English Negative Concord, Negative Polarity, and Double Negation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

English Negative Concord, Negative Polarity, and Double Negation City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works All Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects Dissertations, Theses, and Capstone Projects 9-2015 English Negative Concord, Negative Polarity, and Double Negation Frances Kathleen Blanchette Graduate Center, City University of New York How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/gc_etds/866 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] English Negative Concord, Negative Polarity, and Double Negation by Frances Blanchette A dissertation submitted to the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, The City University of New York 2015 ii © 2015 Frances Blanchette All Rights Reserved iii This manuscript has been read and accepted for the Graduate Faculty in Linguistics in satisfaction of the dissertation requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Christina Tortora _______________ __________________________ Date Chair of Examining Committee Gita Martohardjono _______________ __________________________ Date Executive Officer Christina Tortora _____________________________ Virginia Valian _____________________________ William McClure ______________________________ Chris Collins ______________________________ Supervisory Committee THE CITY UNIVERSITY OF NEW YORK iv Abstract English Negative Concord, Negative Polarity, and Double Negation by Frances Blanchette Advisor: Christina Tortora In Negative Concord (NC) sentences, single negative meanings are expressed by two or more negative words. English speakers that use NC also employ Double Negation (DN), where two negatives yield a logical affirmative. The same speakers also use Negative Polarity Item (NPI) constructions, where words like anything and anybody depend on a preceding negation (e.g. ‘I didn’t eat anything’ vs. ‘I ate anything’). This dissertation accounts for the distributions of NC, NPI, and DN constructions in English. I apply the theory of NPIs in Postal (2005) and Collins and Postal (2014) to NC and DN. These authors argue that some NPIs have the form [NEG SOME X], with a single NEG, while others have two: [[NEG [NEG SOME]] X]. I propose that negative constituents have a structure identical to Collins and Postal’s (2014) unary NEG NPIs. Like NPI constructions, NC with a negative marker (-n’t/not) and a negative object involves syntactic NEG raising from the negative constituent. I further propose that the locus of variation between NC and NPI constructions lies at the level of morphophonological spell out. NPI constructions involve deletion of lower occurrences of a single NEG, but NC does not. Using data from the Audio-Aligned and Parsed v Corpus of Appalachian English (Tortora et al., In Progress), I show that all predictions concerning the distribution of NC, DN, and NPI constructions across clause boundaries are borne out. Two types of NC with negative subjects are also analyzed. NC declaratives like ‘didn’t nobody eat’ and ‘nobody didn’t eat’ are derived via NEG raising from a negative constituent. In these cases, NEG raising is followed by remnant raising of the negative constituent. To explain restrictions on subject type in inverted structures (‘didn’t nobody eat’), I defend a condition stating that the subject must always be negative, despite the fact that it is not always morphologically negative. Differences in usage and interpretation of negative object and negative subject constructions are derived by appeal to a remnant raising condition. The results of a gradient acceptability study support the hypothesized grammatical distinction between Subject and Object NC. vi Acknowledgments I have so many kind, generous, wonderful, and brilliant people to thank for helping me reach this stage. I begin with the members of my dissertation committee. From my first week in graduate school to today, Christina Tortora has been an amazing teacher, advocate, and role model. I cannot even begin to list the many opportunities she created for me, but I am especially grateful for the opportunity to work on The Audio- Aligned and Parsed Corpus of Appalachian English. My work on this project not only provided me with crucial financial support, but it introduced me to my linguistics soul mate, English Negative Concord, and to the data set that inspired this work. My career as a linguist thus far would look very different were it not for Christina’s encouragement, support, and her entrepreneurial spirit. I am very grateful to have her as a teacher and a friend. I am also very grateful for Virginia Valian, who taught me how to design thoughtful and precise experiments, and who always encouraged and supported me in pursuing my interests. I am very grateful for the fact that she allowed me to take chances, and balanced her high expectations with support and patience. I’d also like to thank Virginia for her very important work on gender, which applies names and numbers to the frequently imperceptible forces working against women in science and academia. Virginia’s work on this issue has raised my consciousness and helped me to feel more confident in my own abilities, and I hope to one day be able to pay these gifts forward. Bill McClure introduced me to semantics, and I am very grateful for his encouragement and kindness. While I had less contact with Bill in my last few years at vii CUNY, his positive and encouraging presence has had a lasting impact, and he is one of the major reasons why I will remember CUNY fondly. I was incredibly lucky to have my last class as a doctoral student be a seminar on negation with Chris Collins at NYU. The impact of Chris’s work on mine is impossible to miss, but I am also very grateful for his teaching. His openness to all kinds of data and ideas inspired me to try new things, and his patience and kindness in correcting my mistakes inspired me to persist. I am very grateful for all of the time he put into helping me write and revise this thesis, and also for all the work he put into helping me develop as a linguist. I would also like to thank Paul Postal, whose extensive comments on my defense draft resulted in a final product that is about sixty pages shorter. I greatly appreciate the time he spent writing those comments, and I am sure that the effects of the teaching therein will be lasting. The CUNY Graduate Center has been a wonderful and interesting place to study, and I am very grateful to the Linguistics Department for allowing me to be a part of their community. Beyond my committee, I have many wonderful professors to thank. Marcel den Dikken provided a great deal of inspiration, interesting and exciting courses, and also several opportunities for developing my academic skills beyond linguistics research, for which I am very grateful. I am also grateful to Martin Chodorow for teaching a wonderful introductory course in statistics, and to Dianne Bradley for a very useful QP1 workshop. Thanks also, in no particular order, to Gita Martohardjono, Juliette Blevins, Bob Fiengo, Loraine Obler, Janet Dean Fodor, and Sam Al Khatib for helping to make the CUNY GC an engaging, exciting, and happy place to work. viii My fellow students also deserve many thanks. I am very grateful to Ignacio Montoya for his collaboration in work, and for his friendship in life. I consider myself very lucky to have found such an intelligent and kind collaborator and friend in Ignacio. I am also very grateful to Teresa O’Neill for collaboration, friendship, support, and inspiration. Thanks also to my cohort, and in particular Emily Wilson, for helping make my introduction to the field of Linguistics extra interesting and fun. I’d also like to thank fellow students Lucia Pozzan, Erin Quirk, Ahmed Shamim, Alex Funk, Marisa Nagano, Elizabeth Pratt, Michelle Johnson, Kevin Guzzo, Corbin Neuhauser, Ylana Bleller, Laurie Gluck, Chris Warnasch, Tyler Peckenpaugh, Tally Callahan, Ganga Bhavani, and all the others who provided me with help, support, camaraderie, fun times, and great conversations over the years. Last but definitely not least in the CUNY students category are my peers at Virginia Valian’s Language Acquisition Research Center at Hunter, a community that also included the incredibly helpful Professor Laura Wagner from Ohio State for a time. The LARC community provided crucial support, assistance, and feedback in all of the experimental work I conducted while at CUNY, and I could not have done any of it without their help. I would also like to thank the Appalachian linguists who have helped me, beginning with Greg Johnson. I am very lucky to have made such a wonderful friend in Greg, who regularly makes me laugh so hard it hurts. I am also very grateful to Paul Reed, Michael Montgomery, and Tiffany Williams for their help, their kindness, and their generosity. In addition to these Appalachian linguists, I would also like to thank linguist Neil Myler for his kindness, generosity, and friendship, and for helpful discussion on several different aspects of this thesis. I am also very grateful to Raffaella Zanuttini for ix her time, encouragement, and support at a time when I was just beginning to think about Negative Concord. Thanks also to Beatrice Santorini for taking the time to teach me many useful lessons about corpus research, and to Laura Staum Casasanto for her generous support and advice. I have had the opportunity to present aspects of my research at various conferences, workshops, and talks, and I would like to thank the people whose comments and work encouraged and engaged me, and changed my thinking in very important ways. I name just a small subset of them here: Beatrice Santorini, Anthony Kroch, Stephanie Harves, George Walkden, Alan Munn, Phillip Wallage, Anton Karl Ingasson, Aaron Ecay, Jim Wood, Laura Domínguez, and Elena Herburger.
Recommended publications
  • A Syntactic Analysis of Negation in Latin: Negated Quantifier Phrases
    A Syntactic Analysis of Negation in Latin: Negated Quantifier Phrases Maria C Tierney April 2018 New York University Department of Linguistics Senior Honors Thesis Advisor: Professor Chris Collins Second Reader: Professor Richard Kayne Acknowledgements First and foremost I would like to give innumerable thanks to my advisor and mentor Professor Chris Collins, without whom this thesis would not have been possible. His endless support, assistance, and enthusiasm allowed me to attempt this project and complete it to my best abilities. All of its failings are my own; however, its successes would not have been possible without the guidance and support of Professor Collins. I could not have wished or hoped for a better advisor. Next, I would like to thank the other professors and mentors in New York University’s Department of Linguistics who have helped me reach this point in my academic career. In particular, I would like to thank Richard Kayne, an inspiring scholar and professor who was kind enough to offer me his feedback and encouragement. I would also like to thank Hadas Kotek and Stephanie Harves for their support and guidance, as well as Maria Kouneli, who was an encouraging TA in my very first syntax class. The scholars of syntax in the NYU Department of Linguistics are an amazing, world-class group of individuals, and I feel immeasurably lucky to have had the opportunity to work among them. Outside of the realm of syntax, countless other professors and scholars both inside and outside the NYU Department of Linguistics have offered me their support and guidance.
    [Show full text]
  • ED311449.Pdf
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 311 449 CS 212 093 AUTHOR Baron, Dennis TITLE Declining Grammar--and Other Essays on the English Vocabulary. INSTITUTION National Council of Teachers of English, Urbana, Ill. REPORT NO ISBN-0-8141-1073-8 PUB DATE 89 NOTE :)31p. AVAILABLE FROM National Council of Teachers of English, 1111 Kenyon Rd., Urbana, IL 61801 (Stock No. 10738-3020; $9.95 member, $12.95 nonmember). PUB TYPE Books (010) -- Viewpoints (120) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC10 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *English; Gr&mmar; Higher Education; *Language Attitudes; *Language Usage; *Lexicology; Linguistics; *Semantics; *Vocabulary IDENTIFIERS Words ABSTRACT This book contains 25 essays about English words, and how they are defined, valued, and discussed. The book is divided into four sections. The first section, "Language Lore," examines some of the myths and misconceptions that affect attitudes toward language--and towards English in particular. The second section, "Language Usage," examines some specific questions of meaning and usage. Section 3, "Language Trends," examines some controversial r trends in English vocabulary, and some developments too new to have received comment before. The fourth section, "Language Politics," treats several aspects of linguistic politics, from special attempts to deal with the ethnic, religious, or sex-specific elements of vocabulary to the broader issues of language both as a reflection of the public consciousness and the U.S. Constitution and as a refuge for the most private forms of expression. (MS) *********************************************************************** Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document. *********************************************************************** "PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED BY J. Maxwell TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)." U S.
    [Show full text]
  • Non-Standard English and Dialect Forms
    Non-standard English and dialect forms Teaching notes These notes and answers support the student activities that follow on pp.5-7. Task one: Standard and non-standard sentences All the sentences are examples of non-standard English, and many feature regional dialect forms. Sentence Notes 1 We were stood at the Although this is primarily a regional dialect form common to bus stop for ages. northern England and the Midlands, it is becoming more frequent across the UK. At times this and its equivalent with the verb ‘sit’ e.g. ‘They are sat’ can be heard in places where Standard English might be expected (BBC News on TV/radio for example). A quick online search shows that many people are discussing it (even on Mumsnet), which suggests this is a dialect feature that is spreading. 2 I didn’t do nothing. An example of a double negative, a common feature of a range of regional dialects. 3 They got off of the This might be regarded as an example of language change train at the wrong rather than a distinctive regional dialect feature. Speakers station. seem to consider that the first ‘off’ is part of the verb phrase, and as a result an extra preposition is needed. 4 There are less Again, this is an example of language change in action – opportunities now for think of supermarket checkout signs for ‘10 items or less’. young people than in The distinction between countable and uncountable nouns the previous decade. appears to be changing, with the comparative ‘fewer’ (used for countable nouns) falling out of use.
    [Show full text]
  • A Bi-Clausal Account of English 'To'-Modal Auxiliary Verbs
    A Bi-clausal Account of English ‘to’-Modal Auxiliary Verbs Sungshim Hong Chungnam National University Sungshim Hong. 2014. A Bi-clausal Account of English ‘to’-Modal Auxiliary Verbs. Language and Information 18.1 , 33–52. This paper pro- poses a unified structural account of some instances of the English Modals and Semi-auxiliaries. The classification and the syntactic/structural description of the English Modal auxiliary verbs and verb-related elements have long been the center for many proposals in the history of generative syntax. According to van Gelderen (1993) and Lightfoot (2002), it was sometime around 1380 that the Tense-node (T) appeared in the phrasal structures of the English language, and the T-node is under which the English Modal auxiliaries occupy. Closely related is the existing evidence that English Modals were used as main verbs up to the early sixteenth century (Lightfoot 1991, Han 2000). This paper argues for a bi-clausal approach to English Modal auxiliaries with the infinitival par- ticle ‘to’ such as ‘ought to’ ‘used to’ and ‘dare (to)’ ‘need (to)’, etc. and Semi- auxiliaries including ‘be to’ and ‘have to’. More specifically, ‘ought’ in ‘ought to’ constructions, for instance, undergoes V-to-T movement within the matrix clause, just like ‘HAVEAux’ and all instances of ‘BE’, whereas ‘to’ occupies the T position of the embedded complement clause. By proposing the bi-clausal account, Radford’s (2004, 2009) problems can be solved. Further, the historical motivation for the account takes a stance along with Norde (2009) and Brin- ton & Traugott (2005) in that Radford’s (2004, 2009) syncretization of the two positions of the infinitival particle ‘to’ is no di↵erent from the ‘boundary loss’ in the process of Grammariticalization.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Analysis of Double Negation
    ISSN 1799-2591 Theory and Practice in Language Studies, Vol. 1, No. 10, pp. 1442-1445, October 2011 © 2011 ACADEMY PUBLISHER Manufactured in Finland. doi:10.4304/tpls.1.10.1442-1445 A Constructive Study of English and Chinese Double Negation Fushan Sun Qingdao University of Science and Technology, Qingdao, China Email: [email protected] Abstract—As a universally used linguistic structure, double negation has long attracted the attention of scholars in Chinese and foreign countries. But so far the research of double negation is limited to the study and analysis of negative words, negative markers and the surface structure of double negation, and is also restricted to one particular language. In this essay, based on contrastive linguistics and cognitive linguistics, I launch a comparison between English double negation and its Chinese counterpart from the cognitive perspective to identify similarities and differences between them, and furthermore I also attempt to elaborate the causes and motivations for its meaning. Index Terms—double negation, cognitive, iconicity, M—Principle What is the basis of the sense of double negation? Many linguists are interested in and concentrate on this issue. Palmer and Otto Jespersen are the most typical representatives of these scholars. Palmer said that two negative elements cancel each other to form a positive sense in a sentence. Otto Jespersen (1939) explains "All the languages seem to have a common law, that is, two negative makes a positive". In fact, this theoretical basis used the principle of logic that "two noes mean a yes". As to the issue why double negation can strengthen the.
    [Show full text]
  • “She Didn't Say Nothing About That”. Gender Difference and Similarities in the Use of Negative Concord for the Regional Dialects of England in the BNC
    Department of Humanities English C “She didn't say nothing about that”. Gender difference and similarities in the use of negative concord for the regional dialects of England in the BNC. Roy Stone English C: Essay 4th January 2009 Supervisor: Monika Mondor 1 Table of contents 1. Aim 2. Background 3. Previous research 4. Method 5. Results 6. Discussion 7. Conclusion References Appendix 2 1. Aim The aim of this paper is to investigate differences and similarities in the use of negative concord within English dialects focusing specifically on gender use. The main focus will be to extend the study previously conducted by Anderwald (2002) using the spoken sections which are not pre-planned scripted speeches of the British National Corpus (BNC). I will run a series of searches with the intention of detecting negative concord; thereafter looking at this result for gender trends for each English region. Each geographical area in the BNC will be taken as corresponding to a regional dialect. I will investigate the negative elements ‘-n’t’ and ‘not’ which were part of the investigation covered by Anderwald (2002), comparing this response against the corresponding morpheme any- in order to give a negative meaning to the sentence. This procedure will then be re-run against the non-standard form nothing. In doing so, this procedure will compare the standard grammatical response to non-standard. It is the intention of this study to answer the following questions: Do women use negation concord less frequently than men regionally? Is gender use of negation concord consistent throughout the regions? Are there any significant patterns with regards to gender use regionally of negative concord? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Interpretation of Negation in Mandarin Chinese Feifei Li
    ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en On the interpretation of negation in Mandarin Chinese Feifei Li Ph.D. Dissertation Ph.D. program in Cognitive Science and Langauge Supervisor: M.Teresa Espinal Centre de Lingüística Teòrica Departament de Filologia Catalana Facultat de Filosofia i Lletres 2019 Table of contents Table of contents ........................................................................................................... i Abstract ........................................................................................................................ iv Acknowledgements ..................................................................................................... vi List of Tables ............................................................................................................... ix List of Figures ............................................................................................................... x Abbreviations
    [Show full text]
  • Double Negation in African American Vernacular English and in Standard English: Evidence from Intonation, the History of English and Sociolinguistics
    PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen Double Negation in African American Vernacular English and in Standard English: Evidence from Intonation, the History of English and Sociolinguistics VLS01(04-07-06) 1 PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen The Ban on Double Negation: A Form of Prescriptivism? (1) a. ?This theory ain't make no sense. b. //doesn't make no sense. c. This theory doesn't make any sense. (1a, b) Negative Concord (NC) characterizes African American Vernacular English (AAVE) and other nonstandard varieties. (1c) Simple negation in Standard English (SE) 2 PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen Three Central Questions: 1. How can the distribution of NC in AAVE be described? 2. How can we distinguish between NC and logical double negation? 3. What are the roots of NC in AAVE? 3 PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen Structure of This Talk 1. The distribution of NC and the Allomorph Hypothesis. 2. NC and logical double negation occur with different intonational patterns which influence their respective interpretations. 3. Origins of NC in AAVE: Arguments for the Variationist Hypothesis. 4 PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen 1 Negation in AAVE and in Standard English (SE) 1.1 Negative Concord in AAVE Definition: The term negative concord stems from Mathesius (1937) and describes cases where two or more forms of negation are used to express a single negation in the sentence. 5 PD Dr. Susanne Winkler Universität Tübingen Types of Negative Concord Type 1: NC occurs between the auxiliary and the post-verbal negative indefinite (N-word) of a sentence. (2) a.
    [Show full text]
  • Diction and Idiom Errors: a List of Common Errors in English Usage
    Diction and Idiom Errors: A List of Common Errors in English Usage The errors on this list are, obviously, organized alphabetically. Errors that involve confusion of two or more words or expressions are generally alphabetized according to whichever word comes first alphabetically; “emigrate vs. immigrate,” for example, is listed under emigrate. (Others are listed by the word or expression that is commonly misused, such as “like vs. as,” which is listed under like.) Articles (a, an, the) and the word to in infinitives are disregarded for purposes of alphabetization. Some words or expressions fall under a larger category; “to set vs. to sit,” for example, is listed under the heading “count nouns vs. mass nouns.” Items marked with an asterisk are errors that students whose English is influenced by Chinese should pay special attention to. about vs. around adapt vs. adopt Some grammar fascists insist that around should not To adapt means “to make or become suited (often be used to mean “about” or “approximately”: by alteration)”; to adopt means “to take up for use” “around five hundred people” or “to accept and put into effect.” “around six o‟clock” “freshmen adapting to a new environment” “about five hundred people” “adapted it for use as a weapon” “at about six o‟clock” “adopted a new strategy” “the speaker adopted a tone of urgency” absolute adjectives “adopted the panel‟s recommendations” Some adjectives, including superlative adjectives “adapted the recommendations to fit the new like last and best, along with other adjectives like situation” full, pregnant, perfect, the word absolute itself, and unique (which literally means “one of a kind”), adhere with technically should not be modified by intensifiers adhere to like very and extremely or qualifiers like somewhat “If you do not adhere to the rules, you will be and a little because they indicate qualities of an excommunicated from the Hannah Montana absolute degree.
    [Show full text]
  • Intensifiers in Current English
    Intensifiers in Current English By EDITH MOORE BENZINGER A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1971 TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ABSTRACT iii CHAPTER I. INTENSIFICATION AND THE INTENSIVE QUALIFIER 1 II. HISTORICAL DEVELOPMENT OF THE INTENSIVE QUALIFIER .... 52 III. GRAMMATICAL TREATMENT OF THE INTENSIVE QUALIFIER 101 IV. DISTRIBUTION AND FUNCTION OF THE INTENSIVE QUALIFIER 140 V. CONCLUSION 175 BIBLIOGRAPHY 1^3 BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH 1^7 ii Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate Council of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy INTENSIFIERS IN CURRENT ENGLISH By Edith Moore Benzinger December, 1971 Chairman: Dr. John Algeo Co-Chairman: Dr. Jayne C. Harder Major Department: English It is necessary to separate intensification as a semantic notion from those elements which signal it. The intensive qualifier is perhaps the most common linguistic signal of intensification in current usage. Because it is a discrete lexical unit, it is easily recognized and may be described in terms of its distribution in positions preceding adjectives and adverbs. This study examines the phenomenon of intensification with specific emphasis on the intensive qualifier as a structural class. Attention is given the historical distribution of specific intensive qualifiers, wherein it is shown that these qualifiers lose lexical meaning as they become widely used and that they pass in and out of fashion. As a class they enjoyed a high degree of popularity during the nineteenth century. Despite their widespread use, however, grammatical iii treatment of intensive qualifiers has been largely inade- quate.
    [Show full text]
  • Not Unreasonable: Why Two Negatives Don't Make a Positive
    Not unreasonable: Why two negatives don’t make a positive Michael Henry Tessler1,3 & Michael Franke2 1 Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences 2 University of Osnabrück, Department of Cognitive Sciences 3 Stanford University, Department of Psychology Abstract Logic tells us that two negatives make a positive, but in language, things are not so black and white: A person not unhappy may not be entirely happy. We hypothesize that innovative uses of double negatives like not unhappy stem from listeners entertaining flexible meanings for negation markers like not and un-, which context can then help disambiguate. We formalize this hypothesis in a computational model of language understanding, which predicts that not unhappy means something different than happy but also makes the additional prediction that single negations (unhappy vs. not happy) are interpreted identically except when a speaker uses both in the same context, which we confirm experimentally. Even double negations that flagrantly use the same negation marker twice (e.g., not not happy) are interpreted in subtle ways. These findings suggest that even one of the most logical elements of language—negation—can mean many things at once. Keywords: semantics; pragmatics; negation; Bayesian cognitive model; Ratio- nal Speech Act This work was supported in part by NSF Graduate Research Fellowship DGE-114747 to MHT. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael Henry Tessler, 43 Vassar St, Cambridge, MA 02139. E-mail: [email protected] FLEXIBLE NEGATION IN LANGUAGE 2 I’ve been waiting for you all week. Don’t you know that it is rotten time to not not be me? (El-P, “Works every time (feat.
    [Show full text]
  • The Semantic View of Translating Litotes from English Into Arabic
    J J. Edu. Sci., Vol. (19) No. (5) 2012 K The Semantic View of Translating Litotes from English into Arabic Dhuha Ghanim Mohammed Mohammed Nihad Ahmed College of Pharmacy College of Dentistry University of Mosul University of Mosul Received Accepted 09 / 05 / 2011 05 / 10 / 2011 ﺍﻟﺨﻼﺼﺔ ﻴﻌﻨﻰ ﺍﻟﺒﺤﺙ ﺒﺎﻟﺘﺤﻠﻴل ﺍﻟﺩﻻﻟﻲ ﻟﺘﺭﺠﻤﺔ ﺃﺴﻠﻭﺏ ﺍﻟﺘﻘﻠﻴل ، ﻜﺼﻴﻐﺔ ﺒﻼﻏﻴﺔ . ﻓﻔـﻲ ﺃﺴـﻠﻭﺏ ﺍﻟﺘﻘﻠﻴل، ﻴﺅﺩﻱ ﺍﻟﺴﻴﺎﻕ ﺩﻭﺭﺍ ﺭﺌﻴﺴﺎ ﻓﻲ ﺍﻟﺘﺄﺜﻴﺭ ﻋﻠﻰ ﺍﻹﺩﺭﺍﻙ ﺍﻟﻜﻠﻲ ﻟﻤﻌﻨﻰ ﺘﻌﺎﺒﻴﺭ ﺍﻟﺘﻘﻠﻴـل . ﻜﻤـﺎ ﻭﺘﺭﻯ ﺍﻟﺩﺭﺍﺴﺔ ﺃﻥ ﺍﻟﻅﺎﻫﺭﺓ ﺍﻟﺤﻀﺎﺭﻴﺔ ﺘﺄﺨﺫ ﺩﻭﺭﻫﺎ ﺃﻴﻀﺎ ﻓﻲ ﺘﺤﺩﻴﺩ ﺍﻟﻤﻌﻨﻰ ﺍﻟﻜﻠﻲ ﻟﺴﻴﺎﻕ ﺍﻟﻨﺹ . ﻜﻤﺎ ﻭﻴﻬﺘﻡ ﺍﻟﺒﺤﺙ ﺒﺩﺭﺍﺴﺔ ﺤﺎﻟﺔ ﺍﻟﻨﻔﻲ ﺍﻟﻤﺯﺩﻭﺝ ﻋﻨﺩﻤﺎ ﻴﺘﻡ ﺍﺴﺘﺨﺩﺍﻡ ﺼﻴﻐﺘﻴﻥ ﻟﻠﻨﻔﻲ ﻓـﻲ ﻋﺒـﺎﺭﺓ ﻭﺍﺤﺩﺓ. ﻓﻔﻲ ﻤﻨﻁﻕ ﺒﻌﺽ ﺍﻟﻠﻐﺎﺕ ، ﻴﻠﻐﻲ ﻜل ﻤﻥ ﺼﻴﻐﺘﻲ ﺍﻟﻨﻔﻲ ﺇﺤﺩ ﺍﻫﻤﺎ ﺍﻵﺨﺭ ﻟﺘﻘﺩﻴﻡ ﻤﻌﻨﻰ ﻤﺜﺒﺘﺎ . ﻭﻓﻲ ﻟﻐﺎﺕ ﺃﺨﺭﻯ ﻴﺅﻜﺩ ﺍﻟﻨﻔﻴﺎﻥ ﺒﻌﻀ ﻴﻬﻤﺎ ﺍﻟﺒﻌﺽ . ﻭﺍﻟﻤﺼﻁﻠﺢ ﺍﻟﺒﻼﻏﻲ ﻟﻬﺫﺍ ﺍﻷﺜﺭ ﻋﻨﺩﻤﺎ ﻴـﺅﺩﻱ ﺇﻟﻰ ﻓﻬﻡ ﺍﻹﺜﺒﺎﺕ، ﻫﻭ ﺃﺴﻠﻭﺏ ﺍﻟﺘﻘﻠﻴل. Abstract This study deals with the semantic analysis of translating litotes, the rhetorical figure of speech. In litotes, context plays an essential role in determining the overall understanding of the meaning of litotes expressions. The study hypothesizes that the cultural manifestation also plays an essential role to discriminate the meaning of the overall context of the text. Also the paper deals with the case of double negation when two forms of negation are used in the same clause. In some logics and some languages, double negatives cancel each other out and produce an affirmative sense. In other languages, double negatives intensify the negation. The rhetorical term for this effect, when it leads to understand affirmation, is litotes. 1. Litotes: The Rationale Litotes is a rhetorical figure of speech in which an affirmative is expressed by the negative of the contrary.
    [Show full text]