Evolutionary Psychology in the Round Robin Dunbar and Louise Barrett

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Evolutionary Psychology in the Round Robin Dunbar and Louise Barrett 01-Dunbar-Chap01 12/22/06 3:40 PM Page 3 CHAPTER 1 Evolutionary psychology in the round Robin Dunbar and Louise Barrett 1.1. Introduction questions that biologists can ask, the other two being questions about function (a teleonomic Although the closing decades of the nineteenth question that is, ultimately, about the genetic con- century witnessed a great deal of interchange sequences of behaviour) and phylogeny (the evo- between psychology and evolutionary biology, lutionary history of how a behaviour came to the hasty departure of James Baldwin from the have its current form). Tinbergen (1963) pointed American psychological scene in 1908 (thanks to out that all four provide appropriate answers to the scandal of being caught in a brothel) trig- the generic question ‘Why does an animal do X?’; gered, for reasons that are more political than sci- all must in the end be answered for a full under- entific, the virtual severance of contact between standing, but each can be asked and answered the two disciplines (Plotkin, 2004). Psychology independently. That is to say, our answer to one became more heavily influenced by its alternate does not necessarily commit us to any particular roots in physiology and the neurosciences, on the answer for any of the others. one hand, and the social sciences, on the other. We want to make two general points in this Evolution played no significant role in psychology chapter. First, the evolutionary approach neces- for the better part of a century. Indeed, the 1950s sarily enjoins us to take a broad disciplinary per- in particular witnessed a somewhat tetchy quarrel spective to our subject matter (which, following between the comparative psychologists (with Heyes (2000), we might refer to as ‘evolutionary their feet firmly planted in behaviourism and an psychology in the round’). Second, an evolution- experimental paradigm) and the ethologists (with ary view does not—and should not—commit us their roots in evolutionary biology, and a focus on to any particular assumptions about the genetic the observational study of an animal’s behaviour determination of behaviour. Indeed, learning, in its natural environment). and by extension cultural transmission, play an In part, this drift away from biology reflected an especially important role in the behaviour of increasing focus within academic psychology on humans, and we will never be able to understand questions of mechanism (with a broad focus on human behaviour without understanding cul- stimulus–response processes, motivation, cogni- ture and the way it influences what humans do. tion and, later, neuropsychology) and develop- These issues are explored in more detail else- ment. For the ethologists, these constituted just where in this volume, but our main concern here two of what eventually came to be known as is to provide a framework within which the ‘Tinbergen’s Four Why’s’—the four kinds of chapters that follow can be understood. 01-Dunbar-Chap01 12/22/06 3:40 PM Page 4 4·CHAPTER 1 Evolutionary psychology in the round 1.2. Asking the right questions versus ontogenetic (i.e. developmental) questions. Questions about genetic determinism belong to The important consideration in the present con- the realm of ontogeny (how the individual text is that evolutionary (or Darwinian) theory acquires its capacities during the course of devel- provides a framework within which a diverse opment), but the core to an evolutionary approach range of intellectual questions can be integrated. lies in function (questions about the evolution- The significance of this is well illustrated by the ary goal-directedness of these capacities and the role it has played within biology. A century ago, behaviour they make possible). biology consisted of half a dozen or more quite Of course, as an essentially biological question, separate disciplines (anatomy, zoology, botany, the primary focus of functional questions lies in genetics, physiology, microbiology, biochem- how an organism’s behaviour maximizes its istry, etc.) that rarely interacted. The gradual genetic fitness. But the fact that an individual acts acceptance of the theory of evolution as a central so as to maximize its fitness does not mean that organizing principle has made it possible for its behaviour is genetically determined, merely these diverse interest groups to talk to each other that it has a set of genetically inherited motiva- in a common language in a way that had rarely tions (or goal states) that it seeks to satisfy. How been possible in the past. it achieves those goal states will, at least in neu- Our claim here is that an evolutionary approach rologically advanced species, depend on the can and should do the same for psychology. individual’s assessment of the costs and benefits Evolutionary psychology, we argue, is not a new of acting in one way rather than another, given its and separate sub-discipline within psychology, experience of the world. In evolutionary biology, but rather a framework theory that allows psy- every decision is a contingent one that depends chology’s many diverse sub-disciplines to be on the details of the context. That context will integrated into a unitary whole. It is not our obviously include many features of the physical intention to demonstrate this claim here by environment, but in highly social species like showing how different psychological approaches humans it will also include the social environ- could relate more effectively to each other. ment. We return to this point again below. The Rather, our aim is simply to make it clear that a issue here, however, is that the genes that are developmental stance, for example, is not differ- passed on from one generation to another need ent from, or in intellectual opposition to, an be not the genes for a particular behaviour, but evolutionary approach. Rather, an evolutionary may, rather, be the genes for a brain that is large perspective adds to a developmental approach enough, and complex enough, to make the deci- by offering new ways of seeing development, sions about how best to act in order to satisfy prompting novel questions for empirical study, its motivations (thereby maximizing fitness). and, more broadly, allowing developmental psy- Questions about the roles of genes and the envi- chologists to integrate their findings with those ronment in the production of those brains, or of neuropsychologists, cognitive psychologists any other aspect of the system, are of course and others. Life history theory, for example, is interesting, but they remain quite separate, and major feature of contemporary evolutionary are unaffected by the extent to which an individ- ecology, with enormous relevance both to repro- ual can be shown to be maximizing its fitness. ductive decision-making and to development. The issue of ontogeny is, of course, an impor- Yet, its implications have only recently begun to tant one that has been the focus of yet another be explored. Several of the chapters in Section V long-running debate within psychology in par- draw on it in their explorations of different ticular (the so-called nature/nurture debate). aspects of human reproductive behaviour. Biologists have largely accepted, since the 1960s, In this context, it is particularly important to that this distinction is arbitrary and, worse still understand that an evolutionary approach does perhaps, misguided. We cannot separate genes not commit us ipso facto to genetic determinism. from the environment in the simple-minded To assume that it does is to commit a classic cat- way implied by this dichotomy. Both nature and egory mistake by failing to distinguish two of nurture are deeply implicated in the processes of Tinbergen’s Four Why’s—functional questions development, even though it may be possible to 01-Dunbar-Chap01 12/22/06 3:40 PM Page 5 Asking the right questions · 5 discuss the magnitude of the relative contribu- rump on the periphery where they continued to tion of genetic versus environmental effects to focus on somewhat arcane topics like intelligence the differences between individuals. Nonetheless, and personality theory. The rise of evolutionary no aspect of an organism’s biology or psychology ideas in the study of animal behaviour during can be said to be wholly (or even mainly) due to the 1970s (originally in the form of a sub- its genes or the environment in which it grows up. discipline that named itself sociobiology, but The distinction, nonetheless, is important in which later adopted the alternative name behav- one key respect. If we recognize that questions ioural ecology) seems to have been viewed by the about the mechanisms of inheritance are separate naturists as offering something of a bulwark from questions about the evolutionary function against the nurturists, in part at least because it of behaviour, then the way is opened up for the seemed to imply some form of genetic basis for evolutionary study of culture as an important cognition and behaviour. phenomenon in its own right. Self-evidently, cul- This may, in turn, provide us with a way ture depends on learning—specifically the social of articulating the equally fractionated sub- transmission of beliefs or rules of behaviour— disciplines of evolutionary psychology. Since the but learning is simply another mechanism of mid-1990s, evolutionary psychologists have inheritance in the grand evolutionary scheme. been embroiled in what might seem like an Biologists’ perennial focus on genes (and, more internecine war between those whose intellec- recently, DNA) as the mechanism of inheritance tual tradition lay within behavioural ecology is, perhaps, to be expected given their interest in (who sometimes refer to themselves as evolu- the more hard-wired aspects of biology, such tionary anthropologists) and those whose intel- as anatomy. But it is crucial to remember that lectual tradition lay within psychology (who neither Darwin nor Mendel (widely considered originally referred to themselves as Darwinian to be the founding father of genetics) actually psychologists, but later co-opted the term ‘evo- knew anything about genes or DNA.
Recommended publications
  • Leadership, Followership, and Evolution Some Lessons from the Past
    Leadership, Followership, and Evolution Some Lessons From the Past Mark Van Vugt University of Kent Robert Hogan Hogan Assessment Systems Robert B. Kaiser Kaplan DeVries Inc. This article analyzes the topic of leadership from an evo- Second, the literature focuses on leaders and tends to lutionary perspective and proposes three conclusions that ignore the essential role of followers (Hollander, 1992; are not part of mainstream theory. First, leading and Yukl, 2006). Third, research largely concentrates on prox- following are strategies that evolved for solving social imate issues of leadership (e.g., What makes one person a coordination problems in ancestral environments, includ- better leader than others?) and rarely considers its ultimate ing in particular the problems of group movement, intra- functions (e.g., How did leadership promote survival and group peacekeeping, and intergroup competition. Second, reproductive success among our ancestors?) (R. Hogan & the relationship between leaders and followers is inher- Kaiser, 2005). Finally, there has been little cross-fertiliza- ently ambivalent because of the potential for exploitation of tion between psychology and disciplines such as anthro- followers by leaders. Third, modern organizational struc- pology, economics, neuroscience, biology, and zoology, tures are sometimes inconsistent with aspects of our which also contain important insights about leadership evolved leadership psychology, which might explain the (Bennis, 2007; Van Vugt, 2006). alienation and frustration of many citizens and employees. This article offers a view of leadership inspired by The authors draw several implications of this evolutionary evolutionary theory, which modern scholars increasingly analysis for leadership theory, research, and practice. see as essential for understanding social life (Buss, 2005; Lawrence & Nohria, 2002; McAdams & Pals, 2006; Nettle, Keywords: evolution, leadership, followership, game the- 2006; Schaller, Simpson, & Kenrick, 2006).
    [Show full text]
  • The Adaptive Significance of Human Language
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 College Scholars 2000 The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2 Recommended Citation Oesch, Nathan, "The Adaptive Significance of Human Language" (2000). Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2/52 This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the College Scholars at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Department of Psychology University of Tennessee, Knoxville noesch @ utk. edu Abstract Many experts have argued that human language is fundamentally incompatible with the principles of traditional Darwinian evolutionary theory. According to conventional Darwinian explanations, specific traits evolved among species according to gradual and incremental genetic changes, each of which that were in some way so favorable to the survival and reproduction of ancestral generations that they were ultimately preserved within successive generations of those species. Human language, it has been said, is simply to complex to be explained as a result of Darwinian explanations, since each successive step in the evolution of language would confer no obvious survival benefits to its recipients. According to this idea, language is such an "all-or­ none system," that it could not possibly have existed in any immediately beneficial intermediate forms and thus could not have evolved according to conventional Darwinian modes of explanation.
    [Show full text]
  • Integrative Anthropology and the Human Niche: Toward a Contemporary Approach to Human Evolution
    AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST Integrative Anthropology and the Human Niche: Toward a Contemporary Approach to Human Evolution Agustın´ Fuentes ABSTRACT A niche is the structural, temporal, and social context in which a species exists. Over the last two million years, the human lineage underwent clear morphological changes alongside less easily measurable, but significant, behavioral and cognitive shifts as it forged, and was shaped by, new niches. During this time period, core human patterns emerged, including the following: hypercooperation; lengthy childhood and complex parenting; intricate and diverse foraging and hunting patterns; novel and dynamic material and symbolic cultures; and complex communication and information sharing, eventually resulting in language. Approaches to human evolution grounded in paleoanthropology and archaeology offer fundamental insights into our past, and traditional evolutionary the- ory offers a strong grounding for explaining them. However, given the centrality of distinctive physiological, social, semiotic, and cognitive processes in human evolutionary histories, a broader anthropological approach can facilitate additional understanding of the human story. An integrative anthropology, reaching across subfields and foci, com- bined with contemporary evolutionary theory is an approach that can enhance our abilities to model and understand human evolution. [integrative anthropology, niche construction, evolution, extended evolutionary synthesis, Homo, semiosis, Pleistocene] RESUMEN Un nicho es el contexto estructural,
    [Show full text]
  • Assortative Mating, Autistic Traits, Empathizing, and Systemizing
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.10.28.358895; this version posted October 28, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-ND 4.0 International license. Assortative Mating, Autistic Traits, Empathizing, and Systemizing Gareth Richards1,2*, Simon Baron-Cohen2, Holly Stokes3, Varun Warrier2, Ben Mellor1, Ellie Winspear1, Jessica Davies3, Laura Gee3, John Galvin3 1 School of Psychology, Faculty of Medical Sciences, Newcastle University, UK 2 Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, UK 3 Department of Psychology, Birmingham City University, UK * Corresponding author address: School of Psychology, Newcastle University, 2.27 Ridley Building 1, Queen Victoria Road, Newcastle upon Tyne, UK; email: [email protected] Abstract It has been suggested that the children of parents with particular interests and aptitude for understanding systems via input-operation-output rules (i.e. systemizing) are at increased likelihood of developing autism. Furthermore, assortative mating (i.e. a non-random pattern in which individuals are more likely to pair with others who are similar to themselves) is hypothesised to occur in relation to systemizing, and so romantic couples may be more similar on this variable than chance would dictate. However, no published study has yet tested this hypothesis. We therefore examined intra-couple correlations for a measure of autistic traits (Autism Spectrum Quotient [AQ]), self-report measures of empathizing (Empathy Quotient [EQ]), and systemizing (Systemizing Quotient-Revised [SQ-R]), as well as the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) and Embedded Figures Task (EFT).
    [Show full text]
  • Volume 3, Numbers 1, 2005 JÁNOS LÁSZLÓ AKADÉMIAI KIADÓ
    Volume 3, Numbers 1, 2005 Editor-in-Chief JÁNOS LÁSZLÓ University of Pécs Editors TAMÁS BERECZKEI and CSABA PLÉH University of Pécs/Budapest University of Technology and Economics Managing Editor ANITA VELÕSY Editorial Board GERALD C. CUPCHIK (University of Toronto) DANEL C. DENNETT (Tufts University) ROBIN DUNBAR (University of Liverpool) JOSEPH P. FORGAS (University of New South Wales, Sydney) PAUL HERNADI (University of California, Santa Barbara) SANDRA JOVCHELOVICH (London School of Economics) COLIN MARTINDALE (University of Maine) BORIS VELICHKOVSKY (University of Dresden) ECKART VOLAND (University of Giessen) WOLFGANG WAGNER (University of Linz) AKADÉMIAI KIADÓ, BUDAPEST 2 JCEP 1(2003)1 JCEP 1(2003)1 JCEP 1(2003)1 3 CONTENTS ROBIN I. M. DUNBAR: Why Are Good Writers So Rare? An Evolutionary Perspective on Literature ....................................................................................000 NANCY EASTERLIN: How to Write the Great Darwinian Novel: Cognitive Predispositions, Cultural Complexity, and Aesthetic Evaluation ......................000 DANIEL NETTLE: The Wheel of Fire and the Mating Game: Explaining the Origins of Tragedy and Comedy .......................................................................000 JAMES STILLER and MATTHEW HUDSON: Weak Links and Scene Cliques Within the Small World of Shakespeare ........................................................................000 PAUL MATTHEWS and LOUISE BARRETT: Small-screne Social Groups: Soap Operas and Social Networks ..............................................................................000
    [Show full text]
  • Thinking Big How the Evolution of Social Life Shaped the Human Mind Dr
    AL’S BOOK CLUB DECEMBER 2014 Thinking Big How the Evolution of Social Life Shaped the Human Mind Dr. Robin Dunbar Those of you familiar with the CIMBA Leadership System know that we prescribe heavily to something we referred to internally as the Social Brain Theory of Leadership. Several of the books we have reviewed in this column have both supported and extended this core theory. From Dr. Leslie Brothers' Friday's Footprint (1997), which arguably predated social neuroscience, to Dr. Matt Lieberman's Social (2013), who is arguably the father of social neuroscience, the evidence supports our two most fundamental tenets: (1) The human brain is wired to be social; and, (2) Leadership is a social event. One of the most important lessons we have learned at CIMBA as we developed and tested our behavioral models, and the technology supporting their implementation in the workplace and beyond, is the importance of looking beyond neuroscience and social psychology to encompass other fields that have much to offer in supporting and solidifying our Social Brain Theory of Leadership. In all truthfulness, perhaps the most important step was the decision to move beyond the traditional bounds of leadership as it was then understood in the early 90s. An important contributor to our thinking was the anthropologist Dr. Robin Dunbar. Dr. Dunbar is recognized for building evidence in support of the core notion that the human brain got bigger, it evolved, in order to manage its demanding social responsibilities - his Social Brain Hypothesis. In the book, Thinking Big: How the Evolution of Social Life Shaped the Human Mind, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • The Collective Invention of Language to Access the Universe of Possible
    Commentary/Carruthers: The cognitive functions of language late the mental states of others. Indeed, this is what human com- likely to group, say, a cedar canoe (chem) with an aluminum mo- munication seems to be mostly about. Although such communi- torboat (laanchaj) than with a cedar tree (k’uche’). Frequently cation is clearly mediated by a public language (the product of a cited differences in spatial reasoning (e.g., more cardinal than rel- language module), “the very development of public language is ative positioning because of lack of prepositions in the language) not the cause, but an effect of the development of communication may not be much greater between languages and cultures than be- made possible by the metarepresentational mode.” tween, say, New Yorkers (lousy at cardinal positioning) and rural Carruthers offers a similar solution to Fodor’s Paradox; only, he Midwesterners (who talk about “the north side of the barn”). Even attributes evolved metamodular functions to the language faculty, a near-prepositionless Australian aboriginal wouldn’t likely refer not ToM. Carruthers’s insights about the role of language in to the mole on the right side of the nose only in cardinal terms thought are enlightening (at least to me); his suggestions for fur- (which would imply change of reference with every head move- ther experiments to test claims about how language integrates ment). thoughts across domains are important and should be seriously pursued. But I see nothing here to allow a decision on whether ACKNOWLEDGMENTS language or ToM is ultimately responsible for cross-modular inte- Thanks to Noam Chomsky and Robin Dunbar for information cited.
    [Show full text]
  • Robin Ian Macdonald DUNBAR
    R.I.M. Dunbar Publications R.I.M. Dunbar PUBLICATIONS To date, I have published 20 authored or edited books or special journal issues, over 400 articles in scientific journals, 7 technical reports on commercially commissioned research, and over some 100 pieces of science journalism and 130 book reviews. Academic Books 1975 Dunbar, R. & Dunbar, P.: Social Dynamics of Gelada Baboons. Karger, Basel. 1982 Bertram, B., Clutton-Brock, T., Dunbar, R., Rubenstein, R. & Wrangham, R. (eds): Current Problems in Sociobiology. Cambridge Univeristy Press. 1984 Dunbar, R.: Reproductive Decisions: An Economic Analysis of Gelada Baboon Social Strategies. Princeton University Press. 1988 Dunbar, R.: Primate Social Systems. Chapman & Hall, London [Cornell Univ Press in USA] 1995 Dunbar, R. (ed): Human Reproductive Decisions: Biological and Social Perspectives. Macmillan. 1996 Maynard Smith, J., Runciman, G. & Dunbar, R. (eds): Evolution of Culture and Language in Primates and Humans. British Academy Press & Oxford University Press. 1999 Dunbar, R., Knight, C. & Power, C. (eds) The Evolution of Culture. Edinburgh University Press. [Arabic and Chinese translations] 2000 Cowlishaw, G. & Dunbar, R.: Primate Conservation Biology. Chicago University Press. 2002 Barrett, L., Dunbar, R. & Lycett, J.: Human Evolutionary Psychology. Macmillan/Palgrave and Princeton University Press. [with Czech and Japanese translations]. 2005 Dunbar, R. (guest editor): Darwinism and Literature. Special issue of J. Cult. Evol. Psych. Vol.3 (1): 1-108. 2007 Dunbar, R. & Barrett, L. (eds): Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford Univ Press. 2008 Allen, N.J., Callan, H., Dunbar, R. & James, W. (eds): Early Human Kinship: From Sex to Social Reproduction. Blackwell. 2010 Dunbar, R., Gamble, C. & Gowlett, J.
    [Show full text]
  • Bibliografía
    No part of this book may be distributed, posted, or reproduced in any form by FIRST PROOFS - © Copyright 2017 digital or mechanical means without NOT SUITABLE FOR TRANSLATION Princeton University Press. prior written permission of the publisher. Bibliografía Abbe, Emmanuel A., Amir E. Khandani, and Andrew W. Lo. 2012. “Privacy- Preserving Methods for Sharing Financial Risk Exposures.” American Economic Review 102, no. 3: 65– 70. Acharya, Viral V., Lasse Pedersen, Th omas Philippon, and Matthew Richardson. 2009. “Regulating Systemic Risk.” In Restoring Financial Stability: How to Repair a Failed System, edited by Viral V. Acharya and Matthew Richardson, 283– 303. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Adolphs, Ralph, Daniel Tranel, Hanna Damasio, and Antonio R. Damasio. 1994. “Im- paired Recognition of Emotion in Facial Expressions Following Bilateral Damage to the Human Amygdala.” Nature 372: 669– 672. Alchian, Armen. 1950. “Uncertainty, Evolution and Economic Th eory.” Journal of Po- litical Economy 58: 211– 221. Alexander, Sidney S. 1961. “Price Movements in Speculative Markets: Trends or Ran- dom Walks.” Industrial Management Review 2: 7– 26. American Cancer Society. 2016. Cancer Facts and Figures 2016. Atlanta, GA: Ameri- can Cancer Society. Andersen, Espen S. 1994. Evolutionary Economics: Post- Schumpeterian Contribu- tions. London, UK: Pinter. Anderson, Philip W., Kenneth J. Arrow, and David Pines, eds. 1988. Th e Economy as an Evolving Complex System. Reading, MA: Addison- Wesley. Andrews, Edmund L. 2008. “Greenspan Concedes Error on Regulation.” New York Times, October 23. Aristotle. 1944. Aristotle in 23 Volumes. Vol. 21. Translated by Harris Rackham. Cam- bridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Arrow, Kenneth J. 1964. “Th e Role of Securities in the Optimal Allocation of Risk- bearing.” Review of Economic Studies 31: 91– 96.
    [Show full text]
  • MONREPOS Archaeological Research Centre Honours Evolutionary Psychologist Robin Dunbar with International Research Award
    Press release | 31 July 2019 HUMAN ROOTS AWARD 2019 MONREPOS Archaeological Research Centre honours evolutionary psychologist Robin Dunbar with international research award Neuwied / Mainz. On November 8th, this year's HUMAN ROOTS AWARD will be awarded for the third time by the Archaeological Research Centre and Museum for Human Behavioural Evolution MONREPOS, a department of the Römisch-Germanisches Zentralmuseum Mainz (RGZM), Leibniz Research Institute of Archaeology. This year's winner is Prof. Dr. Robin Dunbar, evolutionary psychologist at Oxford University. The MONREPOS jury justified the choice of Dunbar with his outstanding achievements into understanding human sociality. Dunbar is the author of numerous articles and popular science books. Along with many other fundamental scientific works, Dunbar's outstanding recognition is that cognitive boundaries for maintaining stable social contacts are set within the human brain. Thus, on average, each person maintains about 150 stable relationships with their fellow human beings – this number is known as the "Dunbar Number". This number exceeds those in the social environment of other vertebrates, yet many problems inherent to large, complex-structured societies may result from this limitation of our social capacities. The HUMAN ROOTS AWARD honours scientists from various disciplines for their contributions to the understanding of human behavioural evolution. The prize has been awarded by MONREPOS since 2017 and is now regarded as the "small Nobel Prize" in archaeology and the evolution of human behaviour, as Professor Sabine Gaudzinski-Windheuser, head of the MONREPOS research centre, points out. The international research prize is endowed with 10,000 Euros and stands for the promotion of interdisciplinary scientific dialogue.
    [Show full text]
  • Oxford Handbooks Online (
    The evolution of empathizing and systemizing: assortative mating of two strong systemizers Oxfordand the Handbooks cause of autism Online The evolution of empathizing and systemizing: assortative mating of two strong systemizers and the cause of autism Simon Baron-Cohen Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Edited by Louise Barrett and Robin Dunbar Print Publication Date: Apr Subject: Psychology, Cognitive Psychology, Cognitive 2007 Neuroscience Online Publication Date: Sep DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568308.013.0016 2012 Abstract and Keywords This article discusses two cognitive processes, empathising and systemising, in terms of sex differences and the implications of these for evolutionary psychology. It also discusses the theory that assortative mating of two strong systemisers is a cause of autism, a neurodevelopmental condition involving empathy impairment alongside hypersystemising. Empathising is the drive to identify another person's emotions and thoughts, and to respond to these with an appropriate emotion. Empathy is a skill (or a set of skills). It defines mindreading as the ability to interpret one's own or another agent's actions as driven by mental states. The model in the article shows the ontogenesis of a theory of mind in the first four years of life, and justifies the existence of four components on the basis of developmental competence and neuropsychological dissociation. Keywords: cognitive processes, evolutionary psychology, assortative mating, systemisers, autism, emotion, empathy, mindreading, ontogenesis, neuropsychological dissociation 16.1. Introduction In this chapter, I discuss two cognitive processes, empathizing and systemizing, in terms of sex differences and the implications of these for evolutionary psychology. I then discuss the theory that assortative mating of two strong systemizers is a cause of autism, Page 1 of 27 PRINTED FROM OXFORD HANDBOOKS ONLINE (www.oxfordhandbooks.com).
    [Show full text]
  • Outline for Biology Course 140, Classic Readings in Animal Behavior
    Science Course 634: The Human Animal Professor Joyce Powzyk Office: Hall-Atwater Room 101 Phone and e-mail: 860-685-3391 [email protected] Lecture 1: From Oceans to The Trees-Evolutionary Scenarios/ Selection Pressures on Primate Evolution (Predation-Mates-Food-Pathogens) Natural Selection and the Genetics of Inheritance/ Adaptations/ Mitosis & Meiosis/ Phenotype & Genotype/ Variation & Mutations/ Mitochondrial Genetics & Y Chromosomal Genetics Lecture 2: Ancestral Primates & Primate Speciation/ Primate Traits/ Primate Invasions & Distributions/ Paleoanthropology/ Extant Primates/ Primate Development/ Instinct vs. Learned Behaviors/ Harlow Experiments/ Sexual Dimorphism & Monomorphic Species/ Paedomorphism Lecture 3: Human Lineage/ Australopithecines-Homo habilis-Homo erectus-Homo neanderthalensis-Homo floresiensis (‘hobbit’)-Homo sapiens/ Bipedalism & pelvis/ Foot Arch, Back Arch, Pelvis, Lumbar Curve & Clavicle/ Early Hominids in Africa/ Australopithecines-Taung Child Insights/ Migration out of Africa/ Lecture 4: Other Aspects of Hominid Anatomy- Brain Size-Neoteny/ Disease Vectors- Immune System Response/ Tool Use in Nonhuman Primates and Human Primates, Opposable and Semi-Opposable Thumb-Tactile/ Hair Loss/ Male-Male Competition/ Fighting and the Human Hand/ Female Competition/ Fertility Cues- Waist to Hip Ratio/ Concealed Ovulation/ Father At Home Theory Lecture 5: Human Evolution Movie/ Darwin & Wallace/ Case Study of ‘Ice Man’ from Italy/ Piltdown Man/ Politics of Human Evolution/ Interbreeding with Neanderthals and Neanderthal
    [Show full text]