Leadership, Followership, and Evolution Some Lessons from the Past

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Leadership, Followership, and Evolution Some Lessons from the Past Leadership, Followership, and Evolution Some Lessons From the Past Mark Van Vugt University of Kent Robert Hogan Hogan Assessment Systems Robert B. Kaiser Kaplan DeVries Inc. This article analyzes the topic of leadership from an evo- Second, the literature focuses on leaders and tends to lutionary perspective and proposes three conclusions that ignore the essential role of followers (Hollander, 1992; are not part of mainstream theory. First, leading and Yukl, 2006). Third, research largely concentrates on prox- following are strategies that evolved for solving social imate issues of leadership (e.g., What makes one person a coordination problems in ancestral environments, includ- better leader than others?) and rarely considers its ultimate ing in particular the problems of group movement, intra- functions (e.g., How did leadership promote survival and group peacekeeping, and intergroup competition. Second, reproductive success among our ancestors?) (R. Hogan & the relationship between leaders and followers is inher- Kaiser, 2005). Finally, there has been little cross-fertiliza- ently ambivalent because of the potential for exploitation of tion between psychology and disciplines such as anthro- followers by leaders. Third, modern organizational struc- pology, economics, neuroscience, biology, and zoology, tures are sometimes inconsistent with aspects of our which also contain important insights about leadership evolved leadership psychology, which might explain the (Bennis, 2007; Van Vugt, 2006). alienation and frustration of many citizens and employees. This article offers a view of leadership inspired by The authors draw several implications of this evolutionary evolutionary theory, which modern scholars increasingly analysis for leadership theory, research, and practice. see as essential for understanding social life (Buss, 2005; Lawrence & Nohria, 2002; McAdams & Pals, 2006; Nettle, Keywords: evolution, leadership, followership, game the- 2006; Schaller, Simpson, & Kenrick, 2006). We argue first ory, mismatch hypothesis that an evolutionary approach to leadership raises some important new questions. Next we analyze the implications hy is leadership important? During times of of leader–follower relations in early human and nonhuman peace and prosperity, it seems not to matter. societies for theories of leadership. We use (evolutionary) WHowever, when politicians start wars, when game theory to model the emergence of leadership; this business leaders gamble with our life savings, and when model is followed by a hypothetical account of how lead- religious leaders create violent sectarian divides, leadership ership developed over four stages of evolutionary history. becomes a matter of life and death. We conclude with some novel implications of this analysis We know a lot about leadership (Bass, 1990; House & for leadership theory, research, and practice. Aditya, 1997; Yukl, 2006). It is a universal feature of human societies and affects the quality of life of citizens in An Evolutionary Analysis of important ways (Brown, 1991; R. Hogan, Curphy, & Leadership Hogan, 1994). When people are placed in ad hoc laboratory groups, leader–follower structures quickly emerge (Bales, Researchers define leadership in many ways (Stogdill, 1951; Mann, 1959; Van Vugt & De Cremer, 1999). Hu- 1974). We define it broadly in terms of (a) influencing mans easily recognize leadership potential in others (Lord, individuals to contribute to group goals and (b) coordinat- DeVader, & Alliger, 1986). People also romanticize lead- ership; we often attribute great importance to leaders even Mark Van Vugt, Robert Hogan, and Robert B. Kaiser contributed equally when it is not warranted (Hackman & Wageman, 2007; to this article. Meindl, Ehrlich, & Dukerich, 1985). Leadership is an un- We are indebted to Rob Kurzban for his intellectual input and to avoidable theme in society and arguably the most important Robin Dunbar, Dominic Johnson, Muhammad Ghufran Ahmad, and Peter Richerson for their comments on previous versions. We also acknowledge problem in the social sciences. constructive criticism from William C. Howell. Although the leadership literature is enormous, it Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to lacks an integrative theoretical framework that can make Mark Van Vugt, Department of Psychology, University of Kent, Can- sense of the richness of the data (Chemers, 2000; R. Hogan terbury CT2 7NP, United Kingdom; Robert Hogan, Hogan Assessment Systems, 2622 East 21st Street, Tulsa, OK 74114; or Robert B. Kaiser, & Kaiser, 2005). There are several reasons for this. First, Kaplan DeVries Inc., 1903-G Ashwood Court, Greensboro, NC 27455. the literature contains many useful mid-level theories that E-mail: [email protected], [email protected], or rkaiser@ are not very well connected (Bass, 1990; Yukl, 2006). kaplandevries.com 182 April 2008 ● American Psychologist Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0003-066X/08/$12.00 Vol. 63, No. 3, 182–196 DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.3.182 psychologists. The second question concerns ontogene- sis: When do leader–follower patterns emerge in the life span? Does developmental history predict leadership propensity? Developmental, personality, and educational psychologists are interested in these issues. The third question concerns phylogenesis: When did leadership emerge in our species, and are there parallels in other species? This question concerns comparative psycholo- gists, anthropologists, and zoologists. Finally, there is the question of the ultimate (evolutionary) functions of a mechanism, a question that interests evolutionary psy- chologists and biologists: Did leadership promote the survival of our forebearers so that it became part of our evolved psychology? Each of Tinbergen’s (1963) functions analyzes lead- ership from a different perspective and should be kept distinct. For instance, functional theories assume that lead- ership involves identifying obstacles between groups and their goals and then finding ways to overcome those ob- stacles (Hackman & Walton, 1986; Lord, 1977). These Mark theories offer proximate explanations for why particular Van Vugt leaders are effective in particular circumstances. They can be complemented with an analysis of the functions of leadership in ancestral environments, which may explain why and how the role of leadership evolved in the first ing the pursuit of those goals (cf. Bass, 1990; Hollander, place. 1992; Yukl, 2006). We think pragmatically of leadership as building a team and guiding it to victory (R. Hogan et al., Human Evolution, Group Life, and 1994). Leadership is both a resource for groups and an Leadership attribute of individuals, but we believe that its primary Humans evolved as group-living animals (Baumeister & significance concerns group performance (R. Hogan & Leary, 1995; D. T. Campbell, 1975; Darwin, 1871). The Kaiser, 2005; Kaiser, Hogan, & Craig, 2008). Given the genus Homo is estimated to be about 2.5 million years old, fitness and reproductive benefits associated with social and for most of their existence, hominids lived in small, status (Betzig, 1993; Buss, 2005; Chagnon, 1997), the kin-based bands on the African savannah, adopting a no- “selfish-gene” view of evolution (Dawkins, 1976) suggests madic hunter-gatherer lifestyle. Group living allowed our that everyone should strive to become a leader. From this ancestors to cope with an environment well supplied with same perspective it is not obvious why some would vol- predators but poorly supplied with shelter, water, and food untarily subordinate themselves. Researchers rarely con- (Foley, 1997; E. O. Wilson, 1975). Collective foraging and sider the origins of followership, but the topic is central to hunting, food sharing, division of labor, group defenses, an evolutionary analysis. and communal parenting provided a buffer against external Although Sigmund Freud, William James, William threats (Kenrick, Li, & Butner, 2003). The need for collec- McDougall, and E. L. Thorndike were enthusiastic Dar- tive action raises the question of how individuals in social winians, evolutionary thinking fell out of favor in main- groups decide what to do and how and when to do it. For stream psychology for most of the 20th century (Pinker, example, finding food would require group members to 2002). It is now returning in the form of evolutionary decide on the location and timing of foraging activities psychology. Evolutionary psychology proposes that the (Couzin, Krause, Franks, & Levin, 2005). Such problems mind is composed of mechanisms, called psychological can be solved by a decision-making process in which one adaptations, that were favored by natural selection because individual takes the initiative and provides direction while they solved adaptive problems faced by our ancestors. others acquiesce and follow that direction. Examples of such mechanisms include mating strategies, Individual and group survival would also have de- cheater detection, status sensitivity, and language (Barkow, pended on cooperative effort and group cohesion (Bloom, Cosmides, & Tooby, 1992; Buss, 2005; Schaller et al., 2000; Darwin, 1871; Sober & Wilson, 1998), which are 2006; cf. Darwin, 1871). inversely related to group size (Dunbar, 2004; Ingham, Evolutionary psychologists use Tinbergen’s (1963) Levinger, Graves, & Peckham, 1974). Anthropological ev- four functions model to analyze psychological adapta- idence suggests that life in ancestral groups involved con- tions. This framework first
Recommended publications
  • Happiness Syllabus 3-28-09
    The Pursuit of Happiness Political Science 192 SSB 353 Darren Schreiber Spring 2009 Social Science Building 367 (858) 534-1854 [email protected] Class Meets: Monday 2:00 p.m. – 4:00 p.m. on April 6 & 20, May 4 & 18, June 1 Office Hours: Thursday, 2:00 – 3:00 p.m. My Mission as a Teacher: “To enable my students to learn joyfully, think clearly, read carefully, and write well.” Abstract The Declaration of Independence describes the pursuit of happiness as an inalienable right. Economists are investigating subjective well-being. And, positive psychology is providing new insights. How might political science contribute to our individual and collective pursuit of happiness? Books Required: Jonathan Haidt (2006) The Happiness Hypothesis ($10.85) Recommended: Martin Seligman (2005) Authentic Happiness: Using the New Positive Psychology to Realize Your Potential for Lasting Fulfillment ($10.20) Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi (1998) Finding Flow: The Psychology of Engagement with Everyday Life ($10.17) Topics & Readings Meeting 1 (Monday, April 6th) – Introduction (Book Group 1/Articles Group 5) Alexander Weiss (2008) “Happiness Is a Personal(ity) Thing: The Genetics of Personality and Well-Being in a Representative Sample” Psychological Science (6 pages) Christopher K. Hsee and Reid Hastie (2006) Decision and experience: why don’t we choose what makes us happy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences (7 pages) Carol Graham (2005) “The Economics of Happiness: Insights on globalization from a novel approach” World Economics (15 pages) Haidt The Happiness Hypothesis
    [Show full text]
  • Beowulf and Competitive Altruism,” P
    January 2013 Volume 9, Issue 1 ASEBL Journal Association for the Study of Editor (Ethical Behavior)•(Evolutionary Biology) in Literature St. Francis College, Brooklyn Heights, N.Y. Gregory F. Tague, Ph.D. ~ Editorial Board ~▪▪~ Kristy Biolsi, Ph.D. THIS ISSUE FEATURES Kevin Brown, Ph.D. Wendy Galgan, Ph.D. † Eric Luttrell, “Modest Heroism: Beowulf and Competitive Altruism,” p. 2 Cheryl L. Jaworski, M.A. † Dena R. Marks, “Secretary of Disorientation: Writing the Circularity of Belief in Elizabeth Costello,” p.11 Anja Müller-Wood, Ph.D. Kathleen A. Nolan, Ph.D. † Margaret Bertucci Hamper, “’The poor little working girl’: The New Woman, Chloral, and Motherhood in The House of Mirth,” p. 19 Riza Öztürk, Ph.D. † Kristin Mathis, “Moral Courage in The Runaway Jury,” p. 22 Eric Platt, Ph.D. † William Bamberger, “A Labyrinthine Modesty: On Raymond Roussel Michelle Scalise Sugiyama, and Chiasmus,” p. 24 Ph.D. ~▪~ Editorial Interns † St. Francis College Moral Sense Colloquium: - Program Notes, p. 27 Tyler Perkins - Kristy L. Biolsi, “What Does it Mean to be a Moral Animal?”, p. 29 - Sophie Berman, “Science sans conscience n’est que ruine de l’âme,” p.36 ~▪~ Kimberly Resnick † Book Reviews: - Lisa Zunshine, editor, Introduction to Cognitive Cultural Studies. Gregory F. Tague, p. 40 - Edward O. Wilson, The Social Conquest of Earth. Wendy Galgan, p. 42 ~▪~ ~ Contributor Notes, p. 45 Announcements, p. 45 ASEBL Journal Copyright©2013 E-ISSN: 1944-401X *~* [email protected] www.asebl.blogspot.com ASEBL Journal – Volume 9 Issue 1, January 2013 Modest Heroism: Beowulf and Competitive Altruism Eric Luttrell Christian Virtues or Human Virtues? Over the past decade, adaptations of Beowulf in popular media have portrayed the eponymous hero as a dim-witted and egotistical hot-head.
    [Show full text]
  • The Adaptive Significance of Human Language
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 College Scholars 2000 The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2 Recommended Citation Oesch, Nathan, "The Adaptive Significance of Human Language" (2000). Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2/52 This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the College Scholars at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Department of Psychology University of Tennessee, Knoxville noesch @ utk. edu Abstract Many experts have argued that human language is fundamentally incompatible with the principles of traditional Darwinian evolutionary theory. According to conventional Darwinian explanations, specific traits evolved among species according to gradual and incremental genetic changes, each of which that were in some way so favorable to the survival and reproduction of ancestral generations that they were ultimately preserved within successive generations of those species. Human language, it has been said, is simply to complex to be explained as a result of Darwinian explanations, since each successive step in the evolution of language would confer no obvious survival benefits to its recipients. According to this idea, language is such an "all-or­ none system," that it could not possibly have existed in any immediately beneficial intermediate forms and thus could not have evolved according to conventional Darwinian modes of explanation.
    [Show full text]
  • Work, Wisdom, and Happiness BSPA-GB 3110.30 Mondays 6-9 Pm Beginning 2/11/19, Room Tba BELOW IS the SYLLABUS from 2018
    Work, Wisdom, and Happiness BSPA-GB 3110.30 Mondays 6-9 pm beginning 2/11/19, room tba BELOW IS THE SYLLABUS FROM 2018. THE SPRING 2019 COURSE WILL BE SIMILAR. THE MAIN READING IS CAROLINE WEBB’S BOOK Professor: Jonathan Haidt, [email protected], Office hours: Mondays after class; Thursdays 4-6 in KMC 7-98 Course Overview: For centuries, work was regarded as nothing but toil— a requirement for earning one's daily bread. But in recent decades, expectations about work have been transformed, as has its very nature. While it still provides one’s daily bread, it is also regarded as a major opportunity for people to find purpose, meaning, and happiness in their lives. In this course we'll study the latest research on what makes people happy at work, on how happiness at work improves the quality of work, and on what makes a career not just successful but meaningful. We will also discuss some of the impediments—both individual and organizational—to doing meaningful and satisfying work. Students will develop their own visions of their ideal career, and of the ideal company they’d like to lead or work for. They will also begin making changes to their daily lives, to improve their chances of success and happiness. Course Requirements: 1) 35% of the grade will be based on your class participation, which is not just the quantity of your comments but the nature of your engagement with the class. 2) 20% will be based on 4 one-page integration papers that you’ll submit by 10 am on the day of each class.
    [Show full text]
  • The Happiness Advantage
    THE HAPPINESS ADVANTAGE TODAY YOU’RE GONNA… Directly experience a variety of methods for increasing happiness. YAY! Identify a variety of activities that support your happiness Create a personal plan for incorporating more happy-making activities into your life. HEY, LET’S WATCH THIS VIDEO… “The Happiness Advantage: Research linking happiness and success” - Shawn Achor http://www.ted.com/talks/shawn_achor_the_happy_secret_to_better_work.html KEY POINTS AND INSIGHTS FROM VIDEO The public library is the center of public happiness first, of public education, next. John Cotton Dana ULA Youth Services Round Table Fall Workshop | September 18, 2015 | (CC BY-SA 3.0 US) Peter Bromberg | peterbromberg.com 3 gratitudes 2 Each day, write three things for which you are grateful. September 18 happiness advantage happiness 1. 2. 3. September 19 1. 2. 3. September 20 1. 2. 3. September 21 1. Happiness is not a goal...it's a by-product 2. of a life well lived. 3. Eleanor Roosevelt September 22 1. 2. 3. ULA Youth Services Round Table Fall Workshop | September 18, 2015 | (CC BY-SA 3.0 US) Peter Bromberg | peterbromberg.com ingredients for happiness 3 Write down one positive experience from the last 24 hours advantage happiness Write down three things that inspire a feeling of gratitude. Exercise 3-5 Minutes Meditate/Pray/Mindful Breathing Perform an act of kindness or express appreciation to someone The happiness of your life depends upon the quality of your thoughts. S t r e t c h Marcus Aurelius Pet a pet (a furry pet!) or hug or cuddle with a friend or loved one Spend time with someone you feel good around (“resonant relationship”) Let us be grateful to the people who make us happy; they are the charming Dealer’s Choice: What else facilitates your happiness? gardeners who make our souls blossom.
    [Show full text]
  • Humanum Review Humanum Issues in Family, Culture & Science
    3/5/2020 Searching for Happiness On an Elephant's Back | Humanum Review Humanum Issues in Family, Culture & Science Issue 4 Searching for Happiness On an Elephant's Back COLET C. BOSTICK Haidt, Jonathan, The Happiness Hypothesis: Finding Modern Truth in Ancient Wisdom (Basic Books, 2006). In the beginning of The Everlasting Man, G.K. Chesterton proposes that “there are two ways of getting home; and one of them is to stay there. The other is to walk round the whole world till we come back to the same place.” At the end of The Happiness Hypothesis: Finding Modern Truth in Ancient Wisdom, Jonathan Haidt concludes that “we can choose directions in life that will lead to satisfaction, happiness, and a sense of meaning [but] we can’t simply select a destination and walk there directly.” The juxtaposition of these two images of the search for happiness as a journey lays bare a fundamental question for reflective people in the 21st century: If it’s “not the destination, but the journey” that gives one a sense of purpose, does one need a destination at all? Chesterton and the Western Tradition assert that life’s journey is a quest, but, for moderns like Haidt, life is at best a walkabout in the wilderness. Jonathan Haidt is a social psychologist specializing in the field of positive psychology, the study of behaviors and conditions that lead to human flourishing. The Happiness Hypothesis was written in 2006, while Haidt was a professor at the University of Virginia trying to compress the entire discipline of psychology into a four-month semester.
    [Show full text]
  • Altruism, Morality & Social Solidarity Forum
    Altruism, Morality & Social Solidarity Forum A Forum for Scholarship and Newsletter of the AMSS Section of ASA Volume 3, Issue 2 May 2012 What’s so Darned Special about Church Friends? Robert D. Putnam Harvard University One purpose of my recent research (with David E. Campbell) on religion in America1 was to con- firm and, if possible, extend previous research on the correlation of religiosity and altruistic behavior, such as giving, volunteering, and community involvement. It proved straight-forward to show that each of sev- eral dozen measures of good neighborliness was strongly correlated with religious involvement. Continued on page 19... Our Future is Just Beginning Vincent Jeffries, Acting Chairperson California State University, Northridge The beginning of our endeavors has ended. The study of altruism, morality, and social solidarity is now an established section in the American Sociological Association. We will have our first Section Sessions at the 2012 American Sociological Association Meetings in Denver, Colorado, this August. There is a full slate of candidates for the ASA elections this spring, and those chosen will take office at the Meetings. Continued on page 4... The Revival of Russian Sociology and Studies of This Issue: Social Solidarity From the Editor 2 Dmitry Efremenko and Yaroslava Evseeva AMSS Awards 3 Institute of Scientific Information for Social Sciences, Russian Academy of Sciences Scholarly Updates 12 The article was executed in the framework of the research project Social solidarity as a condition of society transformations: Theoretical foundations, Bezila 16 Russian specificity, socio-biological and socio-psychological aspects, supported Dissertation by the Russian foundation for basic research (Project 11-06-00347а).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 December 19, 2019 the Psychology of Online Political Hostility
    The Psychology of Online Political Hostility: A Comprehensive, Cross-National Test of the Mismatch Hypothesis Alexander Bor* & Michael Bang Petersen Department of Political Science Aarhus University August 30, 2021 Please cite the final version of this paper published in the American Political Science Review at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055421000885. Abstract Why are online discussions about politics more hostile than offline discussions? A popular answer argues that human psychology is tailored for face-to-face interaction and people’s behavior therefore changes for the worse in impersonal online discussions. We provide a theoretical formalization and empirical test of this explanation: the mismatch hypothesis. We argue that mismatches between human psychology and novel features of online environments could (a) change people’s behavior, (b) create adverse selection effects and (c) bias people’s perceptions. Across eight studies, leveraging cross-national surveys and behavioral experiments (total N=8,434), we test the mismatch hypothesis but only find evidence for limited selection effects. Instead, hostile political discussions are the result of status-driven individuals who are drawn to politics and are equally hostile both online and offline. Finally, we offer initial evidence that online discussions feel more hostile, in part, because the behavior of such individuals is more visible than offline. Acknowledgements This research has benefitted from discussions with Vin Arceneaux, Matt Levendusky, Mark Van Vugt, John Tooby, and members of the Research on Online Political Hostility (ROHP) group, among many others. We are grateful for constructive comments to workshop attendees at the Political Behavior Section of Aarhus University, at the NYU-SMAPP Lab, at the NYU Social Justice Lab, at the Hertie School, and to conference audiences at APSA 2019, HBES 2019, and ROPH 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Leadership
    Running Head: EVOLUTION OF LEADERSHIP LEADERSHIP, FOLLOWERSHIP, AND EVOLUTION Some Lessons from the Past Mark Van Vugt Robert Hogan University of Kent Hogan Assessment Systems Robert B. Kaiser Kaplan DeVries Inc. Abstract This paper analyzes the topic of leadership from an evolutionary perspective and proposes three conclusions that are not part of mainstream theory. First, leading and following are strategies that evolved for solving social coordination problems in ancestral environments, including in particular the problems of group movement, intragroup peace-keeping, and intergroup competition. Second, the relationship between leaders and followers is inherently ambivalent due to the potential for exploitation by leaders. Third, modern organizational structures are sometimes inconsistent with aspects of our evolved leadership psychology, which might explain the alienation and frustration of many citizens and employees. The authors draw several implications of this evolutionary analysis for leadership theory, research, and practice. Key Words: EVOLUTION, LEADERSHIP, FOLLOWERSHIP, GAME THEORY, MISMATCH HYPOTHESIS Manuscript # 2006-0958RR in press at American Psychologist Please do not cite or circulate without permission from the authors 11 December, 2007 Author notes Mark Van Vugt is professor of social psychology at the University of Kent; Robert Hogan is the President of Hogan Assessment Systems; Rob Kaiser is the Director of Research & Development and a Partner with Kaplan DeVries Inc. The authors contributed equally to this paper. We are indebted to Rob Kurzban for his intellectual input and to Robin Dunbar, Dominic Johnson, Muhammad Ghufran Ahmad, and Peter Richerson for their comments on previous drafts. We also acknowledge constructive criticism from William C. Howell and three anonymous reviewers.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 This File Contains a Version of Chapter 5 from the Book The
    1 This file contains a version of chapter 5 from the book The Righteous Mind: Why Good People are Divided by Politics and Religion© by Jonathan Haidt. This version has been edited slightly to be used as a stand-alone reading as part of the OpenMind Library. The first four chapters of the book present research showing that moral judgments are based primarily on intuitions and emotions, which arise immediately when people hear a story, or read a newspaper article, or hear gossip. These rapid intuitive reactions then drive our reasoning. Moral reasoning is largely done post-hoc, in the seconds after we have made a moral judgment. It is done to help us justify our judgments to other people. The book uses the metaphor that the mind is divided like a small rider (conscious reasoning) on the back of a large elephant (all the automatic intuitive processes that occur rapidly and often outside of conscious awareness). The rider-and-elephant metaphor is helpful for understanding why it’s almost impossible to win a political argument: each person’s “rider” composes arguments aimed at the other person’s rider, but the elephant is really in charge. Unless you can change the elephant, you can’t persuade the other person. Chapter five begins the analysis of why moral judgments differ across cultures, including the “cultures” of left and right in many countries. CHAPTER 5 Beyond WEIRD Morality I got my Ph.D. at McDonald’s. Part of it, anyway, given the hours I spent standing outside of a McDonald’s restaurant in West Philadelphia trying to recruit working-class adults to talk with me for my dissertation research.
    [Show full text]
  • Nudging Evolutionary Mismatched Behaviors
    Evolutionary Mismatch 1 Nudging Evolutionary Mismatched Behaviors: Implications for Social Psychology and Public Policy Mark van Vugt Lianne P. De Vries Norman P. Li To appear in: J.P. Forgas, K. Fiedler, & W.D. Crano, Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology Draft: 26 April 2019 Word count: approximately 5000 (excl. references) Correspondence address: Mark van Vugt, Department of Experimental and Applied Psychology, Institute for Brain and Behavior Amsterdam, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, [email protected] Evolutionary Mismatch 2 Introduction Aliens visiting planet Earth would find Homo Sapiens a remarkable species. They would discover astonishing examples of human intelligence and ingenuity such as airplanes, skyscrapers, electricity, and the internet. At the same time, they would also witness our miserable failure in preventing poverty, violence, disease, and ecological disasters. Humans are both highly intelligent and highly stupid, the alien anthropologists would probably conclude. It is true, we humans are very good at some things but very bad at others. People are able to recognize the faces of persons they met many years ago, but they easily misremember the telephone numbers of their best friends. People are very patient in accumulating knowledge and training skills through a long period of intense education, but they are not patient enough to save money for their pension or resist the temptation of a Big Mac. And, although most people would be reluctant to steal from shopkeepers, these same individuals have no moral reservations to illegally download music and movies from the net. Why is it that humans are very good at some things but very bad at others? Why is it that we can learn some things much quicker and easier than others? What does this tell us about human psychology, and what can we learn from this for public policy? Here we argue that the human mind, with all its strengths and weaknesses, follows a logical, predictable pattern.
    [Show full text]
  • Integrative Anthropology and the Human Niche: Toward a Contemporary Approach to Human Evolution
    AMERICAN ANTHROPOLOGIST Integrative Anthropology and the Human Niche: Toward a Contemporary Approach to Human Evolution Agustın´ Fuentes ABSTRACT A niche is the structural, temporal, and social context in which a species exists. Over the last two million years, the human lineage underwent clear morphological changes alongside less easily measurable, but significant, behavioral and cognitive shifts as it forged, and was shaped by, new niches. During this time period, core human patterns emerged, including the following: hypercooperation; lengthy childhood and complex parenting; intricate and diverse foraging and hunting patterns; novel and dynamic material and symbolic cultures; and complex communication and information sharing, eventually resulting in language. Approaches to human evolution grounded in paleoanthropology and archaeology offer fundamental insights into our past, and traditional evolutionary the- ory offers a strong grounding for explaining them. However, given the centrality of distinctive physiological, social, semiotic, and cognitive processes in human evolutionary histories, a broader anthropological approach can facilitate additional understanding of the human story. An integrative anthropology, reaching across subfields and foci, com- bined with contemporary evolutionary theory is an approach that can enhance our abilities to model and understand human evolution. [integrative anthropology, niche construction, evolution, extended evolutionary synthesis, Homo, semiosis, Pleistocene] RESUMEN Un nicho es el contexto estructural,
    [Show full text]