Do We Really Teach Abstraction?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Do We Really Teach Abstraction? Do We Really Teach Abstraction? Paolo Bucci, Timothy J. Long, and Bruce W. Weide Computer and Information Science The Ohio State University Columbus, OH 43210 {bucci,long,weide}@cis.ohio-state.edu Abstract According to Norman, when done properly, abstraction Abstraction is one of the cornerstones of software develop- introduces the possibility for completely new ways of think- ment and is recognized as a fundamental and essential prin- ing about the system being abstracted. The new representa- ciple to be taught as early as CS1/CS2. Abstraction tion enhances our ability to think about the external supposedly can enhance students’ ability to reason and behavior of the original system through the use of terms that think. Yet we often hear complaints about the inability of are fundamentally different from those in which its internal CS undergraduates to do that. Do we supply students with details would be explained. It is critical to note that the ben- the tools they need to reach their potential to think carefully eficiaries of both information hiding and abstraction are and to reason rigorously about software behavior? Typically human beings; computers do not care at all about either. we do not, but as educators there are techniques we can use It is the central claim of this paper that, despite broad agree- to help our students develop such skills starting in CS1/CS2. ment about their fundamental importance, both information hiding and abstraction are severely shortchanged by current 1 Introduction CS1/CS2 pedagogy. More specifically, we claim that stan- Abstraction and information hiding have long been recog- dard pedagogical practices tend to compromise the human nized as fundamental and essential principles in software dimension of information hiding and fall well short of help- development, and are usually given positions of prominence ing student learners to realize an increased ability to reason in computer science curricula as early as CS1/CS2 [1, 2, 5]. and think carefully and rigorously as a benefit of using Information hiding and abstraction can be thought of as abstraction. complementary aspects of one general idea: hide the details Our claim is perhaps surprising and certainly requires justi- of a complex system while providing a simpler “cover fication. After all, when we teach objects using, for exam- story” to explain those aspects of the system that are of ple, abstract classes in C++ or interfaces in Java together interest. In this context then, we view abstraction as present- with concrete classes, are we not teaching information hid- ing a cover story for hidden detail. ing and abstraction? Clearly these programming-language Why are these ideas so important? As Don Norman [4] mechanisms provide information hiding in a technical sense points out: “A good representation captures the essential — access to concrete-class internals is restricted by the elements of the event, deliberately leaving out the rest. ... compiler. But what about the human dimension of informa- The critical trick is to get the abstractions right, to represent tion hiding? If our presentations to students of concrete the important aspects and not the unimportant. This allows classes immediately follow our presentations of abstract everyone to concentrate upon the essentials without distrac- classes, inevitably information hiding will be compromised tion from irrelevancies. Herein lie both the power and the in the minds of the students, though not technically compro- weakness of representations: Get the relevant aspects right, mised in programs. Further, the programming-language and the representation provides substantive power to mechanisms we teach do very little to encourage the cre- enhance people’s ability to reason and think; get them ation of appropriate cover stories, other than to require oper- wrong, and the representation is misleading, causing people ation prototypes. Operation prototypes alone do little to to ignore critical aspects of the event or perhaps form mis- increase students’ ability to reason and think about the guided conclusions.” behavior of operations being prototyped, even when com- bined with specification-by-wishful-naming. Of course, by standard practice we often include natural-language descriptions of the operations in addition to the prototypes. However, it is our contention that such descriptions still do not help students reach their potential to reason and think carefully and rigorously about the behavior of operations. In the remainder of this paper, we present two examples in Insert is invoked to insert the integer i into the list loi. If stu- support of our contentions. The first example is a traditional dents were asked to fill in the values of the objects in box 1 container class that is typically presented in CS2. The sec- after the declaration of loi and before the invocation of the ond example is a simple class that might be presented quite Insert operation, and in box 2 after the call, what values early in CS1. would we expect our students to record? 2 Traditional Presentation of Lists It should not be surprising if some students drew pictures of a singly linked list representation of loi in both boxes. Why? For our first example, we consider a typical cover story for a Immediate presentation of one or more implementations of list abstraction. Usually the data type is described first; that lists has compromised information hiding in students’ is, we start with a description of the values that list objects minds and thus any abstraction has been lost [3]. Other stu- might assume. For example, we might say “a list is a linear dents might attempt to record values for loi based on the sequence of items with a current position indicator”. This is description “linear sequence of items with a current position followed by the operation prototypes and descriptions of indicator”. Even in this case, it is not clear exactly what stu- their behavior. Let’s assume there are operations to insert dents might write down. Why? Unless notation has been and remove items from a list and to change the position established for recording values of “linear sequences with a indicator. Then the description of an insert operation might current position indicator”, we should not be surprised to be “Insert (l, x) inserts item x in list l at the current posi- see a considerable variety in how students record values in tion”. (At times, requires and ensures clauses might be used the two boxes, even among students who are attempting to to describe the effect of the Insert operation. Even so, the think in terms of the cover story. degree of formality is essentially what we have just stated.) Other operations would be described similarly. At this point, it is tempting to dismiss concerns about how students might fill in the tracing table. Are uniformity and The cover story above might be followed by an example use rigor in such tasks really that important? After all, students for lists. Whether an example is present or not, it is custom- somehow “get” lists, even if each in their own way. How- ary for CS2 books to present one or more implementations ever, suppose we asked students to trace through a much of lists in close textual proximity to the cover story. A fre- more substantial example. Would their informal intuitions quently used implementation involves a singly linked list of and individualized notations serve them sufficiently well to nodes and pointers. Here is a possible representation of a achieve clear individual understanding? Would differences list containing the integers 3, 1, 2, and 4, in that order, and between individuals facilitate or impede clear communica- with current position indicator on 2: tion between them? Does this presentation of lists employ abstraction in such a way as to provide “substantive power to enhance people’s ability to reason and think”? We are data 3 1 2 4 convinced it does not. next null 3 Improved Presentation of Lists In order to realize the full potential of abstraction as start described by Norman, we need more explicit models of type cur_pos values and of operation descriptions. One way to accom- plish this is to use the time-tested and time-proven idea of To begin to understand the pedagogical shortcomings of this mathematical modeling. Specifically, we can use mathemat- presentation of lists, suppose that immediately following the ical types to describe (i.e., to model) program types. This presentation, we asked students to complete the following powerful idea allows for exact descriptions of the values of tracing table. complex objects (beyond built-in types) and of the behavior of operations on those objects. In CS1/CS2, almost all of the Statement Object Values models we need to use in this approach are familiar to stu- object loi: List_Of_Integer dents, such as sets, functions, tuples, integers, etc. object i: Integer i = 5 We should be careful to introduce students to specific nota- tion to be used with these models. The models then provide loi = ??? i = 5 1 an excellent mechanism for creating cover stories. In a pre- Insert (loi, i) cise notation, all relevant information can be described loi = ??? while leaving out unimportant details. As Norman points i = ??? 2 out, extraneous detail can create confusion and can cause errors and misguided conclusions. In the table, a list of integers, loi, and an integer, i, are Here is the abstract description of a List component in a dia- declared, the value of i is set to 5, and the list operation lect of the RESOLVE language [6, 7]. The abstraction employs mathematical modeling. Note that this is a generic ments from mathematical string theory. For example, the component (i.e., a template) parameterized by the type of post-condition of Insert is s.left = #s.left and s.right = the items that can be inserted in a list (type Item).
Recommended publications
  • Abstraction and the Origin of General Ideas with Broadly Nominalistic Scruples, While Others (E
    Philosophers’ volume 12, no. 19 1. Introduction 1 Imprint december 2012 Given their opposition to innate ideas, philosophers in the empiricist tradition have sought to explain how the rich and multifarious representational capacities that human beings possess derive from experience. A key explanatory strategy in this tradition, tracing back at least as far as John Locke’s An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, is to maintain that the acquisition of many of these Abstraction and the capacities can be accounted for by a process of abstraction. In fact, Locke himself claims in the Essay that abstraction is the source of all general ideas (1690/1975, II, xii, §1). Although Berkeley and Hume were highly critical of Locke, abstraction as a source of generality has Origin of General Ideas been a lasting theme in empiricist thought. Nearly a century after the publication of Locke’s Essay, for example, Thomas Reid, in his Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man, claims that “we cannot generalize without some degree of abstraction…” (Reid 1785/2002, p. 365). And more than a century later, Bertrand Russell remarks in The Problems of Philosophy: “When we see a white patch, we are acquainted, in the first instance, with the particular patch; but by seeing many white patches, we easily learn to abstract the whiteness which they all have in common, and in learning to do this we are learning to be acquainted with whiteness” (Russell 1912, p. 101). Stephen Laurence Despite the importance of abstraction as a central empiricist University of Sheffield strategy for explaining the origin of general ideas, it has never been clear exactly how the process of abstraction is supposed to work.
    [Show full text]
  • Georgia O'keeffe Museum
    COLLABORATIVE ABSTRACTION Grades: K-12 SUMMARY: Georgia O’Keeffe is known for her abstract flowers, landscapes, architecture and the sky. She would find the most interesting lines and colors in the world around her and simplify nature down to a series of organic shapes and forms. In this fun and fast-paced lesson, students will consider the concept and process of abstraction, collaboratively creating a line drawing that will become an abstract painting. This lesson is meant to be flexible. Please adapt for the level of your students and your individual curriculum. GUIDING QUESTIONS: Pelvis Series, Red with Yellow, 1945 What is abstraction? Georgia O’Keeffe Oil on canvas MATERIALS: 36 1/8 x 48 1/8 (91.8 x 122.2) Extended loan, private collection (1997.03.04L) Watercolor paper © Georgia O’Keeffe Museum Watercolor paints Paint brushes Cups of water LEARNING OBJECTIVES: Students will… Closely observe, analyze and interpret one of Georgia O’Keeffe’s abstract paintings Define the word abstraction and how it relates to visual art Work together to make collaborative abstract line drawings Individually interpret their collaborative line drawings and create a successful final composition Present their final compositions to the class Accept peer feedback INSTRUCTION: Engage: (5 min) Ask students to sketch the shapes they see in Pelvis Series, Red and Yellow by Georgia O’Keeffe (attached or found at https://www.okeeffemuseum.org/education/teacher-resources/) and write a short paragraph on what they see in the painting and what they think might be happening in the painting. Compare notes as a class and share the title of the painting.
    [Show full text]
  • Choose the Appropriate Level of Abstraction
    SAGE Flex for Public Speaking Choose the Appropriate Level of Abstraction Brief: Using abstract words can make a concept easier to communicate, but it breaks the connection to the specific meaning that we want to convey. Learning Objective: Understand how to choose the appropriate level of abstraction. Key Terms: • Abstraction: The process of perceiving similarities from our direct, specific observations of things around us, organizing the similarities, and then assigning a word label for the more general concept. • Abstraction Ladder: A concept developed by communication theorist S.I. Hayakawa, who compared levels of abstraction to a ladder, with the most specific concept at the bottom rung and the most abstract concept at the top rung. What is Abstraction? One of the ways we can attempt to make difficult information easier to understand is to simplify our word choices. To make our words more general, we use a higher level of abstraction. Abstraction is the process of perceiving similarities from our direct, specific observations of things around us, organizing the similarities, and then assigning a word label for the more general concept. From Granny Smiths to Produce Apples, oranges, bananas, pears…. Imagine how time consuming and confusing it would be to name each of these ingredients every time you talked about a fruit salad. Thankfully, you can use the general term fruit when you describe this dish. Now, imagine you’re trying to direct someone where to find fruit in a supermarket. Instead of using the name for each individual fruit or the general category of fruit, you’ll probably suggest they look in the produce department.
    [Show full text]
  • Collective Intelligence of Human Conversational Text K
    ISSN 2394-3777 (Print) ISSN 2394-3785 (Online) Available online at www.ijartet.com International Journal of Advanced Research Trends in Engineering and Technology (IJARTET) Vol. 3, Special Issue 20, April 2016 COLLECTIVE INTELLIGENCE OF HUMAN CONVERSATIONAL TEXT K. Bhuvaneswari Dr. K. Saravanan Research Scholar Research Advisor Department of Computer Science Dean, Academic Affairs PRIST University Department of Computer Science Thanjavur PRIST University, Thanjavur Abstract: Collective intelligence refers to the order to reveal themes relevant to collective intelligence. intelligence that emerges from local interactions Three levels of abstraction were identified in discussion among individual people. In the last few decades, web about the phenomenon: the micro-level, the macro-level 2.0 technologies have enabled new forms of collective and the level of emergence. Recurring themes in the intelligence that allow massive numbers of loosely literature were categorized under the abovementioned organized individuals to interact and create high framework and directions for future research were quality intellectual artefacts.Our analysis reveals a set identified. of research gaps in this area including the lack of focus on task oriented environments, the lack of sophisticated The selection of literature for this review methods to analyse the impact of group interaction follows the approach of Zott et al. (2011). A keyword process on each other, as well as the lack of focus on search was conducted on the Web of Knowledge on 7 the study of the impact of group interaction processes July 2011 using the keywords, collective intelligence and on participants over time. swarm intelligence. The searches produced 405 and 646 results, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Masters Theses Graduate School 8-2006 The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake Edgar Cuthbert Gentle University of Tennessee, Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes Part of the English Language and Literature Commons Recommended Citation Gentle, Edgar Cuthbert, "The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake. " Master's Thesis, University of Tennessee, 2006. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes/4508 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Edgar Cuthbert Gentle entitled "The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake." I have examined the final electronic copy of this thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in English. Nancy Goslee, Major Professor We have read this thesis and recommend its acceptance: ARRAY(0x7f6ff8e21fa0) Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) To the Graduate Council: I amsubmitting herewith a thesis written by EdgarCuthbert Gentle entitled"The Politics of Abstraction: Race,Gender, and Slavery in the Poetryof WilliamBlake." I have examinedthe finalpaper copy of this thesis forform and content and recommend that it be acceptedin partialfulfillm ent of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in English.
    [Show full text]
  • The Value of Abstraction
    The Value of Abstraction Mark K. Ho David Abel Princeton University Brown University Thomas L. Griffiths Michael L. Littman Princeton University Brown University Agents that can make better use of computation, experience, time, and memory can solve a greater range of problems more effectively. A crucial ingredient for managing such finite resources is intelligently chosen abstract representations. But, how do abstractions facilitate problem solving under limited resources? What makes an abstraction useful? To answer such questions, we review several trends in recent reinforcement-learning research that provide in- sight into how abstractions interact with learning and decision making. During learning, ab- straction can guide exploration and generalization as well as facilitate efficient tradeoffs—e.g., time spent learning versus the quality of a solution. During computation, good abstractions provide simplified models for computation while also preserving relevant information about decision-theoretic quantities. These features of abstraction are not only key for scaling up artificial problem solving, but can also shed light on what pressures shape the use of abstract representations in humans and other organisms. Keywords: abstraction, reinforcement learning, bounded rationality, planning, problem solving, rational analysis Highlights Introduction • Agents with finite space, time, and data can benefit In spite of bounds on space, time, and data, people are from abstract representations. able to make good decisions in complex scenarios. What en- ables us to do so? And what might equip artificial systems to • Both humans and artificial agents rely on abstractions do the same? One essential ingredient for making complex to solve complex problems. problem solving tractable is a capacity for exploiting abstract representation.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstraction Functions
    Understanding an ADT implementation: Abstraction functions CSE 331 University of Washington Michael Ernst Review: Connecting specifications and implementations Representation invariant : Object → boolean Indicates whether a data structure is well-formed Only well-formed representations are meaningful Defines the set of valid values of the data structure Abstraction function : Object → abstract value What the data structure means (as an abstract value) How the data structure is to be interpreted How do you compute the inverse, abstract value → Object ? Abstraction function: rep → abstract value The abstraction function maps the concrete representation to the abstract value it represents AF: Object → abstract value AF(CharSet this) = { c | c is contained in this.elts } “set of Characters contained in this.elts” Typically not executable The abstraction function lets us reason about behavior from the client perspective Abstraction function and insert impl. Our real goal is to satisfy the specification of insert : // modifies: this // effects: this post = this pre U {c} public void insert (Character c); The AF tells us what the rep means (and lets us place the blame) AF(CharSet this) = { c | c is contained in this.elts } Consider a call to insert: On entry , the meaning is AF(this pre ) ≈ elts pre On exit , the meaning is AF(this post ) = AF(this pre ) U {encrypt('a')} What if we used this abstraction function? AF(this) = { c | encrypt(c) is contained in this.elts } = { decrypt(c) | c is contained in this.elts } Stack rep: int[] elements ;
    [Show full text]
  • Increasing Creativity in Design by Functional Abstraction
    Increasing creativity in design by functional abstraction Manavbir Sahani Honors Thesis Human-Computer Interaction David Kirsh Scott Klemmer Steven Dow Michael Allen (Primary Advisor) (Graduate Advisor) ABSTRACT Opinion is divided, and conflicting literature is found on With advancements in big data, designers now have access to whether and under what conditions do examples fixate or in- design tools and ideation platforms that make it easy for them spire. There are studies that show that there is a design fixation to review hundreds of examples of related designs. But how effect by viewing examples [2]. And there is literature that can designers best use these examples to come up with creative argues that showing examples leads to more creative designs and innovative solutions to design and engineering problems? [3]. The existence of both positive and negative effects make it Building off creative cognition literature, we present an ap- harder to judge if and how the exposure to examples increase proach to increase creativity in design by making people think productivity for designers [4]. This inconclusiveness and di- more abstractly about the functions of the product in their vide in opinion exists in architecture too. In some architectural design problem through morphological concept generation. schools, the review of previous designs, especially excellent We tested this mechanism with an experiment in which the ones, is thought to inhibit imagination, squelching creativity. treatment and control condition had the same design task with Viewing previous work primes the mind to duplicate the past, the same examples for the same amount of time. The results making it harder to think freshly, appropriating the problem supported our claim and showed that the treatment condition completely.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Necessity of Abstraction
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect On the necessity of abstraction George Konidaris A generally intelligent agent faces a dilemma: it requires a as if it is a natural part of the environment. That is complex sensorimotor space to be capable of solving a wide perfectly appropriate for a chess-playing agent, but it range of problems, but many tasks are only feasible given the finesses a problem that must be solved by a general- right problem-specific formulation. I argue that a necessary purpose AI. but understudied requirement for general intelligence is the ability to form task-specific abstract representations. I show Consider one particular general-purpose agent that that the reinforcement learning paradigm structures this reasonably approximates a human—a robot with a question into how to learn action abstractions and how to native sensorimotor space consisting of video input learn state abstractions, and discuss the field’s progress on and motor control output—and the chessboards shown these topics. in Figure 1. Those chessboards are actually perceived by the robot as high-resolution color images, and the Address only actions it can choose to execute are to actuate the Computer Science Department, Brown University, 115 Waterman Street, motors attached to its joints. A general-purpose robot Providence, RI 02906, United States cannot expect to be given an abstract representation suitable for playing chess, just as it cannot expect to be Corresponding author: Konidaris, George ([email protected]) given one that is appropriate for scheduling a flight, playing Go, juggling, driving cross-country, or compos- Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2019, 29:xx-yy ing a sonnet.
    [Show full text]
  • Concrete and Specific Language
    CONCRETE AND SPECIFIC LANGUAGE Effective writers use and mix language at all levels of abstraction, so we must learn to use language on all levels. But first, it's important to understand what is meant by abstract and concrete language and also by general and specific language. In brief, we conceive the abstract though our mental processes and perceive the concrete through our senses. ■Abstract vs. Concrete Language Abstract words refer to intangible qualities, ideas, and concepts. These words indicate things we know only through our intellect, like "truth," "honor," "kindness," and "grace." Concrete words refer to tangible, qualities or characteristics, things we know through our senses. Words and phrases like "102 degrees," "obese Siamese cat," and "deep spruce green" are concrete. ABSTRACT: To excel in college, you’ll have to work hard. CONCRETE: To excel in college, you’ll need to do go to every class; do all your reading before you go; write several drafts of each paper; and review your notes for each class weekly. ■General vs. Specific Language General words refer to large classes and broad areas. "Sports teams," "jobs," and "video games" are general terms. Specific words designate particular items or individual cases, so "ISU Bengals," chemistry tutor," and "Halo" are specific terms. GENERAL: The student enjoyed the class. SPECIFIC: Kelly enjoyed Professor Sprout's 8:00 a.m. Herbology class. ■The Ladder of Abstraction Most words do not fall nicely into categories; they’re not always either abstract or concrete, general or specific. Moreover, the abstract and general often overlap, as do the concrete and specific.
    [Show full text]
  • 1990-Learning Abstraction Hierarchies for Problem Solving
    From: AAAI-90 Proceedings. Copyright ©1990, AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. Learning Abstraction Hierarchies for Problem Solving Craig A. Knoblock* School of Computer Science Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213 [email protected] Abstract consists of a set of operators with preconditions and ef- fects, and a problem to be solved, ALPINE reformulates The use of abstraction in problem solving is an this space into successively more abstract ones. Each effective approach to reducing search, but finding abstraction space is an approximation of the original good abstractions is a difficult problem, even for problem space (base space), formed by dropping lit- people. This paper identifies a criterion for se- erals in the domain. The system determines what to lecting useful abstractions, describes a tractable abstract based on the ordered monotonicity property. algorithm for generating them, and empirically This property separates out those features of the prob- demonstrates that the abstractions reduce search. lem that can be solved and then held invariant while The abstraction learner, called ALPINE, is inte- the remaining parts of the problem are solved. Since grated with the PRODIGY problem solver [Minton this property depends on the problem to be solved, et ab., 1989b, Carbonell et al., 19901 and has been ALPINE produces abstraction hierarchies that are tai- tested on large problem sets in multiple domains. lored to the individual problems. The paper is organized as follows. The next sec- Introduction tion defines the ordered monotonicity property. The third section describes the algorithm for generating ab- Hierarchical problem solving uses abstraction to re- straction hierarchies in ALPINE.
    [Show full text]
  • Conjoining Gestalt Rules for Abstraction of Architectural Drawings
    Conjoining Gestalt Rules for Abstraction of Architectural Drawings Liangliang Nan 1 Andrei Sharf Ke Xie1 Tien-Tsin Wong3 Oliver Deus sen" Daniel Cohen-Or5 Baoquan Chen1 1 SlAT 2 Ben Gurion Univ. 3 CUHK 4 Konstanz Univ. 5 Tel Aviv Univ. Figure 1: Simplification a/a cOlllplex cityscape line-drawing obtained [(sing o/./ /" Gestalt-based abstraction. Abstract those of architectural models. Our approach to abstracting shape directly aims to clarify shape and preserve meaningful structures We present a method for structural summari zation and abstraction using Gestalt principles. of complex spatial arrangements found in architectural draw in gs. Thc wdl-knllwn Gcstalt principles hy Wcrthciml:r rI 92:1 ], rdkct The method is based on the well -known Gestalt rules, which sum­ strategies of the human vi sual system to group objects into forms mari ze how form s, pattern s, and semantics are perceived by humans and create internal representations for them. Wheneve r groups of I"rOI11 hits and pi ccl:s 0 1" gl:o l11 l: tri c inl"ormati on. Although defining visual element have one or several characteristics in common, they a computational model for each rul e alone has been extensively s­ get grouped and form a new larger vi sual object - a gestalt. Psychol­ tudied, modeling a conjoint of Gestalt rules remains a challenge. ogists have tried to simulate and model these principles, by find in g In thi s work, we develop a computational framework which mod­ computational means to predict wh at human perceive as gestalts in els Gestalt rules and more importantly, their complex interaction­ images.
    [Show full text]