Conjoining Gestalt Rules for Abstraction of Architectural Drawings

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Conjoining Gestalt Rules for Abstraction of Architectural Drawings Conjoining Gestalt Rules for Abstraction of Architectural Drawings Liangliang Nan 1 Andrei Sharf Ke Xie1 Tien-Tsin Wong3 Oliver Deus sen" Daniel Cohen-Or5 Baoquan Chen1 1 SlAT 2 Ben Gurion Univ. 3 CUHK 4 Konstanz Univ. 5 Tel Aviv Univ. Figure 1: Simplification a/a cOlllplex cityscape line-drawing obtained [(sing o/./ /" Gestalt-based abstraction. Abstract those of architectural models. Our approach to abstracting shape directly aims to clarify shape and preserve meaningful structures We present a method for structural summari zation and abstraction using Gestalt principles. of complex spatial arrangements found in architectural draw in gs. Thc wdl-knllwn Gcstalt principles hy Wcrthciml:r rI 92:1 ], rdkct The method is based on the well -known Gestalt rules, which sum­ strategies of the human vi sual system to group objects into forms mari ze how form s, pattern s, and semantics are perceived by humans and create internal representations for them. Wheneve r groups of I"rOI11 hits and pi ccl:s 0 1" gl:o l11 l: tri c inl"ormati on. Although defining visual element have one or several characteristics in common, they a computational model for each rul e alone has been extensively s­ get grouped and form a new larger vi sual object - a gestalt. Psychol­ tudied, modeling a conjoint of Gestalt rules remains a challenge. ogists have tried to simulate and model these principles, by find in g In thi s work, we develop a computational framework which mod­ computational means to predict wh at human perceive as gestalts in els Gestalt rules and more importantly, their complex interaction­ images. s. We apply conjoining ru.les to line drawings, to detect groups of objects and repetitions that conform to Gestalt principles. We sum­ The notion of Gestalt is very well -k nown and widely used in var­ mari ze and abstract such groups in ways that maintain structural ious fi elds. In particular, it ex pl ains the tendency of the human semantics by displaying only a reduced number of repeated ele­ vi sual recognition to form whole shapes and forms just from bits ments, or by replacing them with si mpler shapes. We show an ap­ and pieces of geometric information. Naturall y, Gestalt principles plication of our method to line drawings of architectural models of have been used in computer vision, primarily in context with object various styl es, and the potenti al of extending the technique to other recognition and scene understanding. In computer graphics, Gestalt computer-generated illustrations, and three-dimensional models. principles have been applied to a variety of applications, like scene completion [Orori et a!. 2003], image and scene abstraction [Wang et a!. 2004; Mehra et a!. 2009], stroke synthesis [Barla el a!. 2006; Ijiri et a!. 2008] and emerging images generation [Mitra et a!. 2009]. Tn general, these works rely on discrete Gestalt principles, but none addresses the complex interactions emerging from the multitude of Gestalt principles operating simultaneously. A diffi cult problem while dealin g wit.h gestalts is th e conj oin ed ef­ fect of two or more Gestalt principles operating at the same time 1 Introduction on the same site. Modeling gestalts in such cases is especiall y challenging due to the complexity and ambiguity of the scene. Re­ Artistic im agery, architectural renderings, cartography and games cently, attempts to di scover how grouping principles interact were often ex pl oit abstracti on to clari fy, exaggerate, simplify or empha­ made in psychology and computer vision [Oesolneux et a!. 2002; size the visual content. Abstraction is a strategy for communicating Feldman 2003; Cao et a!. 2007; Kubovy and van den Berg 2008]. in fo rmat ion effectively. It all ows artists to hi ghli ght specifi c visu­ These works model limited gestalt interactions, by finding com­ al inform ation and thereby direct the viewer to important aspects putationalmeans which are physiologically pl ausible. Kubovyand of the structure and organization of the scene. In thi s paper, we van den Berg [2008] explore the quantification of perceptual group­ present. an approach to the abstracti on of 20 shapes, in particul ar ings formed conjointly by two grouping principles: similarity and prox imity. Nevertheless, providing general computational means for modeling the interaction of multiple Gestalt principles remains a diffi cult chall enge. In thi s paper, we take a first step in developing a computati onal model fo r conjoining Gestalt rul es. We model a subset of Gestalt rules and their mutual interaction for abstracting architectural line drawings. We choose to focus on architectural drawings since typ­ ically their visual elements are of rather low complexity and their spatial arrangement is strongly biased to the main axes (due to en­ gineering considerati ons). Hence, architectural drawings consist of prevalent similarities, proximities and regul arities among their el- 2 • • • • • • • • • • 0 0 • • • • • 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 • • • • • 0 0 • • • • • (a) (b) (c) Figure 2: Conjoining gestalts (frOIl! KClIlizsa [1 980]). Overlapping (a): white dots are elemellts oj the grid (regularity) and sim ultaneously belong to a curve (continuity). Conflicting (b): colltinllit)' principle OJ IlliO closed curves (b-Ieft) cO Ilf/iets wilh the symlllell y princip le (b-righl). Masking (c): the basis oj the triangle becomes invisible as it is embedded ill a grollp oj regularly paraliellines. ements, forming complex grouping conligurati ons that can be de­ ies focused main ly on qualitative and empirical studies, more recent scribed by Gestalt principles and their interaction. These rules de­ psychological works study quantitative aspects of Gesta lt principles fin e vari ous perceptual scene groupings, naturall y lending them­ and their conjoining. Desolneux et al. [2002] and Cao et al. [2007] selves to simplilicati on and ahstraction of architectural drawings. formulate probabilistic quantities for di stinctive Gesta lt rules and utilize them for detecting collinearity, regul arity and p roximity in Interacting gestalts rules cast a difli cult problem sin ce they oper­ images. In hi s work, Feldman [2003] suggests a hi era rchical rep­ ate simultaneously on common sites. They can compete, confl ict, resentati on for modeling proximity and collinearity grouping prin­ overlap, and mask with each other yielding complex vi sual phe­ ciples. He applies a minimal model theory for selecting the best nomena. Up to now, psychologists are stili studying how exactl y grouping interpretation, pre ferring a maximally explained group. multiple Gestalt principles interact. It is not in our scope to study Kubovy and van den Berg [2008] present a probabilistic model for the human visual system under these complex phenomena. Our measuring proximity and similarity grouping quantities. Similar to goal is to quantitatively model and apply conjoining rules with re­ us, they analyze and model the conjoint effect o f grouping princi­ spect to a specific type of data. Specifically, we propose a co m­ ples operati ng simultaneously. Nevertheless, their work focuses on putational f ramcwork to fa cilitate the integrati on of fi ve principal interacti on of only two principles performing on simple dot lattice Gestalt rul es, namely similarity, proximity, contintlity, closure, and confi gurations, while our goal is to provide a generic fra mework fo r regularity, and then apply it to the abstracti on of architectural line resolving conjoining gestalts in the context of architectural abstrac­ drawings. Due to the grouping nature of Gestalt rules, we fo rmulate tion. Claessens and Wagemans [2008] present a B ayesian model abstraction as a grouping optimizati on problem. The additive nature for contour detecti on using proximity and collinearity grouping. In of Gestalt principles interactions (i.e., gestalts combine additively contrast to ours, their method handles simplified inte racti ons be­ as showed in [Elder and Goldberg 2002; Feldman 2003; Kubovy tween two gestalts that perform as independent variables on dot and van den Berg 2008)) is modeled by a multi-label normali zed lattices. In recent work by Cole et al. [2008; 2009], the authors graph cut formulation. study the correlation between shape conveying hand d rawn lines and speei fi e 3D shape reatures. Their work quanti fi cs the strength We apply conjoining Gestalt grouping rules, inspired by the com­ of different line drawings at depi cting shape. putati onal model of Kubovy and van den Berg [2008]. We devel­ op a global energy function that relates grouping strength , number Abstraction and S implification of 20 Content. Gestalt effect is of di sjoint groups and inter-part characteri sti cs to Gestalt princi­ hi ghl y re lated to abstracti on and simplifi cati on. By understanding ples. The minimal energy de fin es a global segmentati on into di s­ how a collecti on of detail s aggregates, we can replace this collec­ joint groups while accounting for metrics that measure grouping in tion with a simpler, coarser or abstract representati on, while pre­ terms of Gestalt rules. The set of e lements grouped is then abstract­ serving the o riginal semantics. In computer graphics and vi sion, ed by means of repl acin g them with simplilied or abstract represen­ Gestalt prin ciples have been studi ed in the contex t of simpli fica ti on tatives. Our method computes a progressive series of abstracti ons and abstraction in several works. We focus here on works which from the fi nest, most detailed drawin g, up to the coarsest most ab­ address simpli fication usin g perceptual principles. stracted one (see Fi gure I). Our framework naturall y lends itselr to effi cient abstracti on of ar­ In an earl y work, DeCarl o and Santell a [2002] compute image ab­ chitectural line drawings confo rming to perceptual grouping con­ stracti on by preserving meaningful structures using an eye tracker cepts.
Recommended publications
  • Abstraction and the Origin of General Ideas with Broadly Nominalistic Scruples, While Others (E
    Philosophers’ volume 12, no. 19 1. Introduction 1 Imprint december 2012 Given their opposition to innate ideas, philosophers in the empiricist tradition have sought to explain how the rich and multifarious representational capacities that human beings possess derive from experience. A key explanatory strategy in this tradition, tracing back at least as far as John Locke’s An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, is to maintain that the acquisition of many of these Abstraction and the capacities can be accounted for by a process of abstraction. In fact, Locke himself claims in the Essay that abstraction is the source of all general ideas (1690/1975, II, xii, §1). Although Berkeley and Hume were highly critical of Locke, abstraction as a source of generality has Origin of General Ideas been a lasting theme in empiricist thought. Nearly a century after the publication of Locke’s Essay, for example, Thomas Reid, in his Essays on the Intellectual Powers of Man, claims that “we cannot generalize without some degree of abstraction…” (Reid 1785/2002, p. 365). And more than a century later, Bertrand Russell remarks in The Problems of Philosophy: “When we see a white patch, we are acquainted, in the first instance, with the particular patch; but by seeing many white patches, we easily learn to abstract the whiteness which they all have in common, and in learning to do this we are learning to be acquainted with whiteness” (Russell 1912, p. 101). Stephen Laurence Despite the importance of abstraction as a central empiricist University of Sheffield strategy for explaining the origin of general ideas, it has never been clear exactly how the process of abstraction is supposed to work.
    [Show full text]
  • Georgia O'keeffe Museum
    COLLABORATIVE ABSTRACTION Grades: K-12 SUMMARY: Georgia O’Keeffe is known for her abstract flowers, landscapes, architecture and the sky. She would find the most interesting lines and colors in the world around her and simplify nature down to a series of organic shapes and forms. In this fun and fast-paced lesson, students will consider the concept and process of abstraction, collaboratively creating a line drawing that will become an abstract painting. This lesson is meant to be flexible. Please adapt for the level of your students and your individual curriculum. GUIDING QUESTIONS: Pelvis Series, Red with Yellow, 1945 What is abstraction? Georgia O’Keeffe Oil on canvas MATERIALS: 36 1/8 x 48 1/8 (91.8 x 122.2) Extended loan, private collection (1997.03.04L) Watercolor paper © Georgia O’Keeffe Museum Watercolor paints Paint brushes Cups of water LEARNING OBJECTIVES: Students will… Closely observe, analyze and interpret one of Georgia O’Keeffe’s abstract paintings Define the word abstraction and how it relates to visual art Work together to make collaborative abstract line drawings Individually interpret their collaborative line drawings and create a successful final composition Present their final compositions to the class Accept peer feedback INSTRUCTION: Engage: (5 min) Ask students to sketch the shapes they see in Pelvis Series, Red and Yellow by Georgia O’Keeffe (attached or found at https://www.okeeffemuseum.org/education/teacher-resources/) and write a short paragraph on what they see in the painting and what they think might be happening in the painting. Compare notes as a class and share the title of the painting.
    [Show full text]
  • Choose the Appropriate Level of Abstraction
    SAGE Flex for Public Speaking Choose the Appropriate Level of Abstraction Brief: Using abstract words can make a concept easier to communicate, but it breaks the connection to the specific meaning that we want to convey. Learning Objective: Understand how to choose the appropriate level of abstraction. Key Terms: • Abstraction: The process of perceiving similarities from our direct, specific observations of things around us, organizing the similarities, and then assigning a word label for the more general concept. • Abstraction Ladder: A concept developed by communication theorist S.I. Hayakawa, who compared levels of abstraction to a ladder, with the most specific concept at the bottom rung and the most abstract concept at the top rung. What is Abstraction? One of the ways we can attempt to make difficult information easier to understand is to simplify our word choices. To make our words more general, we use a higher level of abstraction. Abstraction is the process of perceiving similarities from our direct, specific observations of things around us, organizing the similarities, and then assigning a word label for the more general concept. From Granny Smiths to Produce Apples, oranges, bananas, pears…. Imagine how time consuming and confusing it would be to name each of these ingredients every time you talked about a fruit salad. Thankfully, you can use the general term fruit when you describe this dish. Now, imagine you’re trying to direct someone where to find fruit in a supermarket. Instead of using the name for each individual fruit or the general category of fruit, you’ll probably suggest they look in the produce department.
    [Show full text]
  • Collective Intelligence of Human Conversational Text K
    ISSN 2394-3777 (Print) ISSN 2394-3785 (Online) Available online at www.ijartet.com International Journal of Advanced Research Trends in Engineering and Technology (IJARTET) Vol. 3, Special Issue 20, April 2016 COLLECTIVE INTELLIGENCE OF HUMAN CONVERSATIONAL TEXT K. Bhuvaneswari Dr. K. Saravanan Research Scholar Research Advisor Department of Computer Science Dean, Academic Affairs PRIST University Department of Computer Science Thanjavur PRIST University, Thanjavur Abstract: Collective intelligence refers to the order to reveal themes relevant to collective intelligence. intelligence that emerges from local interactions Three levels of abstraction were identified in discussion among individual people. In the last few decades, web about the phenomenon: the micro-level, the macro-level 2.0 technologies have enabled new forms of collective and the level of emergence. Recurring themes in the intelligence that allow massive numbers of loosely literature were categorized under the abovementioned organized individuals to interact and create high framework and directions for future research were quality intellectual artefacts.Our analysis reveals a set identified. of research gaps in this area including the lack of focus on task oriented environments, the lack of sophisticated The selection of literature for this review methods to analyse the impact of group interaction follows the approach of Zott et al. (2011). A keyword process on each other, as well as the lack of focus on search was conducted on the Web of Knowledge on 7 the study of the impact of group interaction processes July 2011 using the keywords, collective intelligence and on participants over time. swarm intelligence. The searches produced 405 and 646 results, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Masters Theses Graduate School 8-2006 The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake Edgar Cuthbert Gentle University of Tennessee, Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes Part of the English Language and Literature Commons Recommended Citation Gentle, Edgar Cuthbert, "The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake. " Master's Thesis, University of Tennessee, 2006. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_gradthes/4508 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a thesis written by Edgar Cuthbert Gentle entitled "The Politics of Abstraction: Race, Gender, and Slavery in the Poetry of William Blake." I have examined the final electronic copy of this thesis for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in English. Nancy Goslee, Major Professor We have read this thesis and recommend its acceptance: ARRAY(0x7f6ff8e21fa0) Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) To the Graduate Council: I amsubmitting herewith a thesis written by EdgarCuthbert Gentle entitled"The Politics of Abstraction: Race,Gender, and Slavery in the Poetryof WilliamBlake." I have examinedthe finalpaper copy of this thesis forform and content and recommend that it be acceptedin partialfulfillm ent of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts, with a major in English.
    [Show full text]
  • The Value of Abstraction
    The Value of Abstraction Mark K. Ho David Abel Princeton University Brown University Thomas L. Griffiths Michael L. Littman Princeton University Brown University Agents that can make better use of computation, experience, time, and memory can solve a greater range of problems more effectively. A crucial ingredient for managing such finite resources is intelligently chosen abstract representations. But, how do abstractions facilitate problem solving under limited resources? What makes an abstraction useful? To answer such questions, we review several trends in recent reinforcement-learning research that provide in- sight into how abstractions interact with learning and decision making. During learning, ab- straction can guide exploration and generalization as well as facilitate efficient tradeoffs—e.g., time spent learning versus the quality of a solution. During computation, good abstractions provide simplified models for computation while also preserving relevant information about decision-theoretic quantities. These features of abstraction are not only key for scaling up artificial problem solving, but can also shed light on what pressures shape the use of abstract representations in humans and other organisms. Keywords: abstraction, reinforcement learning, bounded rationality, planning, problem solving, rational analysis Highlights Introduction • Agents with finite space, time, and data can benefit In spite of bounds on space, time, and data, people are from abstract representations. able to make good decisions in complex scenarios. What en- ables us to do so? And what might equip artificial systems to • Both humans and artificial agents rely on abstractions do the same? One essential ingredient for making complex to solve complex problems. problem solving tractable is a capacity for exploiting abstract representation.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstraction Functions
    Understanding an ADT implementation: Abstraction functions CSE 331 University of Washington Michael Ernst Review: Connecting specifications and implementations Representation invariant : Object → boolean Indicates whether a data structure is well-formed Only well-formed representations are meaningful Defines the set of valid values of the data structure Abstraction function : Object → abstract value What the data structure means (as an abstract value) How the data structure is to be interpreted How do you compute the inverse, abstract value → Object ? Abstraction function: rep → abstract value The abstraction function maps the concrete representation to the abstract value it represents AF: Object → abstract value AF(CharSet this) = { c | c is contained in this.elts } “set of Characters contained in this.elts” Typically not executable The abstraction function lets us reason about behavior from the client perspective Abstraction function and insert impl. Our real goal is to satisfy the specification of insert : // modifies: this // effects: this post = this pre U {c} public void insert (Character c); The AF tells us what the rep means (and lets us place the blame) AF(CharSet this) = { c | c is contained in this.elts } Consider a call to insert: On entry , the meaning is AF(this pre ) ≈ elts pre On exit , the meaning is AF(this post ) = AF(this pre ) U {encrypt('a')} What if we used this abstraction function? AF(this) = { c | encrypt(c) is contained in this.elts } = { decrypt(c) | c is contained in this.elts } Stack rep: int[] elements ;
    [Show full text]
  • Increasing Creativity in Design by Functional Abstraction
    Increasing creativity in design by functional abstraction Manavbir Sahani Honors Thesis Human-Computer Interaction David Kirsh Scott Klemmer Steven Dow Michael Allen (Primary Advisor) (Graduate Advisor) ABSTRACT Opinion is divided, and conflicting literature is found on With advancements in big data, designers now have access to whether and under what conditions do examples fixate or in- design tools and ideation platforms that make it easy for them spire. There are studies that show that there is a design fixation to review hundreds of examples of related designs. But how effect by viewing examples [2]. And there is literature that can designers best use these examples to come up with creative argues that showing examples leads to more creative designs and innovative solutions to design and engineering problems? [3]. The existence of both positive and negative effects make it Building off creative cognition literature, we present an ap- harder to judge if and how the exposure to examples increase proach to increase creativity in design by making people think productivity for designers [4]. This inconclusiveness and di- more abstractly about the functions of the product in their vide in opinion exists in architecture too. In some architectural design problem through morphological concept generation. schools, the review of previous designs, especially excellent We tested this mechanism with an experiment in which the ones, is thought to inhibit imagination, squelching creativity. treatment and control condition had the same design task with Viewing previous work primes the mind to duplicate the past, the same examples for the same amount of time. The results making it harder to think freshly, appropriating the problem supported our claim and showed that the treatment condition completely.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Necessity of Abstraction
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect On the necessity of abstraction George Konidaris A generally intelligent agent faces a dilemma: it requires a as if it is a natural part of the environment. That is complex sensorimotor space to be capable of solving a wide perfectly appropriate for a chess-playing agent, but it range of problems, but many tasks are only feasible given the finesses a problem that must be solved by a general- right problem-specific formulation. I argue that a necessary purpose AI. but understudied requirement for general intelligence is the ability to form task-specific abstract representations. I show Consider one particular general-purpose agent that that the reinforcement learning paradigm structures this reasonably approximates a human—a robot with a question into how to learn action abstractions and how to native sensorimotor space consisting of video input learn state abstractions, and discuss the field’s progress on and motor control output—and the chessboards shown these topics. in Figure 1. Those chessboards are actually perceived by the robot as high-resolution color images, and the Address only actions it can choose to execute are to actuate the Computer Science Department, Brown University, 115 Waterman Street, motors attached to its joints. A general-purpose robot Providence, RI 02906, United States cannot expect to be given an abstract representation suitable for playing chess, just as it cannot expect to be Corresponding author: Konidaris, George ([email protected]) given one that is appropriate for scheduling a flight, playing Go, juggling, driving cross-country, or compos- Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2019, 29:xx-yy ing a sonnet.
    [Show full text]
  • Concrete and Specific Language
    CONCRETE AND SPECIFIC LANGUAGE Effective writers use and mix language at all levels of abstraction, so we must learn to use language on all levels. But first, it's important to understand what is meant by abstract and concrete language and also by general and specific language. In brief, we conceive the abstract though our mental processes and perceive the concrete through our senses. ■Abstract vs. Concrete Language Abstract words refer to intangible qualities, ideas, and concepts. These words indicate things we know only through our intellect, like "truth," "honor," "kindness," and "grace." Concrete words refer to tangible, qualities or characteristics, things we know through our senses. Words and phrases like "102 degrees," "obese Siamese cat," and "deep spruce green" are concrete. ABSTRACT: To excel in college, you’ll have to work hard. CONCRETE: To excel in college, you’ll need to do go to every class; do all your reading before you go; write several drafts of each paper; and review your notes for each class weekly. ■General vs. Specific Language General words refer to large classes and broad areas. "Sports teams," "jobs," and "video games" are general terms. Specific words designate particular items or individual cases, so "ISU Bengals," chemistry tutor," and "Halo" are specific terms. GENERAL: The student enjoyed the class. SPECIFIC: Kelly enjoyed Professor Sprout's 8:00 a.m. Herbology class. ■The Ladder of Abstraction Most words do not fall nicely into categories; they’re not always either abstract or concrete, general or specific. Moreover, the abstract and general often overlap, as do the concrete and specific.
    [Show full text]
  • 1990-Learning Abstraction Hierarchies for Problem Solving
    From: AAAI-90 Proceedings. Copyright ©1990, AAAI (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. Learning Abstraction Hierarchies for Problem Solving Craig A. Knoblock* School of Computer Science Carnegie Mellon University Pittsburgh, PA 15213 [email protected] Abstract consists of a set of operators with preconditions and ef- fects, and a problem to be solved, ALPINE reformulates The use of abstraction in problem solving is an this space into successively more abstract ones. Each effective approach to reducing search, but finding abstraction space is an approximation of the original good abstractions is a difficult problem, even for problem space (base space), formed by dropping lit- people. This paper identifies a criterion for se- erals in the domain. The system determines what to lecting useful abstractions, describes a tractable abstract based on the ordered monotonicity property. algorithm for generating them, and empirically This property separates out those features of the prob- demonstrates that the abstractions reduce search. lem that can be solved and then held invariant while The abstraction learner, called ALPINE, is inte- the remaining parts of the problem are solved. Since grated with the PRODIGY problem solver [Minton this property depends on the problem to be solved, et ab., 1989b, Carbonell et al., 19901 and has been ALPINE produces abstraction hierarchies that are tai- tested on large problem sets in multiple domains. lored to the individual problems. The paper is organized as follows. The next sec- Introduction tion defines the ordered monotonicity property. The third section describes the algorithm for generating ab- Hierarchical problem solving uses abstraction to re- straction hierarchies in ALPINE.
    [Show full text]
  • Making Abstraction Less Abstract: the Logical, Psychological, and Metaphysical Dimensions of Avicenna’S Theory of Abstraction
    Making Abstraction Less Abstract: The Logical, Psychological, and Metaphysical Dimensions of Avicenna’s Theory of Abstraction Jon McGinnis Abstract: A debated topic in Avicennan psychology is whether for Avicenna abstraction is a metaphor for emanation or to be taken literally. This issue stems from the deeper philosophical question of whether humans acquire intelligibles externally from an emanation by the Active Intellect, which is a separate substance, or internally from an inherently human cognitive process, which prepares us for an emanation from the Active Intellect. I argue that the tension between these doctrines is only apparent. In his logical works Avicenna limns an account where through the internal human process of abstraction accidents accruing to an es- sence existing in matter are extracted, thus preparing the essence for new accidents emanating externally from the Active Intellect, which make the essence something conceptualized in the intellect. This study, then, outlines the epistemological and metaphysical framework presented in the logical works that underpins Avicenna’s theory of abstraction presented in his psychological works. n his 2001 article “Avicenna on Abstraction” Dag Hasse submits Avicenna’s doctrine of tajrīd, abstraction, to close scrutiny. He focuses on a cluster of concepts that appear in Avicenna’s earliest treatment of abstraction and then followsI their evolution throughout Avicenna’s corpus. In opposition to a well-received view that makes Avicenna’s notion of abstraction a metaphor for an emanation from the Active Intellect, and thus an operation that is external to the human soul, Hasse concludes that for Avicenna abstraction refers to an internal cognitive operation proper to the human soul itself.
    [Show full text]