Josiah Royce on Self and Community

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Josiah Royce on Self and Community JOSIAH ROYCE ON SELF AND COMMUNITY by Jacquelyn Ann K. Kegley Josiah Royce's life-long concern was to explicate adequately the no- tions of "self" and "community" and their fundamental interdepen- dence.' In doing so, he strove to avoid any form of atomistic, reduction- istic individualism, on the one hand, and flagrant collectivism, on the other hand. Instead, he argued that the searches for self, for truth, for an ethical, political, or religious ideal must necessarily be both individual and communal enterprises. This argument leads Royce to a number of important philosophical insights too long neglected in both philosophical and scientific circles, but sorely needed after years of domination by Car- tesian, Humean, or Existential atomism. Too long has human experience been characterized as a passive, atomistic, and highly individualistic affair; and privacy, guilt, and responsibility have been either ignored or recklessly e~alted.~Royce's views on self and community, it will be seen, take us beyond all worn-out dichotomies of mind-body, spiritualism- materialism, freedom-determinism, egoism-altruism, to a refreshing new holistic view. ROYCE'S THEORY OF SELF Four important questions about the self are dealt with and answered by Royce, namely: (1) What is a self? (2) How is a self developed? (3) How is the self known? and (4) What gives a self its unique identity? The answers briefly are: (1) The self is a process, not a thing, a process having both a public, physical, and behavorial aspect and a private, inner aspect; (2) the self is developed out of a process of social interaction vis- i-vis a contrast effect; (3) the self is never known directly, but only Jacquelyn Kegley is Associate Professor of Philosophy at California State College, Bakersfield. 34 RICE UNIVERSITY STUDIES through a communal process of interpretation; and (4) what gives the self its unique identity is an act of will, indeed, an act of love. The self, for Royce, can never be categorized as a thing, but must always be seen as a process, as developing serially in time. He categor- ically rejects any Cartesian notion of the self as an independent substance only temporarily embodied and involved with the realm of nature. "Whatever the self is, it is not a Thing. It is not, in AristotIe's sense, . a Substance."' Further, one cannot speak of self as independent of and separate from either nature or body. Royce affirms both the role of genetic and physiological elements in the development of the self and the affinity of human conscious life with nature. In the second volume of The World and the Individual, Royce dis- cusses four characteristics common to both unconscious and conscious nature, namely, tendencies to (I) irreversibility, (2) communication, (3) formation of habits, and (4) evolutionary gr0wth.j A11 of these, we shall see, are important to Royce's understanding of the self.s Further, Royce puts forth three hypotheses about nature: (I) The vast contrast which we have been taught to make between mater~aland con- scious processes really depends merely upon the accidents of the human point of view. ." (2) . we have no r~ghtwhatever to speak of really unconscious Nature, but only of ilncomrn~~nicativeNature . .' (3) . in the case of Nature in general, as In the case of the particular forms of Nature known as our fellowmen, we are dealing with phenomenal signs of a vast contcious process, whose relat~onto Time vanes vastly, but whose general character- i.jtlcs are throughout the same.8 Whatever difficulties there may be with such a pan-psychic view of reality, it has some merits in light of recent developments in both psy- chology and philosophy. Unlike Descartes, who saw all the animal king- dom in terms of machines, Royce allows that a study of other conscious organisms may lead to valuable insights about the human self. His view is more consonant with new interest in animal language, intelligence, and rights. It also cautions us against the substantialization we seem to engage in when dealing with mind, brain, or body.9 Royce emphasizes not only the continuity of the human self with nature, but also our common-sense experience of an empirical self. "The concept of the human self, like the concept of Nature, comes to us, first, as an empirical concept, founded upon a certain class of experience^."'^ The empirical self is constituted by both public and private experiences. A self is a certain totality of facts. Among such facts are the predomi- JOSIAH ROYCE ON SELF AND COMMUNITY 3 5 nantly corporeal ones, such as countenance, body, clothing, and physical actions-facts that both the self and others may observe and comment on." Royce even acknowledges that if these facts radically changed, so would the self.12 He also recognizes bodily continuity and the sense of a body as a criterion of self identity, though not the ultimate criterion.'' In addition to these public facts there is for Royce a set of inner, private facts of equal empirical status and importance for the self. In addition to the external or corporeal Self of the phenomenal world, there is the equally empirical and phenomenal Self of the inner life, the series of states of con- sciousness, the feelings, thoughts, desires, memories, emotions, moods. These again, both my neighbor and myself regard as belong~ngto me, and as golng to make up what I am.'" Further, as I have pointed out elsewhere,I5 unlike some behaviorists, Royce not only acknowledged consciousness and states of consciousness as important aspects of the self, but he also, before Husserl and other phenomenologists, stressed the intentionality of consciousness. According to Royce, mental acts are always directed toward objects, and also intend the object as having such-and-such meaning, as viewed in such-and-such a way.I6 Early in his career, Royce discussed the role of selective at- tention in all acts of consciousness. Attention selects only a few from the numerous impressions impinging on our sensibilities; many impressions slip through our consciousness without being retained or having any effect. Attention even modifies the quality of our impressions. Royce concludes his discussion of attention by writing: Attention seems to defeat, in part, its own object. Bringing something into the field of knowledgeseems to be a modifying, if not a transforming process." The intentional nature of consciousness reveals that the self engages in meaning-seeking, meaning-conferring activity. It further suggests that knowledge is obtained by a necessarily communal process. If I see the world from particular points of view, the only way I can transcend my narrowness and subjectivity is by checking things out with my fellows. Objectivity is intersubjectivity. Indeed, for Royce, knowledge both of self and of the external world is grounded in social interaction. Throughout his philosophical career, Royce argued that the self could never be considered a datum, but rather that self-consciousness arises out of a social contrast be- tween the self and the not-self, between what is mine and what is not mine. Royce writes: I affirm that our empirical self-consc~ousness,from moment to moment, depends upon a series of contrast effects, whose psychological origin lies in our l~teralsocial life, and 36 RICE UNIVERSITY STUDIES whose continuance in our present conscious Ilfe, whenever we are atone, IS due to hab~t, to our memory of literal social relations, and to an Imaginative idealization of these relation^.'^ Royce describes the process of coming to self-consciousness as follows: Nobody amongst us men comes to self-consc~ousness,so far as I know, except under the persistent influence of his social fellows. A child in the earlier stages of his social development . shows you, as you observe him, a process of development of self-con- sc~ousnessin whlch, at every stage, the Self of the child grows and forms itself through Imitation, and through functions that cluster about the Imitation of others, and that are secondary thereto. And his self-consciousness, as it grows, feeds upon soclal models, so that at every stage of his awakening l~fehis consciousness of the Alter is a step in the advance of his consciousness. His playmates, h~snurse, or mother, or the workmen whose occupations he sees, and whose power fascinates him, appeal to his ~m~tativenessand set him copies for his activities. He learns his little arts, and as he does so, he contrasts h~sown deeds with those of his models, and or other children. Now contrast IS, m otrr conscious life, tile tnother of clearness. What the chrld does ~nstrnc- trvely, and wrtho~rtcotnparison wrth the deeds of others, t7ia.v never cotne to his clear consciorrsness as his own deeds at all. What he learns imitatively, and then reproduces, perhaps in joyous obstinacy, as an act that enables him to display himyelf over against others-thls constitutes the beginning of his self-consc~ouslife." Royce does not need to struggle, as do Descartes and Husserl, to ex- plain how we know other minds. We are first with other selves and only later become differentiated, unique selves. Of course, I will see others as conscious actors not only because by observing their acts I become a con- scious actor, but also because they aid me in developing my self in new directions. Our fellows are known to be real and have their own inner life, because they are for each of us the endless treasury of more rdeas. They answer our questions; they tell us news; they make comments, they pass judgments; they express novel cornb~nationsof feelings; they relate to us stories, they argue with us, and take counsel with us.
Recommended publications
  • Bibliotheca Sacra
    Critical Noles. [Jan. ARTICLE X. CRITICAL NOTES. ROYCE'S "CONCEPTION OF GOD.") THIS work is a report of a discussion held before the Philosophical Union of the University of California in 1895. It coittains the leading address of the symposium by Professor Royce; remarks, critical and con­ structive, by the other participants in the discussion; and finally a sup­ plemental essay by Professor Royce in which he develops more thor­ oughly his central doctrine, and replies to his critics. It is apparent, from Professor Royce's introductory remarks, that the Philosophical Union of the University has beeu studying his work, "The Religious Aspect of Philosophy." .. Were there time, I should be glad indeed if I were able to throw any light 011 that little book. But my time is short. The great problems of philosophy are pressing. It is the death of your philosophizing if you come to believe anything merely be­ cause you have once maintained it. Let us lay aside, then, both text and tradition, and come face to face with ollr philosophical problem it­ self" (pp. 5, 6). This seems to breathe a spirit of admirable candor; but if, as we shall find later, the conclusions of Royce's present discussion are utterly irreconcilable with his results in .. The Religious Aspect," it would seem more candid, and also a saving of time, at least for the stu­ dents of the Philosophical Union, to recognize and deal thoroughly with this fact at the outset. In seeking a philosophical conception of God, Royce begins with the idea of an Omniscient Being.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hiring of James Mark Baldwin and James Gibson Hume at Toronto in 1889
    History of Psychology Copyright 2004 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2004, Vol. 7, No. 2, 130–153 1093-4510/04/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1093-4510.7.2.130 THE HIRING OF JAMES MARK BALDWIN AND JAMES GIBSON HUME AT THE UNIVERSITY OF TORONTO IN 1889 Christopher D. Green York University In 1889, George Paxton Young, the University of Toronto’s philosophy professor, passed away suddenly while in the midst of a public debate over the merits of hiring Canadians in preference to American and British applicants for faculty positions. As a result, the process of replacing Young turned into a continuation of that argument, becoming quite vociferous and involving the popular press and the Ontario gov- ernment. This article examines the intellectual, political, and personal dynamics at work in the battle over Young’s replacement and its eventual resolution. The outcome would have an impact on both the Canadian intellectual scene and the development of experimental psychology in North America. In 1889 the University of Toronto was looking to hire a new professor of philosophy. The normally straightforward process of making a university appoint- ment, however, rapidly descended into an unseemly public battle involving not just university administrators, but also the highest levels of the Ontario govern- ment, the popular press, and the population of the city at large. The debate was not pitched solely, or even primarily, at the level of intellectual issues, but became intertwined with contentious popular questions of nationalism, religion, and the proper place of science in public education. The impact of the choice ultimately made would reverberate not only through the university and through Canada’s broader educational establishment for decades to come but, because it involved James Mark Baldwin—a man in the process of becoming one of the most prominent figures in the study of the mind—it also rippled through the nascent discipline of experimental psychology, just then gathering steam in the United States of America.
    [Show full text]
  • Gender and Modernity in Transnational Perspective: Hugo Munsterberg and the American Woman
    Gender and Modernity in Transnational Perspective: Hugo Munsterberg and the American Woman RENA SANDERSON INTRODUCTION TN ONE OF THE FIRST and best-known collections of cultural criticism in .LAmerica, Civilization in the United States (1922), Harold Stearns begins his chapter on "The Intellectual Life" with this widely quoted passage: When Professor Einstein roused the ire of the women's clubs by stating that "women dominate the entire life of Amer- ica," and that "there are cities with a million population, but cities suffering from terrible poverty - the poverty of intellec- tual things," he was but repeating a criticism of our life now old enough to be almost a clichi. Hardly any intelligent foreigner has failed to observe and comment upon the extraordinary femi- nization of American social life, and oftenest he has coupled this observation with a few biting remarks concerning the intel- lectual anaemia or torpor that seems to accompany it. Stearns goes on to argue that, in the case of America's feminization, "the spontaneous judgment of the perceptive foreigner is to a remarkable de- gree correct."1 By invoking the foreigner's perspective, Stearns was try- ing to impart a certain freshness to what would otherwise have been old news. Although Stearns was speaking of Einstein, his description of the "perceptive foreigner" applies even more fittingly to another German- Jewish intellectual, Hugo Munsterberg (1863-1916).2 Starting some two decades prior to the publication of Civilization in the United States, Mun- sterberg surveyed many of the same phenomena treated in that anthol- ogy and anticipated many of its criticisms of American culture.
    [Show full text]
  • Victor Lenzen Papers, [Ca
    http://oac.cdlib.org/findaid/ark:/13030/tf0580018d No online items Guide to the Victor Lenzen Papers, [ca. 1904-1975] Processed by The Bancroft Library staff The Bancroft Library. University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California, 94720-6000 Phone: (510) 642-6481 Fax: (510) 642-7589 Email: [email protected] URL: http://bancroft.berkeley.edu © 1997 The Regents of the University of California. All rights reserved. Note History --History, University of California --History, UC SystemHistory --History, University of California --History, UC BerkeleyGeographical (By Place) --University of California --University of California BerkeleySocial Sciences --Anthropology --ArchaeologyPhysical Sciences --PhysicsSocial Sciences --Education Guide to the Victor Lenzen BANC MSS 76/206 c 1 Papers, [ca. 1904-1975] Guide to the Victor Lenzen Papers, [ca. 1904-1975] Collection number: BANC MSS 76/206 c The Bancroft Library University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California Contact Information: The Bancroft Library. University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California, 94720-6000 Phone: (510) 642-6481 Fax: (510) 642-7589 Email: [email protected] URL: http://bancroft.berkeley.edu Processed by: The Bancroft Library staff Date Completed: ca. 1976 Encoded by: Xiuzhi Zhou © 1997 The Regents of the University of California. All rights reserved. Collection Summary Collection Title: Victor Lenzen papers, Date (inclusive): [ca. 1904-1975] Collection Number: BANC MSS 76/206 c Creator: Lenzen, Victor Fritz, 1890-1975 Extent: Number of containers: 5 cartons, 7 boxes and 1 oversize folder Repository: The Bancroft Library. Berkeley, California 94720-6000 Physical Location: For current information on the location of these materials, please consult the Library's online catalog. Abstract: Letters written to Lenzen and copies of letters by him; Mss.
    [Show full text]
  • “Nervous Diseases” and the Politics of Healing: William James, Josiah Royce, and the Early Dynamic Psychiatry Movement in America
    Article . “Nervous Diseases” and the Politics of Healing: William James, Josiah Royce, and the Early Dynamic Psychiatry Movement in America Matthew D. Bessette SUNY-Brockport Abstract A prominent feature of the postbellum, industrialized landscape in America was a preoccupation with a protean illness that sapped the vitality of otherwise healthily- constituted people. Termed neurasthenia, and otherwise known as the disease of civilization, the sociomedical discourse that formed around it became the cultural idiom through which a variety of elites, nonspecialists, and medical professionals negotiated their way through a changing social order. Yet social and cultural conflict was also ingrained in this discourse and the optimistic healing narrative that accompanied it. This becomes evident by analyzing the critical writings of William James and Josiah Royce, two Harvard philosophers, public intellectuals, and well- respected psychologists within the “Boston School” of psychology, against the influential articles and treatises that the incipient dynamic psychiatry movement within American medicine generated from 1909 through the Great War. Thus beyond exploring the ways in which the desire for cultural and personal renewal was held in common, the task left to historical inquiry is to analyze the reasons why it diverged between those who saw a fresh need to resuscitate the traditional republican virtues of discipline, self-reliance, and civic responsibility by allowing strenuous, ethical ideals to flourish in everyday life, and those who envisioned the enlightened direction and intervention of scientific physicians ushering in a new age of psychosomatic health and societal progress. A prominent feature of the postbellum, industrialized landscape in America was a preoccupation with a protean illness that sapped the vitality of otherwise healthily-constituted people.
    [Show full text]
  • Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(S): J
    Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations Author(s): J. G. Morawski Source: Isis, Vol. 77, No. 2 (Jun., 1986), pp. 219-242 Published by: University of Chicago Press on behalf of History of Science Society Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/232650 Accessed: 22-12-2015 00:42 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. History of Science Society and University of Chicago Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Isis. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 129.133.6.95 on Tue, 22 Dec 2015 00:42:52 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Organizing Knowledge and Behavior at Yale's Institute of Human Relations By J. G. Morawski* IN 1929 JAMES ANGELL, president of Yale, announced plans for a unique teaching and research center for those fields "directly concerned with the problems of man's individual and group conduct. The purpose is to correlate knowledge and coordinate technique in related fields that greater progress may be made in the understanding of human life. The time has certainly come once again to attempt a fruitful synthesis of knowledge." The New York Times described the experiment as dismantling the disciplinary "Great Wall of China" and compared it with the Renaissance transformation of knowledge.1 The Insti- tute of Human Relations (IHR), as the center was named, received over $4.5 million from the Rockefeller Foundation for its first decade of operation.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychologists and Physicians in the Borderlands of Science, 1900-1942
    PSYCHOLOGISTS AND PHYSICIANS IN THE BORDERLANDS OF SCIENCE, 1900-1942 By WADE EDWARD PICKREN A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1995 For my mother: WILLIE MERLE PICKREN, and in memoriam, BILL PICKREN, You taught me to love and work. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I wish to express my deepest gratitude to the chairman of my dissertation committee, Donald A. Dewsbury. Dr. Dewsbury has, from the beginning of this long project, been a model of encouragement, kindness, and unfailing generosity. He has shared his time, his great breadth of learning, his editorial ability, and his materials with me. My understanding of the history of psychology has been greatly deepened by our conversations. I also wish to acknowledge that Dr. Dewsbury has helped me to understand that data is a plural! Dr. Wilse B. Webb has also stimulated much thought in me about what I was doing and where I was going with my ideas. Although I did not avail myself of his wisdom as oft as I would have liked, his voice and his sharp eye were always with me. I hope that, in the future, time will allow me a greater opportunity to benefit from his great knowledge and experience. Both near at hand and from afar, Dr. Toby Appel has blessed me with the keenness of her insight . Her acceptance and friendly corrections of my halting efforts to write history have been much appreciated. One of my most pleasant memories of this experience is that of sitting at a table at iii Cafe Gardens talking about the history of biology or psychology, while hoping to hear some Van Morrison on the house music system.
    [Show full text]
  • The Misogyny of Psychology: a Tribute to Women Often Overlooked
    Bowling Green State University ScholarWorks@BGSU Honors Projects Honors College Spring 5-8-2020 The Misogyny of Psychology: A Tribute to Women Often Overlooked Gabrielle Miller [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.bgsu.edu/honorsprojects Part of the Feminist, Gender, and Sexuality Studies Commons, and the Psychology Commons Repository Citation Miller, Gabrielle, "The Misogyny of Psychology: A Tribute to Women Often Overlooked" (2020). Honors Projects. 519. https://scholarworks.bgsu.edu/honorsprojects/519 This work is brought to you for free and open access by the Honors College at ScholarWorks@BGSU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Projects by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@BGSU. Table of Contents Preface 3 Mary Whiton Calkins 4 Melanie Klein 5 Karen Horney 6 Leta Stetter Hollingworth 7 Inez Prosser 8 Anna Freud 9 Mary Ainsworth 10 Bernice Neugarten 11 Mamie Phipps Clark 12 Janet Taylor Spence 13 Florence Denmark 14 Bonnie Strickland 15 Afterword 16 References 18 2 | P a g e Preface The basis for this project stemmed from my passion for spreading positive energy. Additionally, my altruistic values provide me with a strong sense of duty to do the right thing at the same time that I believe it is my responsibility to help others, especially because I have been afforded the luxury of a college education although many others do not share this privilege. Under those circumstances, I wanted to speak on the issue of inequal representation of diverse identities, with special attention to the branches of science which historically refused to give due credit to individuals other than straight, white men.
    [Show full text]
  • Routing the Roots and Growth of the Dramatic Instinct
    Routing the Roots and Growth of the Dramatic Instinct Jeanne Klein Associate Professor Department of Theatre 1530 Naismith Dr., 317 Murphy University of Kansas Lawrence, KS 66045 (FAX) (785) 864-5251 (o) (785) 864-5576 (h) (785) 843-3744 [email protected] Unpublished paper. Abstract: The idea of a “dramatic instinct” is routed from its nineteenth-century roots in early childhood education and child study psychology through early twentieth-century theatre education. This historically contextualized routing suggests the functional purposes of pretense for human freedom, self-preservation, and survival. Theatre scholars may influence the discipline of cognitive psychology by employing these philosophical and epistemological theories to unpack the role of empathy in aesthetic experiences with today’s spectators. Jeanne Klein is an associate professor of theatre at the University of Kansas where she teaches theatre for young audiences, child drama, and children’s media psychology. Her articles have appeared in the Journal of Dramatic Theatre and Criticism, Youth Theatre Journal, Journal of Aesthetic Education, and TYA Today, among others. This paper is dedicated to the memory of Robert Findlay whose text, Century of Innovation, co- authored with Oscar Brockett, taught me to innovate by taking scholarly and artistic risks. Routing the Roots and Growth of the Dramatic Instinct The term “dramatic instinct” refers simply to the human drive to dramatize. Dramatic (or pretend) play appears spontaneously in early childhood with few cultural variations as a provocative mystery of the human mind. For what functional purposes does this instinct for drama possibly serve humankind? After decades of innumerable investigations, developmental psychologists have concluded that this instinct for pretense is innate, universal, and cross- cultural, regardless of parental modeling (Lillard 188).
    [Show full text]
  • Appointed Consisting of W. H. Dall, Chairman, A. L. Day, and F. W. Clarke;
    744 REPORT OF THE AUTUMN MEETING GRANT FROM THE J. LAWRENCE SMITH FUND No. 7, GEORGE PEKINS MERRILL, U. S. National Museum, $500. To further aid his studies of rare meteorites. GRANT FROM THE MARSH FUND JOHN M. CLARK, State Hall, Albany, $400. For a study of mutualism, symbiosis, and dependent life among animals of geologic time. REPORT OF THE AUTUMN MEETING Prepared by the Home Secretary The Autumn Meeting of the Academy was held in the buildings of the Massachusetts Institute of Technology at Cambridge, on November 13, 14, 15, 1916. Seventy members were present as follows: Messrs. C. G. Abbot, H. L. Abbot, Barus, Baxter, Becker, Boas, Bogert, Boltwood, Bumstead, Cannon, Castle, Cattell, Chittenden, W. B. Clark, J. M. Clarke, Comstock, Conklin, Crafts, Crew, Cross, Davenport, Davis, Day, Farlow, Fewkes, Folin, Goodale, Hale, E. H. Hall, Harper, Harrison, Hayford, Holmes, Howard, Howell, Iddings, Jackson, Leuschner, Lewis, Lindgren, Lusk, Mall, Mark, Mayer, Meltzer, Mendel, Michelson, Morley, E. S. Morse, A. A. Noyes, H. F. Osborn, T. B. Osborne, Parker, Pearl, Pickering, Prudden, Reid, Richards, Rosa, Scott, Story, Thomson, Van Hise, Walcott, Webster, Welch, Wheeler, D. White, Wilson, R. W. Wood. BUSINESS SESSIONS The Home Secretary announced the following deaths since the last Annual Meeting of the Academy: Cleveland Abbe, elected 1879, died October 28, 1916; Josiah Royce, elected 1906, died September 14, 1916; also Sir William Ramsay, Foreign Associate, elected 1904, died July 24, 1916. The Home Secretary announced that the following publications had been issued since the Annual Meeting: PROCEEDINGS, vol. 2, numbers 5-11;Memoirs, volume 14, first memoir, by George P.
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young
    A Teaching Guide for History of Psychology www.feministvoices.com 1 Psychology’s Feminist Voices in the Classroom A Teaching Guide for the History of Psychology Instructor Compiled by Jennifer Bazar, Elissa Rodkey, and Jacy Young One of the goals of Psychology’s Feminist Voices is to serve as a teaching resource. To facilitate the process of incorporating PFV into the History of Psychology course, we have created this document. Below you will find two primary sections: (1) Lectures and (2) Assignments. (1) Lectures: In this section you will find subject headings of topics often covered in History of Psychology courses. Under each heading, we have provided an example of the relevant career, research, and/or life experiences of a woman featured on the Psychology’s Feminist Voices site that would augment a lecture on that particular topic. Below this description is a list of additional women whose histories would be well-suited to lectures on that topic. (2) Assignments: In this section you will find several suggestions for assignments that draw on the material and content available on Psychology’s Feminist Voices that you can use in your History of Psychology course. The material in this guide is intended only as a suggestion and should not be read as a “complete” list of all the ways Psychology’s Feminist Voices could be used in your courses. We would love to hear all of the different ideas you think of for how to include the site in your classroom - please share you thoughts with us by emailing Alexandra Rutherford, our project coordinator,
    [Show full text]
  • The Papers of Josiah Royce – Harvard Library Archives Call Number: HUG 1755
    The Papers of Josiah Royce – Harvard Library Archives Call Number: HUG 1755 The listing of Box, Folder, and Document information begins on page 5 below. The Papers of Josiah Royce are primarily the product of Royce’s philosophical thought, research, writings, and teachings from his arrival at Harvard University in 1882 to his death in 1916. The collection, which consists of eight series, provides a comprehensive view of Royce's philosophical thought as a student and professor, his research and correspondence regarding the history of California, and his relationship with his colleague William James. The bulk of the collection spans from 1878 to 1916 and covers Royce’s teaching appointments at the University of California at Berkeley and Harvard. The collection contains manuscripts of many, but not all, of Royce's publications, teaching lectures, research notebooks, and drafts, outlines, and professional correspondence. A small amount of Royce’s personal papers, including autobiographical sketches, records of his undergraduate studies at the University of California at Berkeley, and family correspondence are also included. The collection contains photographs of Royce family members, dating from the mid 1860s through early 1900s. The collection also contains supplemental texts by Royce scholars and several Royce family genealogies. Biographical note Josiah Royce (1855-1916) was a member of the “golden age” of American philosophy and a foremost interpreter of absolute idealism early in his career. His philosophical thought later evolved into an examination and embrace of pragmatism, loyalty, and community. He investigated the mathematical and logical aspects of philosophy, providing a rigorous basis for his metaphysical inquiry.
    [Show full text]