Running Head: BELIEF in PHYSIOGNOMY 1 Lay Beliefs In

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Running Head: BELIEF in PHYSIOGNOMY 1 Lay Beliefs In Running head: BELIEF IN PHYSIOGNOMY 1 Lay Beliefs in Physiognomy Explain Overreliance on Facial Impressions Bastian Jaeger and Anthony M. Evans Tilburg University Mariëlle Stel University of Twente Ilja van Beest Tilburg University Draft version: 4 June 2020 Word count: 4992 This paper is currently undergoing peer review. Comments are welcome. Author Note Bastian Jaeger, Anthony M. Evans, and Ilja van Beest, Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, The Netherlands; Mariëlle Stel, Department of Psychology of Conflict, Risk and Safety, The Netherlands. All data, analysis scripts, materials, and preregistration documents are available at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/s9nj8/). Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Bastian Jaeger, Department of Social Psychology, Tilburg University, P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]. BELIEF IN PHYSIGONOMY 2 Abstract People often rely on a person’s facial appearance when judging their character, even when more diagnostic information is available. This can lead to worse decision outcomes and appearance- based discrimination. What explains this overreliance on facial impressions? In three studies, we examine the role of lay beliefs in physiognomy—the idea that facial features are indicative of a person’s character. We find widespread endorsement of physiognomic beliefs in a representative sample of the Dutch population (Study 1, n = 2,624). Crucially, people with stronger physiognomic belief rely more on facial impressions when making trust decisions (Study 2, n = 224). They are also more confident in their ability to detect corrupt politicians based on facial photographs, but this increased confidence is not associated with superior judgment accuracy (Study 3, n = 406). In sum, our studies show that physiognomic beliefs are widespread and related to overreliance on facial impressions. Keywords: physiognomy, lay beliefs, social perception, trait inferences, social decision-making BELIEF IN PHYSIGONOMY 3 Lay Beliefs in Physiognomy Explain Overreliance on Facial Impressions People spontaneously judge the trustworthiness of strangers based on their facial appearance (Todorov et al., 2015). Even though the accuracy of trustworthiness impressions is generally low (Bonnefon et al., 2017; Rule et al., 2010; Todorov et al., 2015), people are relatively confident in their impressions (Ames et al., 2010) and rely on them when making consequential decisions (Olivola et al., 2014). People even rely on facial impressions when they have access to more diagnostic information (Jaeger et al., 2019) and when they are explicitly instructed to discount a person’s appearance (Jaeger et al., 2020). This excessive reliance on facial cues can lead to worse decision outcomes (Olivola & Todorov, 2010) and to appearance- based discrimination. Why do people persistently rely on trustworthiness impressions from faces? Here, we examine the role of lay beliefs in physiognomy. The practice of physiognomy rests on the notion that a person’s character is reflected in their facial appearance (Lavater, 1775). Early writings proposed that the size and orientation of facial features reflect their frequent use (e.g., a disagreeable person who frowns a lot will have lowered eyebrows) and resemblances between humans and other animals were thought to point to shared psychological attributes (e.g., a person who looks like a lion is brave like a lion; Aristotle, trans. 1936). These speculations were not based on rigorous scientific study and many claimed links between specific facial features and personality traits were disconfirmed by empirical work at the beginning of the 20th century (Alley, 1988). Although physiognomy is now widely viewed as pseudo-science in academic circles (Todorov, 2017), it remains unclear how widespread belief in physiognomy is in the general population. Crucially, widespread beliefs in physiognomy may explain the pervasive effect of trustworthiness impressions on social outcomes. If people think facial features are a valid indicator of a person’s character, then they might rely more on facial impressions, especially because these impressions come to mind easily (Todorov et al., 2015). Thus far, little is known about physiognomic beliefs. A recent study by Suzuki and colleagues (2017) showed that physiognomic beliefs are (a) related to other lay beliefs, such as belief in a just world and belief in the biological determinism of personality and (b) associated with more extreme trait judgments based on facial appearance. These are important insights, but do not answer whether belief in physiognomy can also explain why some people (over-)rely on facial impressions. BELIEF IN PHYSIGONOMY 4 The current investigation extends previous work in three important ways. First, we examine the prevalence of physiognomic beliefs in a large, representative sample of the Dutch population (Study 1, n = 2,624, preregistered). We estimate how many people believe that facial features are a good indicator of a person’s personality, and of their moral character in particular. Second, we examine whether people who believe in physiognomy rely more on facial impressions in social decision-making (Study 2, n = 224). Specifically, we test whether belief in physiognomy predicts reliance on trustworthiness impressions in a situation in which people could also rely on more diagnostic information instead. In other words, we investigate overreliance on trustworthiness impressions. Third, we examine whether this increased reliance on facial impressions is justified (Study 3, n = 406, preregistered). To this end, we replicate a recent study by Lin and colleagues (2018) who found that people are able to detect the corruptibility of politicians based on facial photographs at rates higher than chance. We test whether stronger physiognomic beliefs are associated with increased confidence in the accuracy of corruptibility judgments. Crucially, we also examine whether this increased confidence is justified, that is, whether it is based on superior judgment accuracy. We test whether people who believe in physiognomy attach more weight to facial impressions because their impressions are more accurate. For each study, we made the data, analysis scripts, and preregistration documents available at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/s9nj8/). We report all measures, how our sample sizes were determined, and all data exclusions. Study 1: Prevalence of Physiognomic Beliefs The main goal of Study 1 was to estimate the prevalence of belief in physiognomy. The pervasive effects of trustworthiness impressions on social outcomes may be explained by widespread belief in the diagnosticity of facial features for judging a person’s moral character. We measured endorsement of general physiognomic belief (e.g., “I can learn something about a person’s personality just from looking at his or her face”) in a large, representative sample of the Dutch population. We also assessed the specific belief that a person’s moral character (e.g., trustworthiness) is reflected in their facial features. Methods Participants. We recruited a representative sample of 2,807 Dutch participants via the LISS (Longitudinal Internet Studies of the Social Sciences) panel (Scherpenzeel & Das, 2010). BELIEF IN PHYSIGONOMY 5 The panel is based on a probability sample of Dutch households drawn from the population register. Panel members are representative of the Dutch population on indicators like gender, age, education, and income (see http://lissdata.nl for more information). Data from 183 participants (6.52%) who had missing data for at least one question was excluded from analysis, leaving a final sample of 2,624 participants (Mage = 52.60, SDage = 16.50; 52.52% female, 47.48% male). A sensitivity analysis in G*Power (Faul et al., 2007) showed that this sample size afforded us 80% power to detect a small difference (r = .055) between general and morality- specific physiognomic beliefs (with α = 5%). Materials and procedure. We measured physiognomic beliefs in two ways. To measure general physiognomic belief, participants were prompted to imagine seeing the passport photo of a stranger. They were asked to indicate how much they agreed with three statements (e.g., I can learn something about a person’s personality just from looking at his or her face) on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Average scores across the three items constituted our measure of general physiognomic belief (Cronbach’s α = .63). We randomized the order in which the three items were presented. We also measured the specific belief that a person’s moral character is reflected in their facial features. We asked participants how accurately they could judge three traits related to moral character (trustworthiness, sincerity, honesty; α = .92; Brambilla et al., 2011) from looking at a person’s passport photo. For each item, responses were indicated using a slider from 0 (not at all accurately) to 100 (extremely accurately). For exploratory purposes, we also asked participants to indicate their physiognomic beliefs for nine other characteristics: three characteristics related to a person’s sociability (warmth, friendliness, likeability; α = .90), three characteristics related to a person’s competence (competence, intelligence, skillfulness; α = .87) and three physically salient characteristics (gender, age, and attractiveness). The order in which the items were presented was randomized. Results for the additional
Recommended publications
  • Physiognomy in Ancient Science and Medicine
    Physiognomy Mariska Leunissen The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Introduction Physiognomy(fromthelaterGreek physiognōmia ,whichisacontractionoftheclassicalform physiognōmonia )referstotheancientscienceofdeterminingsomeone’sinnatecharacteronthe basisoftheiroutward,andhenceobservable,bodilyfeatures.Forinstance,Socrates’famous snubnosewasuniversallyinterpretedbyancientphysiognomistsasaphysiognomicalsignof hisinnatelustfulness,whichheonlyovercamethroughphilosophicaltraining.Thediscipline initstechnicalformwithitsownspecializedpractitionersfirstsurfacesinGreeceinthefifth century BCE ,possiblythroughconnectionswiththeNearEast,wherebodilysignswere takenasindicatorsofsomeone’sfutureratherthanhischaracter.Theshifttocharacter perhapsarisesfromthewidespreadculturalpracticeintheancientGreekandRomanworld oftreatingsomeone’soutwardappearanceasindicativeforhispersonality,whichisalready visibleinHomer(eighthcentury BCE ).Inthe Iliad ,forinstance,adescriptionofThersites’ quarrelsomeandrepulsivecharacterisfollowedbyadescriptionofhisequallyuglybody(see Iliad 2.211–219),suggestingthatthiscorrespondencebetweenbodyandcharacterisno accident.ThersitesisthustheperfectfoilfortheGreekidealofthe kaloskagathos –theman whoisbothbeautifulandgood.Thesameholdsforthepracticeofattributingcharacter traitsassociatedwithaparticularanimalspeciestoapersonbasedonsimilaritiesintheir physique:itisfirstformalizedinphysiognomy,butwasalreadywidelyusedinanon- 1 technicalwayinancientliterature.Themostfamousexampleofthelatterisperhaps SemonidesofAmorgos’satireofwomen(fragment7
    [Show full text]
  • INTRODUCTION Anthropometry Literally Means the Measurement of Man. It Is Derived from Greek Words, Anrhropos Which Means Man
    INTRODUCTION Anthropometry literally means the measurement of man. It is derived from Greek words, anrhropos which means man and metron which means measure. As an early tool of physical anthropology, it has been used for identification, for the purpose of understanding human physical variation. International Encyclopedia of Ergonomics and Human factors (vol. I) narrated that the word Anthropometry was coined by French naturalist Georges Cuvier. In view of the fact that no two individuals are ever alike in all their measurable characters (except perhaps monozygotic twins) and that the later tend to undergo change in varying degrees, hence persons living under different conditions and members of different ethnic groups and the offspring of unions between them frequently present interesting differences in body form and proportions. It is therefore, necessary to have some means of giving quantitative expression to the variations exhibited by such traits. Anthropometry constitutes a means towards this end, The anthropologists are concemed with functional relationships among traits and between traits and the environment. Anthropometry as defined by Juan Comas is a systematized body of techniques for measuring and taking observations on man, his skeleton, the skull, the limbs and trunk etc., as well as the organs, by the most reliable means and scientific methods." HISTORICAL AND EPISTEMOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE The journey of the concept of anthropometry began in the age of early man when they start to fullfill there needs in the pre-historic times. The units of measurement were probably among the earliest tools invented by humankind. It was this concept of measurement which first initiated the ways of comparison between the ecofacts and artifacts, shapes and sizes of inorganic materials like tools and organic materials like plants and animals and later on man began his/her comparison with others in view of morphology, strength of the body, behaviour,etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Physiognomy and Nineteenth-Century Iconography
    High-Art Music and Low-Brow Types: Physiognomy and Nineteenth-Century Music Iconography Alan Davison The relationship between cultural practices and their particular visual manifestations in art raises innumerable complexities for the art historian; so when a clear connection can be ascertained between the cultural and visual, it reveals much of the artist's intentional, and no doubt unintentional, use of visual cues. In the case of visual art from the nineteenth century, a strong link can be found between artistic conventions in depicting character type and the widespread belief in physiognomy. Physiognomy was a pseudo-science of interpreting faces that imbued many levels of culture in the late eighteenth and most of the nineteenth century. Its most tangible manifestation was in literature, both fiction and nonfiction. Musicologists who have read nineteenth-century letters, reviews or memoirs have most probably encountered descriptions of people made under the influence of physiognomic precepts. These descriptions are notable today only because they tend to be strangely detailed, or even rather tiresome in nature; they can also lead directly to bold character judgments. A typical example, regarding Franz Liszt, dates from 28 November 1840 in the Birmingham Journal: He is a very good-lookingyoung man, pale, thin, and intellectual; with a fine forehead, good nose, and well cut mouth; not a little resembling the portraits of Bonaparte, when a captain of artillery. He is plainly, in his department, a man of great genius and originality.' Underlying much of this fastidious attention to physical appearance and brazen confidence in assessing personality was the widespread belief in physiognomy.
    [Show full text]
  • The Physiognomy of Monarch in Italian and Chinese Literary Description
    QI CHEN Monarchial Body and Body Monarch: The Physiognomy of Monarch in Italian and Chinese literary description In the images of monarchy there are both the deliberate construction and promotion by central governmental authority and the creation by a public projecting their own desires and anxieties on to a prominent figure. (Hackett 2001: 814) In many cases, especially for portraits of ruler in literature, one can see the iconographic tradition. It was the result of “reconstructions" or “inventions". (Casini 2000: 75–88) Many studies have made important contributions to it, yet there still seems to be more things to do. The notion that inner excellence is reflected in superficial beauty–as well as the reverse of that notion–was unquestionably a deep-seated habit of Greek thought, reflected in the perfection of Homer’s gods and heroes–and the inferiority of Thersites–and the ubiquitous expression. Furthermore, the idea that physical appearance signified certain aspects of nature and character suffused Greek culture, though in various ways and with varying degrees of sophisticated reflection, from the vulgar popularity of the handsome to the rather distasteful Greek disapproval of Oriental and barbarian physical traits to the philosophical efforts in the Hippocratic corpus to explain the relationship between climate, physique and disposition. It is the role that physiognomy plays. (Jouanna 1989: 172) As a mechanism to make communication between body and the exterior world, physiognomy is one of the most vivid perspectives to examine the relationship between the individual and the world. Just as G. B. Della Porta said, the physiognomy costume is correspondent the worldly custom.
    [Show full text]
  • Pioneers in Criminology: Cesare Lombroso (1825-1909) Marvin E
    Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology Volume 52 Article 1 Issue 4 November-December Winter 1961 Pioneers in Criminology: Cesare Lombroso (1825-1909) Marvin E. Wolfgang Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarlycommons.law.northwestern.edu/jclc Part of the Criminal Law Commons, Criminology Commons, and the Criminology and Criminal Justice Commons Recommended Citation Marvin E. Wolfgang, Pioneers in Criminology: Cesare Lombroso (1825-1909), 52 J. Crim. L. Criminology & Police Sci. 361 (1961) This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology by an authorized editor of Northwestern University School of Law Scholarly Commons. The Journal of CRIMINAL LAW, CRIMINOLOGY, AND POLICE SCIENCE Vol. 52 NOVEMBER-DECEMBER 1961 No. 4 PIONEERS IN CRIMINOLOGY: CESARE LOMBROSO (1835-1909) MARVIN E. WOLFGANG The author is Associate Professor of Sociology in the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia. He is the author of Patterns in Criminal Homicide, for which he received the August Vollmer Research Award last year, and is president of the Pennsylvania Prison Society. As a former Guggenheim Fellow in Italy, Dr. Wolfgang collected material for an historical analysis of crime and punishment in the Renaissance. Presently he is engaged in a basic research project entitled, "The Measurement of De- linquency." Some fifty years have passed since the death of Cesare Lombroso, and there are several important reasons why a reexamination and evaluation of Lombroso's life and contributions to criminology are now propitious. Lombroso's influence upon continental criminology, which still lays significant em- phasis upon biological influences, is marked.
    [Show full text]
  • Face Value - the Long and Short
    Face value - The Long and Short https://thelongandshort.org/society/is-there-such-thing-as-a-criminal-face A magazine of innovation, new ideas and how the world is changing Face value Long Face value Researchers recently claimed to be able to use AI to infer the inherent 'criminality' of faces. It's an idea with a long, dubious and dangerous history 26th January 2017 By Rhodri Marsden Illustration by Luke Best When the BBC broadcast the recent documentary by Louis Theroux that looked back at the time he spent in the company of Jimmy Savile, there was disbelief across social media that no one had stepped in to stop Savile from committing his crimes. Some blamed the BBC, some blamed those in Savile's immediate circle, but others blamed a simple error of human judgment. "He literally couldn't look more like a paedophile," read one post – one of many to state a supposedly incontrovertible truth: that Savile's criminal tendencies could have been detected from the shape of his features, his eyes, his hair. Moreover, this has nothing to do with the benefit of hindsight and should have been picked up at the time. His looks, they suggested, were a moral indicator, with a wealth of compelling visual 1 of 13 9/29/19, 5:05 PM Face value - The Long and Short https://thelongandshort.org/society/is-there-such-thing-as-a-criminal-face evidence to support the claim. We know that paedophiles, murderers and other violent criminals come in many shapes and sizes. If we knew nothing about their criminal history, some of their photos might even appear attractive.
    [Show full text]
  • Phrenology, Physiognomy, and the Character of Big Business, 1895-1914 Coleman Sherry Undergraduate Senior Thesi
    Corporate Heads: Phrenology, Physiognomy, and the Character of Big Business, 1895-1914 Coleman Sherry Undergraduate Senior Thesis Department of History Columbia University 29 March 2021 Seminar Advisor: Professor Samuel Roberts Second Reader: Professor Richard John Abstract In this thesis I argue that practical phrenology—a loose set of practices for reading character in heads, faces, and bodies—played an important and underappreciated role in the popular coverage of the large new corporations that emerged from the “Great Merger Movement” around the turn of the twentieth century. I suggest that the scope and pace of the transition from proprietor to corporate ownership created a crisis of economic representation, defined by a lack of stable, mature conventions for describing and illustrating the actual activities of the new consolidated firms. In this context, journalists and cartoonists borrowed from the wildly-popular practical phrenology and personalized the corporations, describing the firms as if they were the straightforward extensions of famous individual owners. Through a close, comparative reading of biographical profiles published in Fowler and Wells’ Phrenological Journal, McClure’s Magazine, and the muckraking cartoons of Puck, I document the trespass of phrenological methods, language, and assumptions into popular contexts and publications. This phrenological personalization allowed public commentators to publish powerful polemics focused on the character of the new firms, but obscured and distorted their true forms. Table
    [Show full text]
  • The Physiognomy of Dispersed Power by Jon Marshall
    1 Vol 16 Issue 4 – 5 The Physiognomy of Dispersed Power by Jon Marshall Abstract The web of post-modern power appears nomadic, elusive and always elsewhere. Like our online presences, it has no obvious boundaries and appears as spirit-like, a magic life haunting the net and the world. Government becomes liminal: 'Liminal identities are neither here nor there, they are betwixt and between the positions assigned by law, custom and ceremonial' to quote Victor Turner. This liminality has changed the balance of power between the corporate and non-corporate sectors, however this does not mean that power is straightforward. When everything is interlinked through information technology then exercises of power may even increase confusion and undermine the bases or legitimacy of that power. Modes of ordering can produce perceived disorder. Knowledge of the system becomes divination and trapped in magic. It is suggested that an awareness of this, and focusing on contradiction, or oscillation is more useful than focusing on simplicity. Introduction This is the second in a series of papers developing the consequences of Michael Hardt and Antonio Negri's idea that, nowadays, power forms a complex dispersed and dynamic network encompassing the world, which they call "Empire" [1] and which for reasons discussed later I prefer to call "distributed governance". The first of these papers [2] argued that distributed governance (in particular as manifested on the Internet, including the 'free software or open source movement'), does not necessarily produce inherently democratic, co-operative, egalitarian, non-hierarchical or unauthoritarian movements as Negri and Hardt's imply in their analysis of the internet or through their counterpositionary term 'Multitude'.
    [Show full text]
  • Manuela Bragagnolo Working Paper the Italian Academy for Advanced
    Manuela Bragagnolo Working Paper The Italian Academy for Advanced Studies in America - Columbia University Law, Physiognomy and Medicine in Sixteenth-Century Venice: The Case of Giovanni Ingegneri, Bishop of Capodistria († 1600) 10.27.2015 Educated in medicine, Giovanni Ingegneri later became a Venetian jurist who also served as Bishop of Capodistria from 1576 until his death in 1600. An extremely cultivated man, he was the author of two books: a handwritten legal treatise hitherto unknown to modern scholars, entitled Contra la sofistica disciplina de’ giureconsulti (Against the Sophistic Discipline of Jurists), and a treatise on physiognomy, Fisionomia Naturale, which has been referenced in several studies on physiognomy and magic but never studied in its entirety (THORNDYKE, 1941; PORTER, 2005). At the time of Ingegneri’s death, both texts existed in manuscript form only. Fisionomia Naturale was published posthumously and anonymously by Ingegneri’s nephew, Angelo, in 1606. Both treatises testify to an extraordinary and original mind. Even if Ingegneri has, until recently, been overlooked by modern scholars, Lodovico Antonio Muratori, the prominent eighteenth-century Italian historian, evidently grasped the significance of his work. Indeed, some of the most innovative aspects of his own oeuvre build on Ingegneri’s treatises. As I have previously revealed in my own research, Muratori unearthed Ingegneri’s legal manuscripts at Milan’s Biblioteca Ambrosiana, founded by Federico Borromeo, during the period 1695-1700 (BRAGAGNOLO, 2014). During his residency as a “doctor” there, Muratori recorded, in several notebooks, extracts from what appeared to be a “manuscript project” – “un progetto manoscritto” - of Ingegneri’s treatise Contra la sofistica disciplina de’ giureconsulti.
    [Show full text]
  • Biological Positivist Theories Distribute Or Yychapter Overview and Objectives in This Chapter, We Introduce the Theories Offered by Biological Positivism
    CHAPTERSECTION 13 Biological Positivist Theories distribute or yyChapter Overview and Objectives In this chapter, we introduce the theories offered by biological positivism. These theories focus on individual characteristics that are inherited and present at birth, such as biological and mental traits. Lombroso, Ferri, and Garofalo (known collectively as “the Italian Positivists”) attempted to explore these characteristics by applying the scientificpost, method to the study of criminality. While the simplistic measurement of physical characteristics employed by early biological positivists failed to uncover the ultimate cause of crime, this work led to other, more nuanced approaches, suggesting that there are biological precursors that may make some people more prone to committing crime. Figure 3.1 pro- vides a general overview of the approach we take in this chapter. By the end of the chapter, readers should be able to: • Understand the assumptions made by biological positivist theories • Acknowledge thecopy, historical period in which these theories emerged • Be aware of the level of explanation, problem focus, and scope of biological positivist theories • Consider the historical and social context that gave rise to the biological positivist theories • Explainnot the practical approaches and programs suggested by biological positivist theories yyThe Biological Positivist Tradition As discussed in the previous chapter, in criminology and the other social sciences (e.g., sociology, psychology, and economics), positivism refers to the application of the scientific method to explain Dohuman behavior. Early criminological positivists were primarily biological theorists who sought to explain criminality by identifying physical characteristics and genetic differences that distinguish criminals from noncriminals. However, later theorists started to use psychological and social factors 38 Copyright ©2015 by SAGE Publications, Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • To Make Their Own Way in the World
    To Make Their Own Way in the World The Enduring Legacy of the Zealy Daguerreotypes Edited by Ilisa Barbash Molly Rogers Deborah Willis 020 PRESIDENT AND FELLOWS OF HARVARD COLLEGE COPYRIGHT © 2 To Make Their Own Way in the World The Enduring Legacy of the Zealy Daguerreotypes Edited by Ilisa Barbash Molly Rogers Deborah Willis With a foreword by Henry Louis Gates, Jr. COPYRIGHT © 2020 PRESIDENT AND FELLOWS OF HARVARD COLLEGE Contents 9 Foreword by Henry Louis Gates, Jr. 15 Preface by Jane Pickering 17 Introduction by Molly Rogers 25 Gallery: The Zealy Daguerreotypes Part I. Photographic Subjects Chapter 1 61 This Intricate Question The “American School” of Ethnology and the Zealy Daguerreotypes by Molly Rogers Chapter 2 71 The Life and Times of Alfred, Delia, Drana, Fassena, Jack, Jem, and Renty by Gregg Hecimovich Chapter 3 119 History in the Face of Slavery A Family Portrait by Evelyn Brooks Higginbotham Chapter 4 151 Portraits of Endurance Enslaved People and Vernacular Photography in the Antebellum South by Matthew Fox-Amato COPYRIGHT © 2020 PRESIDENT AND FELLOWS OF HARVARD COLLEGE Part II. Photographic Practice Chapter 5 169 The Curious Art and Science of the Daguerreotype by John Wood Chapter 6 187 Business as Usual? Scientific Operations in the Early Photographic Studio by Tanya Sheehan Chapter 7 205 Mr. Agassiz’s “Photographic Saloon” by Christoph Irmscher Part III. Ideas and Histories Chapter 8 235 Of Scientific Racists and Black Abolitionists The Forgotten Debate over Slavery and Race by Manisha Sinha Chapter 9 259 “Nowhere Else” South Carolina’s Role in a Continuing Tragedy by Harlan Greene Chapter 10 279 “Not Suitable for Public Notice” Agassiz’s Evidence by John Stauffer Chapter 11 297 The Insistent Reveal Louis Agassiz, Joseph T.
    [Show full text]
  • Physiognomy As a Strategy of Persuasion in Early Christian Discourse
    Physiognomy as a Strategy of Persuasion in Early Christian Discourse by Callie Callon A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department for the Study of Religion University of Toronto © by Callie Callon 2015 Physiognomy as a Strategy of Persuasion in Early Christian Discourse Callie Callon Doctor of Philosophy Department for the Study of Religion University of Toronto 2015 Abstract Ancient physiognomic thought held that the body and soul were intrinsically related, and that observation of a subject's physical appearance provided insight into his or her character. Beyond being a diagnostic tool, however, physiognomy was also used as a strategy of persuasion to bolster or malign an individual's character to an author or speaker's audience, and appears in a host of different ancient genres. However, with a few notable exceptions, the important role that physiognomic thought played in early Christian texts and discourse has not received the scholarly attention it warrants. The following addresses some of the different ways in which physiognomy was employed as a form of rhetoric by early Christians. It was utilized as a means of undermining or discrediting theological opponents or "heretics" on the one hand, but as a strategy for positive self-representation among their own detractors on the other. Some early Christians also used physiognomy to reinforce their understanding of Paul as a philosopher: the description of Paul's physical appearance in the Acts of Paul and Thecla is best understood as designed to evoke the image of the prototypical ancient philosopher in general, and Socrates in particular.
    [Show full text]