubbing industries. Despite this central role in THE ORIGINS and adaptive significance of music, long an human culture, the origins and adaptive function of elusive target, are now active topics of empirical study, music remain virtually a complete mystery. Music with many interesting developments over the past few stands in sharp contrast to most other enjoyable human years. This article reviews research in anthropology, eth- behaviors (eating, sleeping, talking, sex) in that it yields nomusicology, developmental and comparative psychol- no obvious benefits to those who partake of it. The evo- ogy, neuropsychology, and neurophysiology that bears lutionary origins of music have thus puzzled scientists on questions concerning the origins and evolution of and philosophers alike since the time of Darwin (1871). music. We focus on the hypothesis that music perception Theories about the evolution of music abound. Many is constrained by innate, possibly human- and music- have suggested that music might be a biological adapta- specific principles of organization, as these are candi- tion, with functions ranging from courtship to social dates for evolutionary explanations.
    [Show full text]
  • Syntax in Music and Language: the Role of Cognitive Control
    Syntax in music and language: The role of cognitive control L. Robert Slevc ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, University of Maryland College Park, MD 20742 USA Jason G. Reitman ([email protected]) Wesleyan University Middletown, CT 06459 USA Brooke M. Okada ([email protected]) Department of Psychology, University of Maryland College Park, MD 20742 USA Abstract electrophysiological response characteristic of a violation of linguistic syntax (the left anterior negativity, or LAN) is The relationship between structural (or syntactic) processing in music and in language is not yet clear. Evidence indicating reduced when linguistic syntactic violations are paired with that these two processes are shared conflicts with other results a concurrent music-syntactic irregularity (Koelsch, Gunter, suggesting that they are largely distinct. These conflicting Wittfoth, & Sammler, 2005). Similarly, sung complex findings suggest that musical and linguistic processing may sentences are especially difficult to understand when critical share some, but not all, underlying processes, raising the regions are sung out-of-key (Fedorenko, Patel, Casasanto, question of what exactly those shared processes might be. Winawer, & Gibson, 2009). Two experiments tested the idea that one shared process is cognitive control by pairing manipulations of musical Other behavioral evidence comes from on-line sentence structure with the Stroop task, a standard test of cognitive processing paradigms, where readers’ slowed processing of control. Manipulations of harmonic expectancy, but not of temporary syntactic ambiguities is especially pronounced timbral expectancy, interacted with Stroop interference when the disambiguating word is paired with a harmonically effects, suggesting that cognitive control is at least one unexpected chord (Slevc, Rosenberg, & Patel, 2009; also specific process underlying shared syntactic processing in see Hoch, Poulin-Charronnat, & Tillmann, 2011, for related music and language.
    [Show full text]
  • Popular Music Analysis and Musicology: Bridging the Gap
    Popular Music (1993) Volume 12/2. Copyright © 1993 Cambridge University Press Popular music analysis and musicology: bridging the gap RICHARD MIDDLETON Since their beginnings, popular music studies have conducted an implicit (some- times explicit) dialogue with musicology. To be sure, the musicological side of this conversation has more often than not been marked by insult, incomprehension or silence; and popular music scholars for their part have tended to concentrate on musicology's deficiencies. But musicology is changing (more about this later); at the same time, recent work on popular music suggests a new confidence, manifest- ing itself in part in a willingness to engage with and adapt mainstream methods.11 believe each needs the other. Within the sphere of analysis, the main problem felt to attach to mainstream methods has been the tendency to formalism. In contrast, popular music analysis has insisted (rightly, I think) on the priority of meaning. Much of the best work has been semiotic or interpretative (Laing, Tagg, Bradby, Grossberg) or has pursued theories of social and cultural homology (Hebdige, Shepherd). However imposing this body of work, though, there is a suspicion that sometimes insufficient atten- tion has been paid to the sounds themselves - to the intra-musical structures of what I call the 'primary' level of signification (Middleton 1990, p. 220). Somehow, we need to find ways of bringing the patterns created in the sounds themselves back into the foreground, without as a consequence retreating into an inappropri- ate formalism. And if we can do this, we may well find that we are contributing to an advance in general musical analysis.
    [Show full text]