Pan Africa News 14(1) PDF(468Kb)

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pan Africa News 14(1) PDF(468Kb) Pan Africa News TheThe Newsletter Newsletter of the of theCommittee Committee for forthe theCare Care andand Conservation Conservation of ofChimpanzees, Chimpanzees, and and the the MahaleMahale Wildlife Wildlife Conservation Conservation Society Society JUNE 2007 VOL. 14, NO. 1 P. A. N. EDITORIAL STAFF Contents Chief Editor: <NOTE> Toshisada Nishida, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan Laterality in Termite-Fishing by Fongoli Chimpanzees: Deputy Chief Editors: Preliminary Report Kazuhiko Hosaka, Kamakura Women’s University, Japan Michio Nakamura, Kyoto University, Japan Paco Bertolani, Clarissa Scholes, William C. McGrew & Jill D. Pruetz 1 Associate Editors: <NOTE> Christophe Boesch, Max-Planck Institute, Germany Jane Goodall, Jane Goodall Institute, USA Evidence of Cultural Differences in Diet between Two Takayoshi Kano, Kyoto University, Japan Neighboring Unit Groups of Chimpanzees in Mahale Tetsuro Matsuzawa, Kyoto University, Japan Mountains National Park, Tanzania William C. McGrew, University of Cambridge, UK Tetsuya Sakamaki, Michio Nakamura & Toshisada Nishida 3 John C. Mitani, University of Michigan, USA Vernon Reynolds, Budongo Forest Project, UK <NOTE> Yukimaru Sugiyama, Kyoto University, Japan Dermatophytosis of M group Chimpanzees, Mahale Richard W. Wrangham, Harvard University, USA Mountains, Tanzania Toshisada Nishida, Shiho Fujita, Takahisa Matsusaka, Editorial Secretaries: Masaki Shimada & Rashidi Kitopeni 5 Noriko Itoh, Kyoto University, Japan Koichiro Zamma, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan <NOTE> Agumi Inaba, Japan Monkey Centre, Japan Bonobos at the “Lola Ya Bonobo” Sanctuary in the Democratic Republic of the Congo Satoshi Hirata & Yasuko Tashiro 6 Instructions to Authors: Pan Africa News publishes articles, notes, reviews, <NOTE> forums, news, essays, book reviews, letters to editor, and Snare Removal for Conservation of Chimpanzees in the classified ads on any aspect of conservation and research Kalinzu Forest Reserve, Uganda regarding chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bilias (Pan Chie Hashimoto, Debby Cox & Takeshi Furuichi 8 paniscus). Contributors are requested to write in English and <NOTE> the papers except forums, reviews and essays should usually New Nut-Cracking Sites in Diecké Forest, Guinea: An be 1,500 words or less. Manuscripts should be submitted by e-mail to: Overview of the Surveys [email protected] Susana Carvalho, Cláudia Sousa & Tetsuro Matsuzawa 11 Photos and figures, however, should be sent by air mail <NOTE> to: T. Nishida, Department of Zoology, Graduate School of Papaya Fruit Sharing in Wild Chimpanzees at Bossou, Science, Kyoto University, Kitashirakawa- Oiwakecho, Sakyo, Guinea Kyoto, Japan. Gaku Ohashi 14 Publication of the next issue will be December 2007. Deadline for manuscripts is the end of November. <NOTE> 1. Leverhulme Centre for Human Evolutionary Studies, Dpt. of Biological Anthropology, University of Cambridge, UK Laterality in Termite-Fishing by 2. Dpt. of Anthropology, Iowa State University, USA Fongoli Chimpanzees: Preliminary Report INTRODUCTION Many studies in both free-ranging and captive apes have shown that some forms of laterality of hand function Paco Bertolani1, Clarissa Scholes1, William C. McGrew1, Jill D. Pruetz2 occur in non-human primates1. However, true handedness 2 (sensu McGrew and Marchant ), when most individuals 1 2 Pan Africa News, 14(1), June, 2007 show a skew in hand preference in the same direction analysis was done only on individual sessions, which do across different tasks, seems to be restricted to humans. not equal events 2. Laterality was tested with the binomial Other hominoids appear unlateralized in simpler tasks, test for each individual who attained at least 6 sessions. such as reaching, picking up objects, and grooming3, but All tests are two-tailed, with alpha set at 0.05 (A further they show hand preference for more complex tasks, such 10 chimpanzees showed termite-fishing but did not reach as tool-using2, 4, 5 or elaborate food processing6, 7. this criterion, which is the minimum number necessary to Laterality in termite-fishing8 has been studied only show a statistic significance). at Gombe, and the two published data-sets are congruent. PB recorded which hand inserted and extracted the McGrew and Marchant2, 9 reported that most (27 of 36) tool into and from the hole in the termite mound for each chimpanzees showed an individualized hand preference insertion event. for right or left, as did Lonsdorf and Hopkins10 (16 of 17) for termite-fishing in the same community. No other data RESULTS have been published for chimpanzee communities Table 1 shows the data for 47 sessions of elsewhere. This study asks if termite-fishing by Fongoli termite-fishing shown by 5 males. Each session averaged chimpanzees is lateralized, shows hand preference 9.1 data-points (range per individual: 7.3-10.6) and lasted (individuals are lateralized, but with no populational an average of 45 minutes. Two individuals, BN and KL, preference for either hand), or task specialization (all or were exclusively lateralized to the left and right most individuals use the same hand). respectively; YO was significantly right lateralized, and MM and SI were right-biased and left-biased respectively, METHODS but not significantly so. Although the sample is very Fongoli, in southeastern Senegal, is a mosaic small, no skew emerges in the direction of hand savanna, and the only hot, dry and open study site with preference among these subjects. The results for habituated chimpanzees (see Pruetz, 200611, for a instantaneous samples reflect those for sessions. description of the habitat and the diet of Fongoli Table 1. Frequency of lateralized (L vs. R) termite-fishing by chimpanzees). The only published study of sessions and instantaneous samples. 12 termite-fishing at Fongoli is based on indirect data LH RH LH inst. RH inst. Hand Subject Obs p-value (Bogart and Pruetz, in prep.). sessions sessions samples samples preference BN 6 6 0 41 0 0.032 L PB collected these data in April and May 2005, KL 8 0 8 0 74 0.008 R after habituation of the first males of the Fongoli MM 16 4 12 27 130 0.076 (R) community. The chimpanzees were observed for 543 SI 8 7 1 56 25 0.07 (L) hours over 50 days, when termites were a major food YO 9 1 8 14 52 0.04 R source. PB collected data using scan sampling with a Total 47 18 29 138 281 - - focal subject target on the subject’s activity and on the other individuals in sight, by instantaneous recording DISCUSSION every 5 minutes13. Termite-fishing at Fongoli appears to be an A session of termite-fishing was a continuous individually lateralized task, in which some of the period during which the individual ate termites. Sessions individuals specialize in using one hand, and some use are separated by other activities (e.g. rest, groom, etc). the other, but none is ambilateral. These results agree During a session, change of posture or position and with studies of Gombe chimpanzees3, 9, 10, despite the change of tool occurred: thus a session contained several sites being separated by thousands of kilometers on bouts (sensu McGrew and Marchant2). The data-points opposite sides of Africa. Extending data collection to the taken in each session by instantaneous sampling cannot rest of the Fongoli community is needed to validate these be considered independent observations, so statistical preliminary findings. Pan Africa News, 14(1), June, 2007 3 Acknowledgments. We thank the Republic of Senegal and <NOTE> Department of Eaux et Forets for permission to work in Senegal; D. Kante and M. Camara for assistance in the field. J.P. Evidence of Cultural Differences was supported by grants from the National Geographic Society, in Diet between Two Neighboring Center for the Study of Violence at Iowa State University (ISU), ISU Foreign Travel Grant, ISU Faculty Professional Unit Groups of Chimpanzees in Development Grant, and the American Society of Primatologists Mahale Mountains National Park, Conservation Grant. Tanzania REFERENCES 1 2 1. McGrew WC, Marchant LF 1997. On the other hand: Tetsuya Sakamaki , Michio Nakamura , 3 Current issues in and meta-analysis of the behavioral laterality Toshisada Nishida of hand function in nonhuman primates. Yearb Phys Anthropol 1. Faculty of International Studies, Meiji Gakuin University, Japan 40: 201-232. 2. Graduate School of Science, Kyoto University, Japan 2. McGrew WC, Marchant LF 1996. On which side of the 3. Japan Monkey Centre, Japan apes? Ethological study of laterality of hand use. In: Great Ape Societies, McGrew WC, Marchant LF, Nishida T (eds), Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 255-272. Assessing behavioral diversity is key to investigating 3. Marchant LF, McGrew WC 1996. Laterality of limb the existence of culture in wild chimpanzees and function in wild chimpanzees of Gombe National Park: determining their ability to adapt to various environments1. Comprehensive study of spontaneous activities. J Hum Evol 30: 427-443. However, most evidence of behavioral diversity among 4. Boesch C 1991. Handedness in wild chimpanzees. Int J chimpanzees has come from comparisons of distantly Primatol 12: 541-558. separated populations2. Although a few reports have 5. Sugiyama Y, Fushimi T, Sakura O, Matsuzawa T 1993. suggested differences in social customs between two Hand preference and tool use in wild chimpanzees. Primates 34: 151-159. neighboring unit groups3, 4, these studies were retrospective 6. Byrne RW, Byrne JME 1993. Complex leaf-gathering skills comparisons using photographs. Thus, a need exists to of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): Variability and investigate whether any behavioral differences occur standardization. Am J Primatol 31: 241-261. 7. Corp N, Byrne RW 2002. Leaf processing by wild between neighboring groups sharing a similar environment. chimpanzees: Physically defended leaves reveal complex We report preliminary evidence of new feeding habits by manual skills. Ethology 108: 673-696. chimpanzees that we are habituating, habits that differ 8. Goodall J 1963. Feeding behaviour of wild chimpanzees. A preliminary report. Symp Zool Soc Lond 10: 39-47. from those of a neighboring group in the Mahale 9.
Recommended publications
  • Food Hardness and Stone Tool Weight in Wild Primate Nut-Cracking
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/267542; this version posted February 19, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 Food hardness and stone tool weight in wild primate nut-cracking Michael Haslam1* 1School of Archaeology, University of Oxford, Oxford OX1 3QY, UK *Corresponding author email: [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0001-8234-7806 Abstract This study presents data on average stone tool weights and the hardness of foods processed by the three known stone-tool-using primate species: Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea), bearded capuchins (Sapajus libidinosus) and Western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Each of these primates uses stone hammers to crack open nuts in the wild, making them suitable for inter-species behavioural comparison. This work draws on published results to identify a distinct difference in the tool weight/food hardness curve between chimpanzees and the two monkey taxa, with the latter reaching an asymptote in mean tool weight of just over 1 kg regardless of increasing food hardness. In contrast, chimpanzees rapidly increase their tool weight in response to increasing hardness, selecting average masses over 5 kg to process the hardest nuts. Species overlap in their preference for tools of 0.8-1 kg for opening foods of hardness 2-3 kN, suggesting that this conjunction may represent a primate stone-tool-use optimum. Keywords Stone tool; percussion; hammer; macaque; capuchin; chimpanzee bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/267542; this version posted February 19, 2018.
    [Show full text]
  • Novembre 2020
    Novembre 2020 Abelmoschus esculentus Acacia auriculiformis Adansonia grandidieri Gombo - Ocra Acacia Baobab Lieu de récolte : Cameroun Lieu de récolte : Fruit rond Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Ile de la Réunion 14 € Le fruit Non disponible 0,10 € La valve Abelmoschus esculentus Acrocomia aculeata Adenanthera pavonina Gombo - Ocra Macauba Poids de l’or - Vakana Lieu de récolte : Cameroun Lieu de récolte : Bresil Fruit foncé Lieu de récolte : Madagascar 1,50 € Le fruit 3 € Le fruit 4,50 € Les 100 graines Abelmoschus esculentus Adansonia digitata Adenanthera pavonina Gombo - Ocra Baobab Poids de l’or - Vakana Lieu de récolte : Cameroun Fruit long Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Les douze graines 3 € Le fruit 2 € 2 € Le fruit Abrus precatorius Adansonia digitata Afrostyrax lepidophyllus Voamaintilany Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Baobab Ail de Brousse Vente réservée aux musées Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Conditions particulières, Lieu de récolte : Cameroun voir en page 23. 14 € Le fruit 1 € Les 5 graines 3 € Les 35 graines Abrus precatorius Adansonia grandidieri Afzelia africana Voamaintilany Baobab Doussié rouge Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Vente réservée aux musées Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Lieu de récolte : Cameroun Conditions particulières, Les huit graines 15 € Le fruit fermé voir en page 23. 2 € 12 € La grappe 55 € Le fruit ouvert Aleurites moluccana Aspidosperma macrocarpon Barringtonia asiatica Noix de Bancoule Peroba-cetim Fotabé Lieu de récolte : Madagascar Lieu de récolte : Bresil Lieu de récolte : Madagascar
    [Show full text]
  • Hunting Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees in the Taï National Park
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 78547-573 (1989) Hunting Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees in the Tai’ National Park CHRISTOPHE BOESCH AND HEDWIGE BOESCH Department of Ethology and Wildlife Research, University of Zurich, CH-8057 Zurich, Switzerland KEY WORDS Cooperation, Sharing, Traditions ABSTRACT Hunting is often considered one of the major behaviors that shaped early hominids’ evolution, along with the shift toward a drier and more open habitat. We suggest that a precise comparison of the hunting behavior of a species closely related to man might help us understand which aspects of hunting could be affected by environmental conditions. The hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees is discussed, and new observations on a population living in the tropical rain forest of the TaY National Park, Ivory Coast, are presented. Some of the forest chimpanzees’ hunting performances are similar to those of savanna-woodlands populations; others are different. Forest chimpanzees have a more specialized prey image, intentionally search for more adult prey, and hunt in larger groups and with a more elaborate cooperative level than sa- vanna-woodlands chimpanzees. In addition, forest chimpanzees tend to share meat more actively and more frequently. These findings are related to some theories on aspects of hunting behavior in early hominids and discussed in order to understand some factors influencing the hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees. Finally, the hunting behavior of primates is compared with that of social carnivores. Hunting is generally
    [Show full text]
  • Revisiting Panda 100, the First Archaeological Chimpanzee
    1 Revisiting Panda 100, the first archaeological chimpanzee 2 nut-cracking site 3 4 Proffitt. T.1*, Haslam. M.2, Mercader. J.F.3, Boesch. C.4, Luncz. L.V.5 5 6 1 Institute of Archaeology, University College London, 31-34 Gordon Square, London, WC1H 0PY 7 2 Primate Archaeology Research Group, School of Archaeology, University of Oxford, Dyson Perrins 8 Building, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3QY, United Kingdom 9 3 Department of Anthropology and Archaeology, University of Calgary, 2500 University Dr., NW 10 Calgary, Alberta T2N 1N4, Canada 11 4 Department of Primatology, Max Plank Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 12 D - 04103 Leipzig, Germany 13 5 School of Anthropology and Museum Ethnography, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX2 6PE, UK. 14 15 * Corresponding Author: [email protected] 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 1 27 Abstract 28 Archaeological recovery of chimpanzee Panda oleosa nut cracking tools at the Panda 100 (P100) and 29 Noulo sites in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast, showed that this behaviour is over 4,000 years old, making 30 it the oldest known evidence of non-human tool use. In 2002, the first report on P100 directly compared 31 its lithic assemblage to early hominin stone tools, highlighting their similarities and proposing the name 32 ‘Pandan’ for the chimpanzee material. Here we present an expanded and comprehensive technological, 33 microscopic, and refit analysis of the lithic assemblage from P100. Our re-analysis provides new data 34 and perspectives on the applicability of chimpanzee nut cracking tools to our understanding of the 35 percussive behaviours of early hominins.
    [Show full text]
  • Body Size and Diet Mediate Evolution of Jaw Shape in Squirrels (Sciuridae)
    Rare ecomorphological convergence on a complex adaptive landscape: body size and diet mediate evolution of jaw shape in squirrels (Sciuridae) *Miriam Leah Zelditch; Museum of Paleontology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109; [email protected] Ji Ye; Earth and Environmental Sciences, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109; [email protected] Jonathan S. Mitchell; Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109; [email protected] Donald L. Swiderski, Kresge Hearing Research Institute and Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan. [email protected] *Corresponding Author Running head: Convergence on a complex adaptive landscape Key Words: Convergence, diet evolution, jaw morphology, shape evolution, macroevolutionary adaptive landscape, geometric morphometrics Data archival information: doi: 10.5061/dryad.kq1g6. This is the author manuscript accepted for publication and has undergone full peer review but has not been through the copyediting, typesetting, pagination and proofreading process, which may lead to differences between this version and the Version of Record. Please cite this article as doi: 10.1111/evo.13168. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved. Abstract Convergence is widely regarded as compelling evidence for adaptation, often being portrayed as evidence that phenotypic outcomes are predictable from ecology, overriding contingencies of history. However, repeated outcomes may be very rare unless adaptive landscapes are simple, structured by strong ecological and functional constraints. One such constraint may be a limitation on body size because performance often scales with size, allowing species to adapt to challenging functions by modifying only size. When size is constrained, species might adapt by changing shape; convergent shapes may therefore be common when size is limiting and functions are challenging.
    [Show full text]
  • Akɔɔse Words for Plants (Info from KMB)
    Akɔɔse words for Plants (info from KMB) M. Cheek, B.J. Pollard, I. Darbyshire, J-M. Onana, and C. Wild, C. 2004. The Plants of Kupe, Mwanenguba and the Bakossi Mountains, Cameroon: A Conservation Checklist. Kew: Royal Botanical Gardens. Akoose Ortho English Latin Comment ? ayɛd shrub Mallotus oppositifolius ? 290 ? ebɔ́lɔ́g tree Neoboutonia mannii 291 ? mmwɛn herb, used for medicine Ageratum conyzoides 266 ? ndum or ndun herbaceous climber Zehneria minutiflora 278 ndom Zehneria scabra ? nkab herb, used to mark boundaries, tie fences Adenostemma mauritanum 266 ? nkom tree used for timber Termianlia superba 266 see trees check ?? nzuh Bakossi tea Dicliptera laxata 224 ?? nzoh kunte Dischistocalyx grandifolius 224 ?? haricot beans Phaseolus vulgaris 323 ?? Malay apple Syzygium malaccense 351 abii cola nut Cola spp. 87 van Haaren abíi á mbhɔrɛ ? mbi-mbura “goat breast” ? Phaulopsis angolana 229 achaŋ dé esembe pineapple Ananas comosus =? Ananas sativus 429 achebé tree, seeds from fallen fruits used as groundnut Magnistipula tessmannii 265 substitute achum shrub or tree used for timber Cordia aurantiaca 255 ákɔle okra Abelmoschus esculentus 331 akum epiphytic shrub Ficus ardisioides 344 tree Ficus chlamydocarpa asáá á kém monkey plum Pseudospondias microcarpa 234 atên raffia Raphia regalis 472 bɛdébɛde ?? ebony Dyospyros pseudomespilus 282 chyěŋgwaa pepper shrub, leaves used in soup, etc Piper umbellata 361 dǐd-á-mbwɛ́ɛ herb Dorstenia barteri 343 dií oil palm Elaeis guineensis 470 dyɔ̌ g-á-kud /akón parasitic climbing stem, fruits producing sticky fluid ?? 343 ebɔɔ tree Annickia chlorantha 236 1 Akoose Ortho English Latin Comment ebilékam herb Crassocephalum montuosum 268 eʼchatɛ́n tree Cleistopholis staudtii 237 echeb African tulip tree Spathodea campanulata 255 echém é nyə́ə shrubby herb Gomphocarpus physocarpus 248 echəŋgé fruit of okongobong vegetable (edible) ?? 307 echin taro, cocoyam Colocasia esculenta 87 ?? Colocasia antiquorum, C.
    [Show full text]
  • Percussive Tool Use by Taı Western Chimpanzees and Fazenda Boa
    Percussive tool use by Taı¨Western chimpanzees and Fazenda Boa Vista rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org bearded capuchin monkeys: a comparison Elisabetta Visalberghi1, Giulia Sirianni2, Dorothy Fragaszy3 2 Review and Christophe Boesch 1Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, 00197 Rome, Italy Cite this article: Visalberghi E, Sirianni G, 2Department of Primatology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 04103 Leipzig, Germany 3 Fragaszy D, Boesch C. 2015 Percussive tool Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA use by Taı¨Western chimpanzees and Fazenda Percussive tool use holds special interest for scientists concerned with Boa Vista bearded capuchin monkeys: a human origins. We summarize the findings from two field sites, Ta¨ı and comparison. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 370: Fazenda Boa Vista, where percussive tool use by chimpanzees and bearded 20140351. capuchins, respectively, has been extensively investigated. We describe the http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0351 ecological settings in which nut-cracking occurs and focus on four aspects of nut-cracking that have important cognitive implications, namely selection of tools, tool transport, tool modification and modulation of actions to reach Accepted: 23 June 2015 the goal of cracking the nut. We comment on similarities and differences in behaviour and consider whether the observed differences reflect ecological, One contribution of 14 to a theme issue morphological, social and/or cognitive factors. Both species are sensitive to ‘Percussive technology in human evolution: physical properties of tools, adjust their selection of hammers conditionally a comparative approach in fossil and living to the resistance of the nuts and to transport distance, and modulate the energy of their strikes under some conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Complete Document (Pdf )
    March 2001 ISSN 1020-3435 8 An information bulletin on Non-Wood Forest Products EDITORIAL CONTENTS We invited Mr Cherukat Chandrasekharan, who founded Non-Wood News in 1994, to write the 3 SPECIAL FEATURES editorial for this, the first issue of the new millennium. We would like to thank Mr Chandrasekharan for accepting our invitation. • Plan of action towards the sustainable development of the am very happy to have the honour and privilege of writing this editorial. rattan sector It is gratifying that Non-Wood News has proved to be a great success. The reasons are clear. • Les produits forestiers non ligneux I First, it has met a seriously felt need. Second, it came with a refreshing identity – not as old wine comestibles, utilisés dans les pays in a new bottle, but as new wine in a rather large barrel. Each issue of Non-Wood News is packed africains francophones with so much useful and interesting material that it would probably be better to spread it out and produce at least two issues annually. Non-Wood News also has a major mission: to help realize the potential that the future holds for 14 NEWS AND NOTES non-wood forest products (NWFP). NWFPs had a colourful and glorious past. Subsequently, • Action for Natural Medicines however, they suffered serious downfall and (Anamed) neglect. Some 5 000 years ago, the Chinese • Agroforestree Database – call for Emperor Shen Nung wrote (what is believed to be the earliest recorded use of plants as photos medicine) that chalmugra oil, an extract from • Anti-cancer diterpene taxol the fruit of Hydnocarpus spp., was an efficient • Arab Organization for Agricultural treatment against leprosy.
    [Show full text]
  • Determination of in Vitro Antioxidant Activity, Acute Toxicity in Guinea
    Human Journals Research Article September 2018 Vol.:13, Issue:2 © All rights are reserved by Justin N. Kadima et al. Determination of in Vitro Antioxidant Activity, Acute Toxicity in Guinea Pig and TLC-Fingerprint of Panda oleosa (Pierre) Bark Extract Used to Treat Diabetes in Folk Medicine Keywords: Panda oleosa, Plant, Fingerprint, Antioxidant activity, Toxicity, Guinea pig ABSTRACT 1 1,2* Frederick M. Katemo , Justin N. Kadima , Roland Objective: Panda oleosa (Pierre) is a rainforest plant used in D. Marini1 traditional medicine to treat diabetes among other diseases. The study aimed at determining TLC fingerprint profile and 1 measuring antioxidant activity and toxicity of trunk bark Department of Pharmacy, Faculty of Medicine and extracts. Methods: The fingerprint was processed for saponins, Pharmacy, University of Kisangani, DR Congo flavonoids and tannins fractions. The antioxidant activity was tested with 2,2-diphenyl-1-picrylhydrazyl (DPPH) radical 2 Department of Pharmacy, School of Medicine and scavenging and anti-hydroxyl radical capacity methods using Pharmacy, University of Rwanda, Rwanda quercetin, ascorbic acid and tannic acid as references. The toxicity was carried out in guinea pigs to estimate the range of LD . Results: Various colored spots appeared at different Rf on Submission: 20 August 2018 50 TLC chromatogram comprised of at least 8 spots in saponins Accepted: 27 August 2018 fractions, 4 in flavonoids, and 3 catechin derivatives in tannins Published: 30 September 2018 fractions. The EC50 values for DPPH radical scavenging capacity were 0.072 mg/ml, 0.156 mg/ml, 0.188 mg/ml and 0.432 mg/ml for total polyphenols, flavonoids, quercetin and tannins.
    [Show full text]
  • The Social Dimensions of Human-Elephant Conflict in Africa: a Literature Review and Case Studies from Uganda and Cameroon
    The social dimensions of human-elephant conflict in Africa: A literature review and case studies from Uganda and Cameroon Lisa Naughton*, Robert Rose* and Adrian Treves† *Department of Geography University of Wisconsin, Madison 550 N. Park Street Madison, WI 53706 [email protected]; [email protected] †Department of Zoology University of Wisconsin, Madison 250 N. Mills Street Madison, WI 53706 [email protected] A Report to the African Elephant Specialist , Human-Elephant Task Conflict Task Force, of IUCN, Glands, Switzerland. December 1999 Acknowledgments: Several individuals and organizations provided support: In Cameroon, Robert Rose’s fieldwork was funded by the Wildlife Conservation Society, through a grant by Dutch Foreign Aid. The Cameroon case study was aided greatly by the expertise of WCS field staff, particularly Anthony Nchanji, Roger Fotso, and Bryan Curran. Walters Arrey was responsible for monitoring crop damage during July-October 1999. In Uganda, Lisa Naughton and Adrian Treves’ fieldwork was funded by the Wildlife Conservation Society, National Geographic, Makerere University Biological Field Station, NSF, and Fulbright-Hays. Pascal Baguma and Patrick Katuramu provided first-rate assistance with data collection and interviews around Kibale National Park. Patrick Ilukol and Erica Cochrane generously shared their knowledge of elephant movement and raiding behavior at Kibale. In Madison, Karen Archabald provided comments on draft excerpts of this report. Erin Olson- Dedjoe and Nora Alvarez helped with data entry. Finally, Richard Hoare deserves special thanks for his expert counsel and assistance throughout the study. 2 Table of Contents Acknowledgements…………………………………………………………………………………….2 Table of Contents………………………………………………………………………………………3 List of Tables and Figures……………………………………………………………………………...4 I.
    [Show full text]
  • Ethnobotanical Survey of Plants Used in the Treatment of Malaria in the Sekyere Central District of Ashanti Region of Ghana
    ISSN 2394-966X International Journal of Novel Research in Life Sciences Vol. 2, Issue 6, pp: (17-25), Month: November-December 2015, Available at: www.noveltyjournals.com Ethnobotanical Survey of Plants Used In the Treatment of Malaria in the Sekyere Central District of Ashanti Region of Ghana 1Vigbedor Bright Yaw, 2Osafo Acquah Samuel, 3Ben Adu Gyan Department of Chemistry, Kwame Nkrumah University of Science and Technology, Kumasi-Ghana Department of immunology, Noguchi memorial institute for medical research, College of health sciences, University of Ghana Abstract: An ethnobotanical survey was conducted from September to December 2013 in the Sekyere Central district of the Ashanti region of Ghana. The survey aimed at identifying the plants used in the treatment of malaria in the Sekyere central district of Ashanti region of Ghana. The survey involved use of questionnaires and interviews with herbalist, house–to-house and the field that have a rich knowledge on the plants. A total of 29 medicinal plants species were recorded from the survey. To compare the usages of the plant species, an index of performance (Ip) was calculated for each plant species, from the number of citations of treatment actually recorded from the households against the proportion of each plant among the general flora. The survey revealed that, 6 of the 29 species of plants identified have not been authenticated for its antimalarial activity. Afzelia africana, Antrocaryon micraster, Afraegle paniculata, Persea americana and Antiaris africana spp. and Panda oleosa. The remaining 23 species have gone through various degrees of scientific authentications and validations. Keywords: Afzelia africana, Antrocaryon micraster, Afraegle paniculata, Persea Americana, Antiaris africana spp.
    [Show full text]
  • Ecological and Economic Potential of Non-Timber Forest Products in Southeast Cameroon
    It's Not the Availability, But the Accessibility that Matters: Title Ecological and Economic Potential of Non-Timber Forest Products in Southeast Cameroon Author(s) HIRAI, Masaaki; YASUOKA, Hirokazu African study monographs. Supplementary issue (2020), 60: Citation 59-83 Issue Date 2020-03 URL https://doi.org/10.14989/250128 Copyright by The Center for African Area Studies, Kyoto Right University, March 1, 2020. Type Departmental Bulletin Paper Textversion publisher Kyoto University African Study Monographs, Suppl. 60: 59–83, March 2020 IT’S NOT THE AVAILABILITY, BUT THE ACCESSIBILITY THAT MATTERS: ECOLOGICAL AND ECONOMIC POTENTIAL OF NON-TIMBER FOREST PRODUCTS IN SOUTHEAST CAMEROON HIRAI Masaaki The Center for African Area Studies, Kyoto University, Japan YASUOKA Hirokazu The Center for African Area Studies, Kyoto University, Japan ABSTRACT This study examined the ecological availability and economic potential of 10 non-timber forest product (NTFP) species. Irvingia gabonensis and Ricinodendron heudelotii produce large quantities of fruits and have higher economic values compared to other species. The number of nuts and kernels harvested and sold was a small percentage of the total annual production in the area. This is likely to be due to the low human population density and the difficulty in increasing the efficiency of gathering the nuts and kernels. Given the affluent ecological availability demonstrated in this study, even when innovations to improve the efficiency of harvesting are achieved, the trees produce more than 10 times the amount of fruits that can be gathered by all the people in the region, even with their maximum labor input. However, because of the forest zoning carried out by the government in the 1990s, the area that local people can harvest, without any concerns, is limited.
    [Show full text]