Spatial Maps in Frontal and Prefrontal Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Spatial Maps in Frontal and Prefrontal Cortex www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 29 (2006) 567 – 577 Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex Donald J. Hagler Jr. and Martin I. Sereno* Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr. #0515, La Jolla, CA 92093-0515, USA Received 20 April 2005; revised 22 July 2005; accepted 2 August 2005 Available online 11 November 2005 Though the function of prefrontal cortex has been extensively Working memory tasks also activate prefrontal cortex. investigated, little is known about the internal organization of Physiological recordings in monkeys have demonstrated that individual prefrontal areas. Functional magnetic resonance imaging prefrontal areas have spatially selective memory fields that was used to show that some frontal and prefrontal cortical areas primarily represent restricted regions of contralateral space represent visual space in orderly, reproducible, topographic maps. The (Funahashi et al., 1989; Rainer et al., 1998). It seems plausible map-containing areas partly overlap dorsolateral prefrontal areas that maps of visual space might be preserved, rather than engaged by working memory tasks. These maps may be useful for attending to task-relevant objects at various spatial locations, an aspect scrambled, as posterior visual areas project to frontal and of the executive control of attention. prefrontal cortex. Indeed, Sawaguchi and Iba have recently D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. identified one or more spatial maps in monkey dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) related to working memory and attention using delayed saccade paradigms (Sawaguchi and Iba, 2001). These maps may primarily be used to hold locations in Introduction memory for spatial working memory tasks or they might be more generally useful for a variety of tasks that require the allocation The prefrontal cortex has often been implicated in working of working memory and attention. The homologies between memory processes. Several more specific functions have been monkey and human frontal and prefrontal areas are not proposed for this region, including maintenance and manipulation completely clear, and so the relationship between these macaque of information in working memory (Goldman-Rakic, 1987; Owen monkey prefrontal maps and working-memory-related dorso- et al., 1996), attentional control (Corbetta et al., 1998), choosing lateral prefrontal areas in humans remains to be clarified. between alternative responses (Rowe et al., 2000), and representa- In this paper, we ask whether working-memory-related areas in tion and storage of the rules of a task (Miller et al., 2002; Derrfuss human DLPFC contain neurons with preferences for working et al., 2004). The neuronal circuitry required to implement these memory stimuli appearing at particular spatial locations, and if so, functions is not yet well understood. whether these neurons are arrayed in a topological map of visual Mammalian brains represent visual space in multiple retino- space. To address this question, we had subjects perform a working topic visual cortical areas. Though all of the maps in V1, V2, V3, memory task with peripherally displayed images, steadily varying VP, V4v, and V3A represent all or part of the same contralateral the polar angle of presentation. We have found that at least two of visual hemifield, each area provides a unique mixture of the frontal and prefrontal areas active during this task contain such preferences for particular stimulus properties. The topology of maps of visual space. visual space is preserved to some degree in these maps, in that The n-back working memory task is conceptually simple, yet neighboring patches of cortex represent neighboring parts of the it requires a complex set of mental functions to perform. A visual field, forming continuous maps across the cortical surface. A subject monitors for instances in which a stimulus is repeated similar mapping of contralateral visual space across the cortical after n À1 intervening stimuli. This task requires attention to surface was recently revealed in the putative human lateral stimulus features and feature comparison, the encoding and intraparietal area (LIP), located in posterior parietal cortex (Sereno retrieval of stimuli in working memory, the maintenance, et al., 2001). Unlike the maps in many earlier visual areas, temporal ordering, and refreshing of working memory contents, however, this map only became evident when the subject was and finally – when an overt response such as a button press is required to engage attention and working memory. required – the choice, initiation, and sometimes suppression of motor responses. These subtasks require the cooperation of several cortical areas known to be involved in aspects of * Corresponding author. Fax: +1 858 534 1128. attention, working memory, and motor planning, including the E-mail address: [email protected] (M.I. Sereno). frontal eye fields (FEF), posterior parietal cortex (PPC), DLPFC, Available online on ScienceDirect (www.sciencedirect.com). and premotor cortex (Simon et al., 2002). 1053-8119/$ - see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.058 568 D.J. Hagler Jr., M.I. Sereno / NeuroImage 29 (2006) 567–577 Materials and methods or location – for the identity blocks was identical to that for location blocks. The number of randomly chosen locations, eight, Participants was also identical for all three block types. Simultaneous identity and location matches were disallowed to reduce the likelihood that A total of 21 adults participated in this study (7 women). The subjects would focus on the identity of the face during a location mean age was 25 T 7 years (ranging from 19 to 47). All subjects block or vice versa. To prevent spontaneous recognition of were right handed and had normal or corrected to normal vision. matches, no 2-back matches of any kind occurred during the The experimental protocol was approved by the UCSD internal passive blocks. Additional tests outside the scanner with an optical review board, and informed consent was obtained from each eye-tracker confirmed that several subjects included in this study participant. There were two types of experimental designs used in were able to maintain precise, reliable, central fixation during these this study, block design and phase-encoded. Twelve subjects tasks. participated in the block design experiment, and 20 participated in the phase-encoded experiment, with 11 subjects participating in Analysis of block design fMRI experiments both experiments. Before scanning, subjects were allowed, if they desired, to consume caffeinated beverages in order to better Distortion in images was reduced with an image-based, B0-map maintain alertness during the scan session. correction method (Reber et al., 1998). Volume-registered block design experiment images were analyzed using AFNI’s (Analysis fMRI scanning of Functional NeuroImages) (Cox, 1996) 3dDeconvolve (Ward, B.D., Medical College of Wisconsin, 1998). A quadratic poly- A Varian 4 T or a GE 3 T scanner was used with a head coil, a nomial was used to fit the baseline. Motion estimates from single small surface coil, or a whole-head array of four or eight 3dvolreg were added to the baseline model to further reduce the small surface coils (Nova Medical, Wakefield MA; General contribution of motion to activation patterns. Correlation coef- Electric). See Supplementary Table 1 for individual scan informa- ficients and F statistics were generated for the area under the tion for each subject. An echo planar T2*-weighted gradient echo hemodynamic response function, which freely varied over 15 s. pulse sequence (8V 32VV scan time, TR = 2000 or 4000 ms for For each subject in the group analysis, their cortical white matter phase-encoded, 8V 15VV scan time, TR = 3000 ms for block design, surface was inflated to a sphere and warped into a best-fit sulcal TE = 26.3 ms, flip angle = 90-, bandwidth = 125 kHz, 64  64 alignment with an average of 40 spherical surfaces using Free- matrix, 21–36 axial slices, 3  3  3 mm or 3.75  3.75  3.8 Surfer (Fischl et al., 1999). After resampling the deconvolution mm voxels) was used for functional scans. A T1-weighted coefficients onto the average spherical surface, 10 steps of surface- MPRAGE scan (TR = 10.5 ms, TE = 4.8 ms, flip angle = 11-, based smoothing were performed, equivalent to a 4 mm full-width, bandwidth = 50 kHz, 256Â256 matrix, 84 axial slices, 1  1Â1.5 half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian filter (Hagler et al., in prepa- mm voxels) was also acquired during each session to align the ration). AFNI’s 3dttest was used to generate t statistics and means functional images to a previously obtained (1.5, 3, or 4 T) high for each condition and the identity vs. location contrast. To resolution (1Â1Â1 mm) T1-weighted MPRAGE scan. Subjects’ facilitate viewing, results were sampled back onto the surface of a heads were immobilized with subject-specific dental impression single subject. Cluster size exclusion was used for multiple bite-bars supported by a 4-ball-joint yoke. A custom image display comparison correction (Forman et al., 1995), with t statistics program, run on a Silicon Graphics O2, was used to present images thresholded at P < 0.05 and surface clusters smaller than 145 (¨5- wide¨6- high) centered at ¨8- eccentricity. Images were contiguous vertices excluded. A cortical-surface-based program projected onto a screen over the subject’s ribcage using a standard adapted from AFNI’s AlphaSim (Ward, 2000) was used to estimate video projector with a 7.38–12.3VV focal length Xtra Bright Zoom that these thresholds result in a corrected P value of 0.05 (Hagler et lens (Buhl Optical, USA). Subjects viewed the screen indirectly via al., in preparation). For comparison, group statistics were also a mirror above the eyes. generated from Talairach-transformed, 4 mm FWHM Gaussian smoothed, 3D volume data ( P < 0.05, uncorrected). Stimuli Analysis of phase-encoded fMRI experiments Gray-scale images of 68 different faces were selected from the FERET database (Phillips et al., 1998), excluding those with facial A Fourier transform of a time series generates a vector with real hair or glasses.
Recommended publications
  • Connectivity of BA46 Involvement in the Executive Control of Language
    Alfredo Ardila, Byron Bernal and Monica Rosselli Psicothema 2016, Vol. 28, No. 1, 26-31 ISSN 0214 - 9915 CODEN PSOTEG Copyright © 2016 Psicothema doi: 10.7334/psicothema2015.174 www.psicothema.com Connectivity of BA46 involvement in the executive control of language Alfredo Ardila1, Byron Bernal2 and Monica Rosselli3 1 Florida International University, 2 Miami Children’s Hospital and 3 Florida Atlantic University Abstract Resumen Background: Understanding the functions of different brain areas has Estudio de la conectividad del AB46 en el control ejecutivo del lenguaje. represented a major endeavor of contemporary neurosciences. Modern Antecedentes: la comprensión de las funciones de diferentes áreas neuroimaging developments suggest cognitive functions are associated cerebrales representa una de las mayores empresas de las neurociencias with networks rather than with specifi c areas. Objectives. The purpose contemporáneas. Los estudios contemporáneos con neuroimágenes of this paper was to analyze the connectivity of Brodmann area (BA) 46 sugieren que las funciones cognitivas se asocian con redes más que con (anterior middle frontal gyrus) in relation to language. Methods: A meta- áreas específi cas. El propósito de este estudio fue analizar la conectividad analysis was conducted to assess the language network in which BA46 is del área de Brodmann 46 (BA46) (circunvolución frontal media anterior) involved. The DataBase of Brainmap was used; 19 papers corresponding con relación al lenguaje. Método: se llevó a cabo un meta-análisis para to 60 experimental conditions with a total of 245 subjects were included. determinar el circuito o red lingüística en la cual participa BA46. Se utilizó Results: Our results suggest the core network of BA46.
    [Show full text]
  • Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex-Based Control with an Implanted Brain–Computer Interface
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex‑based control with an implanted brain–computer interface Sacha Leinders 1, Mariska J. Vansteensel 1, Mariana P. Branco 1, Zac V. Freudenburg1, Elmar G. M. Pels1, Benny Van der Vijgh1, Martine J. E. Van Zandvoort2, Nicolas F. Ramsey1* & Erik J. Aarnoutse 1 The objective of this study was to test the feasibility of using the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as a signal source for brain–computer interface control in people with severe motor impairment. We implanted two individuals with locked‑in syndrome with a chronic brain–computer interface designed to restore independent communication. The implanted system (Utrecht NeuroProsthesis) included electrode strips placed subdurally over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. In both participants, counting backwards activated the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex consistently over the course of 47 and 22months, respectively. Moreover, both participants were able to use this signal to control a cursor in one dimension, with average accuracy scores of 78 ± 9% (standard deviation) and 71 ± 11% (chance level: 50%), respectively. Brain– computer interface control based on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity is feasible in people with locked‑in syndrome and may become of relevance for those unable to use sensorimotor signals for control. Locked-in syndrome (LIS) is characterized by intact cognition and (almost) complete loss of voluntary movement1. Brain–computer interface (BCI) systems allow computer control with brain activity and can there- fore provide an alternative communication channel to people with LIS. BCI systems depending on self-initiated modulation of brain activity generally measure from sensorimotor areas, e.g.2–9. However, not all people with LIS may be able to reliably modulate sensorimotor activity, due to for instance atrophy secondary to neurode- generative disease10,11, damage afer stroke or injury, or atypical neural activity 12.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Correlates Underlying Change in State Self-Esteem Hiroaki Kawamichi 1,2,3, Sho K
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Neural correlates underlying change in state self-esteem Hiroaki Kawamichi 1,2,3, Sho K. Sugawara2,4,5, Yuki H. Hamano2,5,6, Ryo Kitada 2,7, Eri Nakagawa2, Takanori Kochiyama8 & Norihiro Sadato 2,5 Received: 21 July 2017 State self-esteem, the momentary feeling of self-worth, functions as a sociometer involved in Accepted: 11 January 2018 maintenance of interpersonal relations. How others’ appraisal is subjectively interpreted to change Published: xx xx xxxx state self-esteem is unknown, and the neural underpinnings of this process remain to be elucidated. We hypothesized that changes in state self-esteem are represented by the mentalizing network, which is modulated by interactions with regions involved in the subjective interpretation of others’ appraisal. To test this hypothesis, we conducted task-based and resting-state fMRI. Participants were repeatedly presented with their reputations, and then rated their pleasantness and reported their state self- esteem. To evaluate the individual sensitivity of the change in state self-esteem based on pleasantness (i.e., the subjective interpretation of reputation), we calculated evaluation sensitivity as the rate of change in state self-esteem per unit pleasantness. Evaluation sensitivity varied across participants, and was positively correlated with precuneus activity evoked by reputation rating. Resting-state fMRI revealed that evaluation sensitivity was positively correlated with functional connectivity of the precuneus with areas activated by negative reputation, but negatively correlated with areas activated by positive reputation. Thus, the precuneus, as the part of the mentalizing system, serves as a gateway for translating the subjective interpretation of reputation into state self-esteem.
    [Show full text]
  • Healthy and Abnormal Development of the Prefrontal Cortex
    Developmental Neurorehabilitation, October 2009; 12(5): 279–297 Healthy and abnormal development of the prefrontal cortex MEGAN SPENCER-SMITH1,2 & VICKI ANDERSON1,2,3 1Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia, 2University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia, and 3Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia (Received 8 October 2008; accepted 3 June 2009) Abstract Background: While many children with brain conditions present with cognitive, behavioural, emotional, academic and social impairments, other children recover with seemingly few impairments. Animal studies and preliminary child studies have identified timing of brain lesion as a key predictor in determining functional outcome following early brain lesions. Review: This research suggests that knowledge of healthy developmental processes in brain structure and function is essential for better understanding functional recovery and outcome in children with brain lesions. This review paper aims to equip researchers with current knowledge of key principles of developmental processes in brain structure and function. Timetables for development of the prefrontal cortex (PFC), a brain region particularly vulnerable to lesions due to its protracted developmental course, are examined. In addition, timetables for development of executive skills, which emerge in childhood and have a prolonged developmental course that parallels development of the PFC, are also discussed. Conclusions: Equipped with this knowledge, researchers are now in a better position to understand functional
    [Show full text]
  • Human Prefrontal Cortex: Processing and Representational Perspectives
    REVIEWS HUMAN PREFRONTAL CORTEX: PROCESSING AND REPRESENTATIONAL PERSPECTIVES Jacqueline N. Wood and Jordan Grafman Through evolution, humans have acquired ‘higher’ cognitive skills — such as language, reasoning and planning — and complex social behaviour. Evidence from neuropsychological and neuroimaging research indicates that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) underlies much of this higher cognition. A number of theories have been proposed for how the PFC might achieve this. Although many of these theories focus on the types of ‘process’ that the PFC carries out, we argue for the validity of a representational approach to understanding PFC function. Although it is clear that the PFC is important for higher The first of our five proposed criteria is that a theory cognitive skills, particularly in humans, how it achieves must be explicit about the information that is stored in these functions is unknown. The human PFC is not nec- the PFC. Does it store information akin to a memory essarily larger than that of other primate species1, but its function (representational approach)? Does it store neural architecture is probably more sophisticated or algorithms or computational procedures only for organized differently to accommodate higher cognitive manipulating information stored elsewhere in the brain functions that are superior to those of related species2,3. (processing approach)? Does it do a combination of Researchers have proposed a number of theories of PFC these things (hybrid approach)? Second, the theory function, many of which centre around the representa- must be consistent with our knowledge of stimulus rep- tions or processes that are mediated by the PFC. We resentation in the brain.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Pathways to Functional Brain Networks: Emerging Principles
    TICS-1251; No. of Pages 14 Review Special Issue: The Connectome Developmental pathways to functional brain networks: emerging principles 1,2,3 V. Menon 1 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA 2 Department of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA 3 Program in Neuroscience, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94304, USA The human brain undergoes protracted developmental disorders. The perspective advanced in this review is that changes during which it constructs functional networks a thorough understanding of the functional architecture of that engender complex cognitive abilities. Understand- the adult brain requires critical consideration of the devel- ing brain function ultimately depends on knowledge of opmental pathways by which plasticity and learning lead how dynamic interactions between distributed brain to the construction of dedicated large-scale brain systems. regions mature with age to produce sophisticated cog- Most, if not all, major psychopathologies, with the excep- nitive systems. This review summarizes recent progress tion of the dementias, have a prominent origin in childhood in our understanding of the ontogeny of functional brain or adolescence [4]. The onset and diagnosis of these psycho- networks. Here I describe how complementary methods pathologies vary greatly: some, like autism, are mainly for probing functional connectivity are providing unique diagnosed in early childhood, others such as attention deficit insights into the emergence and maturation of distinct hyperactivity disorder and anxiety disorders are mainly functional networks from childhood to adulthood. diagnosed in middle childhood, whereas bipolar disorder, I highlight six emerging principles governing the devel- depression, and schizophrenia are predominantly diag- opment of large-scale functional networks and discuss nosed in late adolescence.
    [Show full text]
  • A Brief Anatomical Sketch of Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Jamil P
    This article is a supplement referenced in Delgado, M. R., Beer, J. S., Fellows, L. K., Huettel, S. A., Platt, M. L., Quirk, G. J., & Schiller, D. (2016). Viewpoints: Dialogues on the functional role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 19(12), 1545-1552. Brain images used in this article (vmPFC mask) are available at https://identifiers.org/neurovault.collection:5631 A Brief Anatomical Sketch of Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Jamil P. Bhanji1, David V. Smith2, Mauricio R. Delgado1 1 Department of Psychology, Rutgers University - Newark 2 Department of Psychology, Temple University The ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) is a major focus of investigation in human neuroscience, particularly in studies of emotion, social cognition, and decision making. Although the term vmPFC is widely used, the zone is not precisely defined, and for varied reasons has proven a complicated region to study. A difficulty identifying precise boundaries for the vmPFC comes partly from varied use of the term, sometimes including and sometimes excluding ventral parts of anterior cingulate cortex and medial parts of orbitofrontal cortex. These discrepancies can arise both from the need to refer to distinct sub-regions within a larger area of prefrontal cortex, and from the spatially imprecise nature of research methods such as human neuroimaging and natural lesions. The inexactness of the term is not necessarily an impediment, although the heterogeneity of the region can impact functional interpretation. Here we briefly address research that has helped delineate sub-regions of the human vmPFC, we then discuss patterns of white matter connectivity with other regions of the brain and how they begin to inform functional roles within vmPFC.
    [Show full text]
  • What Does the Prefrontal Cortex “Do” in Affect: Perspectives on Frontal EEG Asymmetry Research
    Biological Psychology 67 (2004) 219–233 Commentary What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: perspectives on frontal EEG asymmetry research Richard J. Davidson∗ Laboratory for Affective Neuroscience, W.M. Keck Laboratory for Functional Neuroimaging and Behavior, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI 53706, USA Abstract This commentary provides reflections on the current state of affairs in research on EEG frontal asymmetries associated with affect. Although considerable progress has occurred since the first report on this topic 25 years ago, research on frontal EEG asymmetries associated with affect has largely evolved in the absence of any serious connection with neuroscience research on the structure and function of the primate prefrontal cortex (PFC). Such integration is important as this work progresses since the neuroscience literature can help to understand what the prefrontal cortex is “doing” in affective processing. Data from the neuroscience literature on the heterogeneity of different sectors of the PFC are introduced and more specific hypotheses are offered about what different sectors of the PFC might be doing in affect. A number of methodological issues associated with EEG measures of functional prefrontal asymmetries are also considered. © 2004 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Frontal EEG asymmetry; Emotion; Prefrontal cortex; Neuroimaging 1. Introduction It is very gratifying for me personally to be writing this commentary since the publi- cation of this Special Issue marks the fact that there now exists a substantial corpus of scientific literature investigating methodological and conceptual issues surrounding the use of measures of asymmetric prefrontal electrical signals recorded from the scalp surface to make inferences about emotional processes.
    [Show full text]
  • A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems
    REVIEW ARTICLE FUNCTIONAL A Practical Review of Functional MRI Anatomy of the Language and Motor Systems X V.B. Hill, X C.Z. Cankurtaran, X B.P. Liu, X T.A. Hijaz, X M. Naidich, X A.J. Nemeth, X J. Gastala, X C. Krumpelman, X E.N. McComb, and X A.W. Korutz ABSTRACT SUMMARY: Functional MR imaging is being performed with increasing frequency in the typical neuroradiology practice; however, many readers of these studies have only a limited knowledge of the functional anatomy of the brain. This text will delineate the locations, anatomic boundaries, and functions of the cortical regions of the brain most commonly encountered in clinical practice—specifically, the regions involved in movement and language. ABBREVIATIONS: FFA ϭ fusiform face area; IPL ϭ inferior parietal lobule; PPC ϭ posterior parietal cortex; SMA ϭ supplementary motor area; VOTC ϭ ventral occipitotemporal cortex his article serves as a review of the functional areas of the brain serving to analyze spatial position and the ventral stream working Tmost commonly mapped during presurgical fMRI studies, to identify what an object is. Influenced by the dorsal and ventral specifically targeting movement and language. We have compiled stream model of vision, Hickok and Poeppel2 hypothesized a sim- what we hope is a useful, easily portable, and concise resource that ilar framework for language. In this model, the ventral stream, or can be accessible to radiologists everywhere. We begin with a re- lexical-semantic system, is involved in sound-to-meaning map- view of the language-processing system. Then we describe the pings associated with language comprehension and semantic ac- gross anatomic boundaries, organization, and function of each cess.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Correlates of Strategic Memory Retrieval: Differentiating Between Spatial-Associative and Temporal-Associative Strategies
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by MPG.PuRe r Human Brain Mapping 29:1068–1079 (2008) r Neural Correlates of Strategic Memory Retrieval: Differentiating Between Spatial-Associative and Temporal-Associative Strategies Mischa de Rover,1* Karl Magnus Petersson,1 Sieberen P. van der Werf,2 Alexander R. Cools,3 Hans J. Berger,2 and Guille´n Ferna´ndez1,4 1F.C. Donders Center for Cognitive Neuroimaging, Radboud University Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 2Department of Medical Psychology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 3Department of Psychoneuropharmacology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands 4Department of Neurology, University Medical Centre Nijmegen, 6500 HB Nijmegen, The Netherlands Abstract: Remembering complex, multidimensional information typically requires strategic memory re- trieval, during which information is structured, for instance by spatial- or temporal associations. Although brain regions involved in strategic memory retrieval in general have been identified, differences in re- trieval operations related to distinct retrieval strategies are not well-understood. Thus, our aim was to identify brain regions whose activity is differentially involved in spatial-associative and temporal-associa- tive retrieval. First, we showed that our behavioral paradigm probing memory for a set of object-location associations promoted the use of a spatial-associative structure following an encoding condition
    [Show full text]
  • Frontal Lobe and Cognitive Development
    Journal of Neurocytology 31, 373–385 (2002) Frontal lobe and cognitive development JOAQUIN´ M. FUSTER Neuropsychiatric Institute and Brain Research Institute, UCLA School of Medicine Los Angeles, California [email protected] Received December 1, 2002; accepted December 12, 2002 Abstract In phylogeny as in ontogeny, the association cortex of the frontal lobe, also known as the prefrontal cortex, is a late-developing region of the neocortex. It is also one of the cortical regions to undergo the greatest expansion in the course of both evolution and individual maturation. In the human adult, the prefrontal cortex constitutes as much as nearly one-third of the totality of the neocortex. The protracted, relatively large, development of the prefrontal cortex is manifest in gross morphology as well as fine structure. In the developing individual, its late maturation is made most apparent by the late myelination of its axonal connections. This and other indices of morphological development of the prefrontal cortex correlate with the development of cognitive functions that neuropsychological studies in animals and humans have ascribed to this cortex. In broad outline, the ventromedial areas of the prefrontal cortex, which with respect to other prefrontal areas develop relatively early, are involved in the expression and control of emotional and instinctual behaviors. On the other hand, the late maturing areas of the lateral prefrontal convexity are principally involved in higher executive functions. The most general executive function of the lateral prefrontal cortex is the temporal organization of goal-directed actions in the domains of behavior, cognition, and language. In all three domains, that global function is supported by a fundamental role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in temporal integration, that is, the integration of temporally discontinuous percepts and neural inputs into coherent structures of action.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Pharmacological Opioid Blockade on Neural Measures of Drug Cue-Reactivity in Humans
    Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41, 2872–2881 © 2016 American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. All rights reserved 0893-133X/16 www.neuropsychopharmacology.org The Effects of Pharmacological Opioid Blockade on Neural Measures of Drug Cue-Reactivity in Humans 1 2 ,1,2 Kelly E Courtney , Dara G Ghahremani and Lara A Ray* 1 2 Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA; Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA Interactions between dopaminergic and opioidergic systems have been implicated in the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. The present study investigated the effects of opioid blockade, via naltrexone, on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) measures during methamphetamine cue-reactivity to elucidate the role of endogenous opioids in the neural systems underlying drug craving. To investigate = = = this question, non-treatment seeking individuals with methamphetamine use disorder (N 23; 74% male, mean age 34.70 (SD 8.95)) were recruited for a randomized, placebo controlled, within-subject design and underwent a visual methamphetamine cue-reactivity task during two blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) fMRI sessions following 3 days of naltrexone (50 mg) and matched time for placebo. fMRI analyses tested naltrexone-induced differences in BOLD activation and functional connectivity during cue processing. The results showed that naltrexone administration reduced cue-reactivity in sensorimotor regions and related to altered functional connectivity of dorsal striatum, ventral tegmental area, and precuneus with frontal, visual, sensory, and motor-related regions. Naltrexone also weakened the associations between subjective craving and precuneus functional connectivity with sensorimotor regions and strengthened the associations between subjective craving and dorsal striatum and precuneus connectivity with frontal regions.
    [Show full text]