<<

Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in , Neurofeedback and Applied Neuroscience Limbic Beta Activation and LORETA: Can Hippocampal and Related Limbic Activity Be Recorded and Changes Visualized Using LORETA in an Affective Condition? Rex Cannon BA a , Joel Lubar PhD b , Keri Thornton BA b , Stuart Wilson BA b & Marco Congedo PhD c a experimental psychology program , University of Tennessee, Research and Neuropsychology Lab , Knoxville, TN b University of Tennessee , Knoxville c National Institute for Research in Informatics and Random Systems (IRISA) , France Published online: 08 Sep 2008.

To cite this article: Rex Cannon BA , Joel Lubar PhD , Keri Thornton BA , Stuart Wilson BA & Marco Congedo PhD (2004) Limbic Beta Activation and LORETA: Can Hippocampal and Related Limbic Activity Be Recorded and Changes Visualized Using LORETA in an Affective Memory Condition?, Journal of Neurotherapy: Investigations in Neuromodulation, Neurofeedback and Applied Neuroscience, 8:4, 5-24

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J184v08n04_02

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

© International Society for Neurofeedback and Research (ISNR), all rights reserved. This article (the “Article”) may be accessed online from ISNR at no charge. The Article may be viewed online, stored in electronic or physical form, or archived for research, teaching, and private study purposes. The Article may be archived in public libraries or university libraries at the direction of said public library or university library. Any other reproduction of the Article for redistribution, sale, resale, loan, sublicensing, systematic supply, or other distribution, including both physical and electronic reproduction for such purposes, is expressly forbidden. Preparing or reproducing derivative works of this article is expressly forbidden. ISNR makes no representation or warranty as to the accuracy or completeness of any content in the Article. From 1995 to 2013 the Journal of Neurotherapy was the official publication of ISNR (www. Isnr.org); on April 27, 2016 ISNR acquired the journal from Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. In 2014, ISNR established its official open-access journal NeuroRegulation (ISSN: 2373-0587; www.neuroregulation.org).

THIS OPEN-ACCESS CONTENT MADE POSSIBLE BY THESE GENEROUS SPONSORS Please note that this electronic prepublication galley may contain typographical errors and may be missing artwork, such as charts, photographs, etc. Pagination in this version will differ from the published version.

SCIENTIFIC ARTICLES

Limbic Beta Activation and LORETA: Can Hippocampal and Related Limbic Activity Be Recorded and Changes Visualized Using LORETA in an Affective Memory Condition?

Rex Cannon, BA Joel Lubar, PhD Keri Thornton, BA Stuart Wilson, BA Marco Congedo, PhD

Rex Cannon is a graduate student working toward a doctoral degree in the experi- mental psychology program at the University of Tennessee, Brain Research and Neuropsychology Lab, Knoxville, TN. Joel Lubar is Professor at the University of Tennessee, Knoxville. Stuart Wilson, and Keri Thornton are students at the University of Tennessee, Knoxville. Marco Congedo is a post-doctoral fellow at the National In- stitute for Research in Informatics and Random Systems (IRISA) in France. Address correspondence to: Rex Cannon, University of Tennessee, Department of Psychology, Brain Research and Neuropsychology Laboratory, Knoxville, TN 37996 (E-mail: [email protected]). The authors give special thanks to Dr. John C. Malone, Dr. Deborah Baldwin, and Jesse Rothove, BA, for sharing their knowledge, time, and editorial assistance. Journal of Neurotherapy, Vol. 8(4) 2004 Copyright © 2004 ISNR. All rights reserved. Digital Object Identifier: 10.1300/J184v08n04_02 5 6 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

ABSTRACT. Background. The purpose of this study was to determine the validity of Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) in visualizing limbic structures and possibly identifying electroenceph- alographic (EEG) frequencies in the limbic region during an anger mem- ory process. Method. This study was conducted with twelve subjects, non-clinical students at the University of Tennessee, Knoxville. A pre-study screen- ing was conducted. Eyes-open baselines were obtained employing 300 epochs, or five minutes, using a 19-channel quantitative electroenceph- alographic (qEEG) acquisition system with linked ear reference. The ex- perimental condition recording directly followed an eyes-open baseline. The experimental condition was to allocate a memory that created in- tense anger and retain the state as long as possible. All files were no less than 100 total epochs upon editing. The data were analyzed in both indi- vidual and group conditions with LORETA imaging software. Statisti- cal differences between conditions were evaluated for significance, then computed and transformed into LORETA images. Results. The data revealed significant differences between the anger condition and baseline recordings in limbic structures and frontal regions. The data suggests that and hippocampal activity can be recorded and visualized using LORETA during affective memory recall. There are several notable differences between the baseline and condition images. One of the more interesting of these differences is possible activation of the , uncinate and surrounding struc- tures in the beta (12-32 Hz) frequencies. The hemispheric asymmetries dur- ing anger memory recall offer further support for the lateralization of hemispheric activity relating to affective states. Conclusion. LORETA may be an effective method used to differentiate and visualize limbic lobe, and other related structures during affective anger memory recall.

KEYWORDS. LORETA, amygdaloid complex, limbic lobe, hippo- campus, anger, , mnemonic processes, qEEG, affective cortical processes, mid-brain beta activation

INTRODUCTION

Research pertaining to the amygdala and hippocampal region is ex- tensive; however, no studies have been conducted on this sub-cortical Scientific Articles 7

area using Low Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography (LORETA) during affective (anger) memory recall. Moreover, studies specific to the amygdala and often use external stimuli or a combina- tion of emotional processes rather than focusing on one specific self- generated emotion. The amygdala and hippocampal formation are re- ported to be involved in declarative memory and emotional processes; however, to date activation of either structure in electroencephalo- graphic (EEG) frequencies remains elusive. The purpose of this study was to inquire as to the validity of LORETA in visualizing limbic struc- tures and possibly identifying EEG frequencies in the limbic region dur- ing an affective anger memory process. LORETA (Pascual-Marqui, 1995, 1999, 2000; Pascual-Marqui, Michel, & Lehmann, 1994; Pascual-Marqui, Esslen, Kochi, & Lehmann, 2002a, 2002b) is currently the most accepted inverse solution procedure (Congedo, 2003), and has been widely used in electrophysiological studies (Bosch-Bayard et al., 2001, Isotani et al., 2001; Pizzagalli et al., 2002; Lehmann et al., 2001; Pascual-Marqui et al., 1999) and has been used in numerous laboratories (Fernández-Bouzas et al., 1999; Fuchs, Wagner, Köhler, & Wischmann, 1999; Gomez & Thatcher, 2001; Prichep, John, & Tom, 2001; Worrell et al., 2000). The inverse problem of neurophysiology suggests that electroencephalogram/magnetoen- cephalogram (EEG/MEG) measurements do not provide enough infor- mation for ascertaining the neuronal activity distribution in the volume the brain occupies (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994). LORETA approxi- mates the distribution of electrical neuronal activity in three-dimen- sional space, basing the approximations on a dense grid of electrodes positioned over the entire scalp (Congedo, 2003). The EEG is a measure of electric potential variations. LORETA estimates the current density that results in a quantifiable potential divergence or the source of the electric field on the scalp (EEG), for which it is feasible to co-register the resolution to a brain atlas (Lancaster et al., 1997, 2000; Talairach & Tournoux, 1988; Towle at al., 1993), and map electrical activity in all cortical structures (Congedo, 2003). LORETA current density is mapped in a space of 2394, 7 ϫ 7 ϫ 7 mm3 voxels. Many of the most recent studies use event-related functional mag- netic resonance imaging (fMRI) techniques and often utilize external stimuli to elicit emotive cortical activity. Damasio et al. (2000) per- formed a positron emission tomography (PET) study on subcortical areas during self-generated emotions, including anger. The results suggest that during self-generated emotional events, limbic and cortical struc- tures that regulate or influence homeostasis are activated. Their study 8 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY reported activation of the insular and cingulate cortices and limbic areas during these affective mnemonic events, and suggested a region of emo- tion-specific neural patterns that receive or project input to thalamic and other sub-cortical structures. Maratos, Dolan, Morris, Henson, and Rugg (2001) suggest that cortical regions activated by environmental factors are also activated when affective are accessed. LORETA methodology has chiefly been exploited to visualize activation in the cortex. Isotani et al. (2001) reported differences between negative, neu- tral and joyful states in the left-posterior regions of the cortex and using LORETA, and also noted a decrease in theta activity during a joyful state as compared with a sad state. Studies involving the hippocampal formation and amygdala suggest that both of these structures and the have different patterns of activation that are dependent on the subject’s state of vigilance (Pare, Collins, & Pelletier, 2002). The amygdala is located in the anterior medial portion of each tempo- ral lobe and is suggested to be involved with declarative memory and emotions associated with mnemonic events (Kolb & Whishaw, 1996). It is suggested to have extensive, complex connections to the prefrontal and frontal cortex and thalamo-cortical structures, and is suggested to be part of a circuit mediating fear responses (Rosenzweig, Leiman, & Breedlove, 1996). The parahippocampal gyrus forms a large part of the limbic lobe along the ventromedial part of the temporal cortical mantle. It has entorhinal and perirhinal sub-divisions and is suggested to be di- rectly involved in both declarative and non-declarative memory pro- cesses (Kolb & Whishaw, 1996). The uncinate gyrus is medial to the amygdala. The amygdala causes a bulge () in the medial part of the . There is little information on these two structures in current literature. Research suggests that the amygdala and frontal cor- tex showed an increased coherence of theta activity associated with arousal, and that arousal is accompanied by increases in amplitude of ‘fast focal activities and amygdala spindles’ and this increase in ‘beta/ gamma activity’ is associated with different behavioral states (Pare et. al., 2002). Damasio et al. (2000) suggest that emotions of happiness, sadness, fear and anger activate or deactivate structures that influence homeostasis, such as the , secondary somatosensory cor- tex, and the anterior and posterior regions of the cingulate gyrus. Their experiment did not report activation of the amygdala for any of the con- ditions in the experiment. In experiments with monkeys, research sug- gests that the hippocampus is involved with spatial and other forms of declarative memory and the amygdala is involved in memory where Scientific Articles 9

there is an emotional context (Kolb & Whishaw, 1996). EEG studies of emotion offer support that hemispheric activity differs during affective states (Rosenzweig et al., 1996; Ahern & Schwartz, 1979), with left activation during more positive emotions and the right for more nega- tive affective processes.

METHODS

Participants. The participants consisted of twelve subjects, eight fe- male and four male non-clinical students at the University of Tennes- see, Knoxville with a mean age of 23. Ten of the subjects were right- handed and two were left-handed. Subjects were excluded under the conditions of previous head trauma, history of epilepsy, drug or alcohol use and any previous psychiatric diagnosis. The subjects received extra credit points for participating in this study. All participants read and signed an informed consent form. Procedure. Participants were prepared for EEG recording using a measure of the distance between the nasion and inion to determine the appropriate cap size for recording (Electrocap, Inc; Blom & Anneveldt, 1982.). The ears and forehead were cleaned for recording with Nuprep, a mild abrasive gel used to remove any oil and dirt from the skin. The caps were fitted and each electrode site was injected with electrogel and prepared so that impedances between individual electrodes and each ear were between 3 and 5 KΩ. The EEG data were then collected using the standard international 10/20 system (FP1, FP2, F3, F4, Fz, F7, F8, C3, C4, Cz, T3, T4, T5, T6, P3, P4, Pz, O1 and O2) and the Lexicor Medical Technology, Inc. Neurosearch-24 EEG analog to digital acquisition system. The data was collected and stored using a Pentium III computer with Lexicor’s V-41e software with a band pass set at 0.5-64.0 Hz for 256 samples per second. All recordings obtained in this study were in the eyes-opened resting condition and were conducted in a comfortably lit, sound attenuated room in the Neuropsychology and Brain Research Laboratory at the University of Tennessee, Knoxville. Both the eyes- open baselines and experimental condition recordings were obtained for 300 epochs or five minutes. Subjects were instructed to control eye movements, blinks and muscle activity from forehead, neck and jaws. The experimental condition recordings were obtained immediately fol- lowing baselines. The subjects were asked to recall and retain a memory that created intense anger and hold the experience as long as possible. 10 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

All subjects reported being able to hold the experience for the length of the recording. The subjects were oblivious to the experimental condi- tion under the hypothesis that a spontaneous memory would be more valid than a planned or suggested memory. The recordings were taken using typical frequencies: delta = 0.5-3.5; theta = 4.0-7.5; Lalpha = 8.0-10.0; Halpha = 10.0-12.0; Lbeta = 12.0-21.0; beta = 21.0-32.0; gamma = 32.0-42.0, respectively. The frequencies within beta band were calculated: beta 1 (12-16 Hz); beta 2 (16-20 Hz); beta 3 (20-24 Hz); beta 4 (24-28 Hz); beta 5 (28-32 Hz). These beta frequencies were extracted in order to observe more specific ranges of activity. Data Analysis. The data collected was stringently artifact rejected; eye blinks, eye movements, teeth clenching, jaw tension, body move- ments and possible EKG were removed from the EEG stream using Eureka3! software (Nova Tech EEG, Inc.). All files were no less than 100 total epochs after editing. The EEG stream was edited as suggested by Thornton (1996). LORETA current density was extracted in all bands using the LORETA-Key package (Key Institute for Brain-Mind Research). Current density for total relative and absolute power was also extracted. The statistical data was analyzed using the Multiple Hy- pothesis Testing (MhyT3!) software (Nova Tech EEG, Inc.). The pro- cedure used is known as the t-max test using the step-down version of the procedure with 1000 and 5000 random data permutations (Blair & Karniski, 1994; Holmes, Blair, Watson, & Ford, 1996, Westfall & Young, 1993). The statistical differences within and between condi- tions were corrected for all multiple comparisons using non-parametric analysis. Baseline and experimental condition files were contrasted by means of voxel by voxel t-statistics. All data was normalized, log trans- formed and smoothed with a 21 mm moving average filter (MAF 3-D) before entering statistical analysis. Type one error (< 0.05) was con- trolled for using family wise error in the strong sense by means of per- mutation theory and maximum t-statistics (Pesarin, 2001). The specific contrast between conditions was entered into one sample t-test corr- esponding to a corrected P < .05. All voxels were examined and statistical data was transformed into LORETA images.

RESULTS

Statistical Data. The average cross-spectral matrices were calculated for each subject in this study and statistical representations were trans- formed into LORETA images for the classical bands mentioned earlier. Scientific Articles 11

The cross-spectral matrices (FFT) were computed and averaged over four-second epochs, which resulted in one cross-spectral matrix for each subject and for each discrete frequency within each band. LORETA current density is computed directly from the cross-spectral matrix and incorporates a three-shell spherical head model registered to the Talairach and Tournoux (1988) anatomical brain atlas and makes use of EEG electrode coordinates derived from the cross-registration between spherical and realistic head geometry (Towle et al., 1993). LORETA has a very limited range of voxels for localization of activity in the limbic region; especially regarding the hippocampus and amygdala. Therefore, identi- fication of artifact-free activity in these regions suggests that it is authentic. Statistical data were analyzed by two separate methods: pri- marily with the multiple hypotheses testing software (Nova Tech EEG, Inc.) and secondly, with non-parametric testing software (LORETA, Key Institute for Brain-Mind Research). The results of both paradigms were similar. The maximum cumulative activation reported by the test-statistics is at (X = 25, Y = Ϫ4, Z = Ϫ20), the amygdala, uncus and limbic lobe. Computations were based on the Talairach and Tournoux probability atlases of the Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurologic Institute (2394 voxels; resolution: 7 mm3). Specific Beta Frequencies. The anger condition and baseline record- ings differ in many respects. Activity in the theta (4-8 Hz) and alpha (8-12 Hz) frequencies appear in all recordings in similar regions, how- ever, they appear to vary in intensity. The differences in alpha (8-12 Hz) and beta (12-32 Hz) specifically appear in the inferior-right-frontal sections, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, amygdala, uncus and insu- lar cortex. Figure 1 is a combined statistically significant representation of the differences between the current density of anger condition and baseline recordings. The regions significant in activation are the uncus, amygdala and hippocampal formation (the areas of interest in this study) and frontal areas. There are other areas that deserve consideration such as the . However, in studies this region is suggested to be more involved with olfactory processes projecting to the ventro- lateral and medial divisions of the amygdala, but not the dorsolateral division of the lateral amygdaloid nucleus (Savander, LeDoux, & Pitkänen, 1996). Figure 2 shows the significant differences between anger condition and baseline recordings in the beta 1 frequency (12-16 Hz). This image suggests that the frontal activity is decreased in both hemispheres but more significantly in the left frontal while there is a significant increase 5

5

0

0

10

10

+5

+5

(Y)

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

(X)

R

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

L

(Z = 22)

(Z = 13)

5

0

5

0

10

+5

10

+5

(Y)

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

R

(X)

(X)

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

L

(Z = 15)

(Z = 20)

5

0

5

0

10

+5

10

+5

Ϫ

Ϫ

(Y)

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

(X)

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

(Z=8)

L

(Z = 27)

0

5

R

5

0

10

+5

10

(Y)

+5

Ϫ

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

(X)

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

L

(Z = 34)

(Z=1)

5

0

5

0

10

+5

10

+5

Ϫ

(Y)

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

(X)

R

R

+5 cm

+5 cm 20) amygdala, uncus, and limbic lobe (F = 1.728, p = 0.0146).

0

0 Ϫ

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ 4, Z =

L

(Z = 6)

L

(Z = 41) Ϫ FIGURE 1. This t-statistical imageand is baseline a recordings cumulative in representation all of frequencies.sents the It areas significant is of a differences statistically horizontal between significant view anger activity= of condition increase the between brain conditions. from The left highest to right. level The of red activation in is the at image (X repre- = 25, Y

12 5

0

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

0

5

Ϫ

L

(Z = 57)

5

0

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

3.361

0

5

Ϫ

1.680

L

(Z = 50)

(Y)

5

0

10

+5

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

0.000

R

+5 cm

1.680

0

Ϫ

5

Ϫ

3.361

Ϫ

L

(Z = 43)

LORETA-KEY

5

0

5

0

10

+5

10

+5

(Y)

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

(X)

R

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

L

(Z = 36)

(Z = 71)

5

0

5

0

10

+5

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

(X)

R

R

+5 cm

+5 cm

0

0

5

5

Ϫ

Ϫ

L

(Z = 64)

L

(Z = 29)

13 5

0

(Z)

+5

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

LORETA-KEY

0

5

Ϫ

L

TF-1

5

0

(Z)

+5

Ϫ

R

10 cm

Ϫ

5

Ϫ

0

иϪ

+5

(X, Y, Z) (32, 46, 13) (mm) : (0.00E+0)

A

(Y)

5

0

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ 13) 18, inferior occipital gyrus and the

Ϫ

(X) Ϫ

R

+5 cm 95, Z =

0 Ϫ

5

Ϫ

L FIGURE 2. This figureand shows baseline the recordings difference(12-16 between Hz). for The the red the anger inactivity the relative condition increase image between power conditions represents while areas the of ofcant blue statistically indicates decrease. the significant areas The of voxel signifi- beta with32, 1 the Y highest = frequency difference in activation is at (X = (t = 2.32, pfrontal = lobe. .0039). The highest level of decrease is in the left

14 Scientific Articles 15

in the right occipital and parietal regions. The significant increase in beta 1 activity can possibly be attributed to the allocation and visualiza- tion of the mnemonic event. Figure 3 shows the statistical differences between the anger condi- tion and baseline recordings in the beta 2 frequency (16-20 Hz). The beta activity appears more pronounced in the amygdala and hippo- campal regions in this frequency, specifically to the amygdala, insular cortex, parahippocampal gyrus, hippocampus, sub-gyral and uncus. It is possible that these structures are primarily involved in an anger/mem- ory circuit. The activity becomes focused in the right in this frequency, which could be attributed to the cognitive evaluation of the stimuli. Figure 4 shows the differences between anger condition and baseline recordings in the beta 3 frequency (20-24 Hz). The significant differ- ences in this image appear in the uncus, hippocampus, amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus. This activation is possibly attributed to the al- location and processing of the memory and the associated emotion. The decrease in activity in the temporal lobes is not at levels (in scaling) for the blue to appear in the image. Figure 5 shows the significant differences between the anger condi- tion and baseline recordings in the beta 4 frequency (24-28 Hz). The significant activation occurs in the and frontal lobe. Again, this could possibly be a cognitive evaluation of the stimuli creating the emotion. The decrease in activation was not at levels (in scaling) for the blue to register in the image. Figure 6 shows the difference between the anger condition and base- line recordings in the beta 5 frequency (28-32 Hz). The activation in this image occurs in the right frontal, anterior and posterior cingulate gyrus and insular cortex. It is possible that the activation in the cingulate gyrus (both anterior and posterior) relates to the suggestion that it is a possible generator or gating mechanism between the and cortex (Pizzagalli, Oakes, & Davidson, 2003). The beta frequencies presented the most significant statistical differences between conditions. The re- gions with the highest statistically significant activation are the right hippocampus, amygdala, uncus, parahippocampal gyrus, insular cortex, sub-gyral, right dorsolateral prefrontal, and frontal lobes. This identifi- cation of the amygdaloid complex contrasts studies in which this area was reportedly decreased, non-active or undetectable in a self-gener- ated anger condition (Damasio et al., 2000). Meanwhile the regions most significantly decreased in activity were the left prefrontal, tempo- ral and frontal regions. This lateralization of activation was not a pre- 5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

LORETA-KEY

0

5

Ϫ

L

TF-1

5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

P

10 cm

Ϫ

5

Ϫ 38, Y = 59, Z = 8) Brodmann area 10, Ϫ

0

иϪ

+5

(X, Y, Z) (18, 3, 13) (mm) : (2.65E+0)

A

(Y)

5

0

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

0

5

Ϫ

L , and frontal lobe. FIGURE 3. This figureand shows baseline the recordings difference(16-20 between Hz). for Red the represents the anger statisticallyity significant relative condition areas as power of increased compared of activ- compared to to the the baseline. baseline beta Thetion maximum while 2 in t-statistic this voxel frequency blue image forfrontal is increased indicates gyrus activa- at and decreased (X frontal = activity lobefor 46, (t decreased Y as = activation = 2.32, is 10, p at Z = (X = .0039). = 22) The Brodmann maximum t-statistic area 44, inferior

16 25, 41)

5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ Ϫ Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

LORETA-KEY 11, Z =

0 Ϫ

5

Ϫ

L

TF-1 52, Y = Ϫ

5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

P

10 cm

Ϫ

5

Ϫ

0

иϪ

+5

(X, Y, Z) (25, 3, 33) (mm) : (4.48E+0)

A

(Y)

0

5

10

+5

(Y)

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

0

5 4, Z = 20) amygdala, parahippocampal gyrus and limbic lobe (t = 4.80, p =

Ϫ Ϫ

L FIGURE 4. This figure shows thebaseline difference recordings between for the the anger relative conditionimage power and of represents beta 3 areas (20-24 ofconditions. Hz). The The statistically maximum red significant t-statistic in for the activity activation increase in this between image is at (X = .0006). The maximum deactivation is at (X = Brodmann area 20, ,in activation and was temporal not lobe. at The levels decrease (in scaling) for the blue to register in the image. Y=

17 0

5

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

LORETA-KEY

0

5

Ϫ 39, Z = 43) Brodmann

L

L Ϫ

TF-1

5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

p

10 cm

Ϫ 31, Y = Ϫ

5

Ϫ

0

иϪ

+5 20) Brodmann area 47, inferior frontal gyrus and

(X, Y, Z) (25, 24, 20) (mm) : (4.41E+0) Ϫ

A

(Y)

5

0

+5

(Y)

10

Ϫ

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

0

5

Ϫ

L FIGURE 5. This figure shows thebaseline difference recordings between for the the anger relative conditionThe power and of red the beta in 4 thecrease frequency (24-28 between image Hz). conditions. represents The areas maximumis t-statistic of at for (X statistically increased = significant activation 25, activity Y in- = 24, Z = frontal lobe (t = 2.32,vation p between = the .0010). The conditions voxel is with at the highest (X decrease = in acti- area 40, inferior parietalnot lobe, at parietal levels lobe. (in The sealing) for decrease the in blue activation to was register in the image.

18 13)

0

5

+5

(Z)

Ϫ Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

LORETA-KEY 60, Z = Ϫ

0

5

Ϫ 38, Y =

L Ϫ

TF-1

5

0

+5

(Z)

Ϫ

P

10 cm

Ϫ

5

Ϫ

0

иϪ

+5

(X, Y, Z) (25, 31, 13) (mm) : (3.86E+0)

A

(Y)

5

0

10

+5

Ϫ

(Y)

Ϫ

(X)

R

+5 cm

0

5

Ϫ

L FIGURE 6. This figure showsbaseline the recordings difference for between theThe the relative power anger red of condition in the and crease beta the 5 between image frequency conditions (28-32 representscrease. Hz). while The areas voxel the of with blue the statisticallyZ maximum indicates = increased significant activation 36) areas is activity Brodmann at of area.0085). in- (X 9, The significant = superior highest 18, de- frontal Y decrease gyrus = in and 52, activation frontal is lobe at (t (X = = 3.20, p = Brodmann area 37, and temporal lobe.

19 20 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

dicted outcome of this study; nonetheless it was observed and contrasts to the meta analysis by Wager, Phan, Liberzon, and Taylor (2003). The reasons for this contrast are unknown at this time; however, they may be attributed to the focus of the experiment being anger from an internal mechanism rather than an external stimuli, or recording one specific emotion rather than a combination of emotions. One benefit to this study could have been to have the participants provide a subjective written record of the memory and to rate the intensity of the event.

DISCUSSION

The anger condition and baseline recordings show significant differ- ences in many regions. The cortical areas of interest for this study were identified by the data and differences in these regions were statistically significant. Alpha (8-12 Hz) and theta (3.5-7.5 Hz) activity were pres- ent in both conditions. However, the intensity of activity differed de- pendent on the condition–in all cases increased activity occurred during the anger condition recordings. The data suggests that when the subject is in an angered state, the right hemisphere and the limbic structures are more active in the beta frequency while limbic structures in the left hemisphere are less active as are the left frontal areas. The data col- lected and analyzed suggest that limbic lobe activity including the amygdaloid complex; hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, uncus, posterior and anterior regions of the cingulate gyrus can be visualized using LORETA. The highest statistical difference for all frequencies between the anger condition and baselines was given at Talairach coor- dinates (X = 25, Y = Ϫ4, Z = Ϫ20) amygdala, uncinate gyrus, and limbic lobe. The highest decrease in activity was at (X = Ϫ45, Y = Ϫ67, Z=Ϫ6) Brodmann area 37 and inferior temporal gyrus. Theta rhythm is suggested to serve as a gating function for information flow in the limbic region and facilitates communication between these structures and the cortex (Pizzagalli, Oakes, & Davidson, 2003; Pizzagalli, Hendrick, Horras, & Davidson, 2002). This idea is supported in that the activity in both the anterior and posterior regions of the cingulate gyrus in the range of 4 to 8 Hz is significant between conditions, with increased acti- vation occurring in the anger condition. Other areas of significant dif- ference are Brodmann areas 21, 13, 29 and the insular cortex. It is interesting that the increase in beta activity occurs principally in the right hemisphere in all the structures in the area of interest in this study. The right amygdaloid complex, hippocampus, insular cortex, parahip- Scientific Articles 21 pocampal gyrus, uncinate gyrus and sub-gyral appear in most beta frequencies, suggesting that anger may be a predominantly right hemi- spheric process including the allocation and possible cognitive evalua- tion of the memory and the associated emotion.

CONCLUSION

The limbic lobe (or limbic system) has been the subject of numerous studies, and remains a focal point for understanding the complexities of human behavior. Technology has allowed us to visualize activity in this region with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and computerized axial tomography (CT) methodologies, yet these studies have used mostly task specific orienta- tion for experiments, which fail to maintain activity in the structures for an adequate amount of time. This study was able to record mnemonic activity, anger processes, and the cortical asymmetries between these states and an eyes-open baseline. It also provided an opportunity to evaluate possible generators for emotional cortical events. Both mne- monic and anger events appear to generate alpha (8-12 Hz), theta (4-8 Hz) but more predominantly beta (12-32 Hz) activity in the limbic lobe including the amygdaloid complex and hippocampal regions, frontal, pre-frontal and parietal areas discussed earlier. Research suggests that these specific structures are involved in circuits of fear or systems of memory (Kolb & Whishaw, 1996). It is not implausible to implicate these structures in a possible anger circuit. Lateralization of activity during affective (anger) memory processes was an observed outcome, but not an expected one and contrasts with results by Wager et al. (2003). The reason for this contrast is not known, but one possibility is that this study focused on one emotional element whereas other similar studies use three or more. Damasio et al. (2000) suggest that the reason for lack of amygdaloid complex activity in their study could be attrib- uted to the skewing of data collection by the emotion-feeling cycle rather than induction of emotion; another is the possible habituation of the amygdala during the feeling phase of the experiment. Kolb and Whishaw (1996) suggest that the hippocampus is responsible for spatial and other declarative memories, while the amygdala stores, or is a con- trol station for, the retrieval of the associated emotional content. The combined data suggest that activity in the limbic lobe, hippocampal for- 22 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

mation and other sub-cortical structures may be recorded and changes can be differentiated and visualized in affective (anger) memory processes by using LORETA. The possibilities this study presents are exciting, but further study is important and necessary to confirm these results.

REFERENCES

Ahern, G. L., & Schwartz, G. E. (1979). Differential lateralization for positive versus negative emotion. Neuropsychologia, 17, 693-698. Blair, R. C., & Karniski, W. (1994) Distribution-free statistical analyses of surface and volumetric maps. In R.W. Thatcher, M. Hallett, E. R. John, & M. Huerta (Eds.), Functional neuroimaging: Technical foundations, (pp. 114-118). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Blom, J. L., & Anneveldt, M., (1982). An electrode cap tested. Electroencephalogra- phy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 54, 591-594. Bosch-Bayard, J., Valdés-Sosa, P., Virues-Alba, T., Aubert-Vázquez, E., John, E. R., Harmony, T., et al. (2001). 3D statistical parametric mapping of EEG source spectra by means of variable resolution electromagnetic tomography (VARETA). Clinical Electroencephalography, 32, 47-61. Congedo, M. (2003). Tomographic neurofeedback: A new technique for the self-regu- lation of brain electrical activity. An unpublished dissertation. University of Ten- nessee, Knoxville. Damasio, R. A., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. B., Parvizi, J., et al. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-gener- ated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3 (10), 1049-1050. Fernández-Bouzas, A., Harmony, T., Bosch-Bayard, J., Aubert-Vázquez, E., Fer- nández, T., Valdés-Sosa, Silva J., et al. (1999). Source of abnormal EEG activity in the presence of brain lesions. Clinical Electroencephalography, 30, 46-52. Fuchs, M., Wagner, M., Köhler, T., & Wischmann, H. (1999). Linear and nonlinear current density reconstructions. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 16 (3), 267- 295. Gomez, J. F., & Thatcher, R. W. (2001). Frequency domain equivalence between po- tentials and currents using LORETA. International Journal of Neuroscience, 167, 161-171. Holmes, A. P., Blair, R. C., Watson, J. D. G., & Ford, I. (1996). Nonparametric analysis of statistic images from functional mapping experiments. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism, 16, 7-22. Isotani, T., Lehmann, D., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Fukushima, M., Saito, N., Yagyu, T., et al. (2001). Source localization of brain electric activity during positive, neutral and negative emotional states. International Congress Series, 1232, 165-173. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1996). Fundamentals of human neuropsychology (4th ed.; pp. 59-168, 289-290, 370-371, 424). New York: W.H. Freeman and Co. Lancaster, J. L., Rainey, L. H., Summerlin, J. L., Freitas., C. S., Fox., P. T., Evans, A. C., et al. (1997). Automated labeling of the : A preliminary report on the development and evaluation of a forward-transform method. Human Brain Mapping, 5, 238-242. Scientific Articles 23

Lancaster, J. L., Woldorff, M. G., Parsons, L. M., Liotti, M., Freitas, C. S., Rainey, L., et al. (2000). Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping. Hu- man Brain Mapping, 10, 120-131. Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., & Pizzagalli, D., (2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitional meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psy- chiatric Research: Neuroimaging Section, 108, 111-121. Maratos, E. J., Dolan, R. J., Morris, J. S., Henson, R. N. A., & Rugg, M. D. (2001). Neu- ral activity associated with episodic memory for emotional context. Neuropsych- ologia, 39, 910-920. Pare, D., Collins, D. R., & Pelletier, J. G. (2002). Amygdala oscillations and the con- solidation of emotional memories. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6-7, 306-314. Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (1995). Reply to comments by Hämäläinen, Ilmonieni, and Nunez. In W. Skrandies (Ed.), Source localization: Continuing discussion on the in- verse problem. ISBET Newsletter, 6, 16-28. Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (1999). Review of methods for solving the EEG inverse prob- lem. International Journal of Bioelectromagnetism, 1 (1), 75-86. Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2002). Standardized low resolution brain electromagnetic to- mography (sLORETA): Technical details. Methods and Findings in Experimental & Clinical Pharmacology, 24D, 5-12. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Lehmann, D., Koenig, T., Kochi, K., Merlo, M. C. G., Hell, D., et al. (1999). Low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) func- tional imaging in acute, neuroleptic-naïve, first-break, productive schizophrenics. Psychiatry Research in Neuroimaging, 90, 169-179. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K., & Lehmann, D. (2002a). Functional im- aging with low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): A re- view. Methods & Findings in Experimental & Clinical Pharmacology, 24C, 91-95. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Esslen, M., Kochi, K., & Lehmann, D. (2002b) Functional im- aging with low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA): Review, new comparisons, and new validation. Japanese Journal of Clinical Neurophys- iology, 30, 81-94. Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., & Lehmann, D. (1994). Low resolution electro- magnetic tomography: A new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 18, 49-65. Pesarin, F. (2001). Multivariate permutation tests. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Pizzagalli, D., Hendrick, A. M., Horras, K. A., & Davidson, R. R. (2002). Anterior cingulate theta activity is associated with degree of treatment in major depression. International Congress Series, 1232, 711-717. Pizzagalli, D., Oakes, T. R., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Coupling of theta and glucose metabolism in the human rostral anterior : An EEG/PET study of normal and depressed subjects. Psychophysiology, 40, 1-11. Prichep, L. S., John, E. R., & Tom, M. (2001). Localization of deep white matter lym- phoma using VARETA: A case study. Clinical Electroencephalography, 32, 62-66. Rosenzweig, M, Leiman, A., & Breedlove, S. M. (1996). Biological psychology (pp. 385-390, 584-620). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, Inc. 24 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Savander, V., LeDoux, J. E., & Pitkänen, A. (1996). Topographic projections from the periamygdaloid cortex to select sub regions of the lateral nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. UKU Neuroscience Publications, Neuroscience Letter, 211, 167-170. Talairach, J., & Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar stereoaxic atlas of the human brain. New York: Theme Medical Publishers. Thornton, K. E. (1996). On the nature of artifacting the qEEG. Journal of Neuro- therapy, 1 (3), 31-39. Towle, V. L., Bolaños, J., Suarez, D., Tan, K., Grzeszczuk, R., Levin, D. N., et al. (1993). The spatial location of EEG electrodes: Locating the best fitting sphere rela- tive to cortical anatomy. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 86, 1-6. Wager, T. D. K., Phan, L., Liberzon, I., & Taylor, S. F. (2003). Valence, gender and lateralization of functional brain anatomy in emotion: A meta-analysis of findings from neuroimaging. Neuroimage, 19, 513-531. Westfall, P. H., & Young S. S. (1993). Resampling-based multiple testing. Examples and methods for p-values adjustment. New York: John Wiley & Sons. Worrell, G. A., Lagerlund, T. D., Sharbrough F. W., Brinckmann, B. H., Bucacker, N. E., Cicora, K. M., et al. (2000). Localization of the epileptic focus by low-resolution electromagnetic tomography in patients with a lesion demonstrated by MRI. Brain Topography, 12, 273-282.

RECEIVED: 12/17/03 REVISED: 02/23/04 06/14/04 ACCEPTED: 07/07/04