Is Cognitive Neuroscience an Oxymoron? 1 Forthcoming: Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eds

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Is Cognitive Neuroscience an Oxymoron? 1 Forthcoming: Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eds Is cognitive neuroscience an oxymoron? 1 Forthcoming: Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eds. S. Cullen, S.J. Leslie & A. Lerner Is cognitive neuroscience an oxymoron? Fiery Cushman 1. Introduction have a clear image in mind of just what “cognitive research” is. Cognitive theories Cognitive neuroscience research requires a attempt to describe the functional organization great deal of time and money, and it attracts a of the mind (Gallistel, 1990). The guiding great deal of attention. Consequently, there is a principle of the cognitive revolution is that we subversive temptation to declare it useless. This can understand how the mind processes general grumble comprises many related gripes: information. Specifically, we can characterize Cognitive neuroscience is poorly executed; its mental activity as a set of representations and costs do not justify its benefits; for a long time we computations over those representations. This all got along just fine without it, and I still do; it’s depends crucially upon an understanding of the just a fad; people are just wowed by pictures of function of the mental processes under study brains; etc. (Marr, 1982). Still, cognition is the study of mental compu- Some classic examples of psychological tations that are performed by a neural machine. research questions formulated in this framework It is remarkable to suppose that studying the are visual object recognition, language machine itself would not usefully inform production, and theory of mind. In the case of cognitive research. So which is it? Does visual object recognition, for instance, it is neuroscience illuminate cognitive research, or is assumed that the mind begins with a pixel-like it the moon that never rose? Is cognitive representation of the wavelengths and neuroscience an oxymoron? luminosities of light across the retina. It ends This essay dodges numerous gripes. It does with the capacity to categorize objects in the not address whether most cognitive neurosci- visual scene—e.g., categorizing one as a CAT and ence is well executed; whether its benefits another as HAT. This occurs as successive typically outweigh its costs; whether neurosci- computations are performed on the early ence could ever be necessary to develop a representation, transforming it over a series of psychological theory; or whether its celebrity representations. outstrips its merit. But it does conclude that This commitment to characterizing the neuroscience can play important role in internal representations that mediate between cognitive research. The argument has three percept and action is precisely why behaviorists parts: A general account of the relationship found the cognitive revolution so revolting. between neuroscience and cognition, a specific Behaviorism is committed, of course, to merely case study of the contribution of neuroscience to characterizing the relationship between a the study of visual cognition, and a quantitative stimulus and a response. This is a kind of analysis of citation records in the journal psychological theory. Indeed, it aptly describes Cognition. the kinds of theories of human behavior applied in some other disciplines. For instance, several 1.1 What is cognitive research? key principles of microeconomics do not attempt to mirror the representations or computations Before asking whether cognitive neurosci- involved in choice, except insofar as they ence contributes to cognitive research, it helps to appropriately characterize the relationship 1 Is cognitive neuroscience an oxymoron? 2 Forthcoming: Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eds. S. Cullen, S.J. Leslie & A. Lerner between input and output of a human chooser of theory they seek to advance, this would beg over time. Such theories achieve descriptive the question of whether neuroscience methods adequacy, in the sense that they accurately (e.g., cognitive neuroscience) can inform describe and predict human behavior (Chomsky, cognitive theory. The answer would be 1965). Cognitive theories, in contrast, guaranteed to be “no” for a trivial reason: Any additionally achieve explanatory adequacy. That study using apparent “neuroscience” methods to is, they aim to characterize the set of internal answer cognitive questions would simply have representations and computations that provide been “cognitive” research all along, by definition. an explanation for the reliability of descriptive relations between input and output. Thus, if 1.3 What is “important”? neuroscience plays an important role in cognitive research, it must do so by constraining theories What does it mean to say that neuroscience of representation and computation. has played, or could play, an “important” role in the development of cognitive theories? This 1.2 What is neuroscience? question is most interesting if it is reframed pragmatically: If we are interested in developing Neuroscience is the study of the nervous an accurate model of cognition, is it a useful system, including its physical and biological strategy to seek evidence from neuroscience properties. Like all cognitive theories, many methods? neuroscience theories are motivated by the Thus, neuroscience could be a useful source ultimate desire to explain the functional of evidence without being sufficient for the organization of the mind. Yet theories of development of cognitive theories. In other neuroscience can be distinguished from words, having a complete theory of cognition cognitive theories in part because they are stated may require us to know about more than just the over physical and biological terms (or explicit brain. A complete cognitive theory of visual abstractions from them) rather than over processing, for instance, might depend upon exclusively representational and computational information about typical scenes in natural terms. contexts (i.e., the things we see), as well as the Neuroscience comprises not only a set of functional role of vision in guiding human theories, but also a set of methods. A neurosci- interactions in the world. Whether cognitive ence method involves either manipulating or theories must be stated over terms external to measuring neural processes directly. In other the nervous system is a matter of significant words, it either involves an intervention upon philosophical controversy. But, even if we adopt neural processes in a manner that is not the position that a theory of mental representa- mediated by ordinary perception (e.g., neural tion and computation must be stated partially stimulation by electrical or magnetic forces, over terms external to the nervous system, the lesion by natural or artificial means, experi- neurosciences could still play a useful role in mental control of gene expression, etc.), or else it developing those theories. involves measuring neural processes in a manner Similarly, neuroscience could be a useful that is not mediated by ordinary behavior (e.g., source of evidence in developing cognitive EEG, MRI, single unit recording, etc.). In theories without being a definitive source of contrast, in “behavioral” methods, the evidence. (Indeed, it’s hard to say whether any experimenter’s access to mental processes is form of scientific evidence is ever definitive). mediated by ordinary perceptual and behavioral Presumably neuroscience will constrain processes. cognitive theories via inference to the best It is not necessary that neuroscience meth- explanation. That is, certain cognitive theories ods are used exclusively to inform neuroscience will provide a better explanation for the theories, or that behavioral methods are used neuroscience evidence, others will provide a exclusively to inform cognitive theories. This worse explanation, and we can use this to inform point is crucial: If we defined methods in terms our assessment of the quality of the cognitive 2 Is cognitive neuroscience an oxymoron? 3 Forthcoming: Current Controversies in Philosophy of Cognitive Science, Eds. S. Cullen, S.J. Leslie & A. Lerner theories. In this method of reasoning the For instance, there are detailed theories of the neuroscience evidence can play an important cellular mechanisms that enable neuronal firing; role in adjusting our assessment of the however, these are unlikely to be sufficiently probability of cognitive theories being correct, related to mental representation and computa- even without definitively endorsing or defeating tion to play an important role in cognitive any particular theory. research. Even if every cognitive representation Neuroscience could also be a useful source of is encoded by the firing of neurons, this does not evidence without being necessary for the mean that a theory of the mechanics of neural development of cognitive theories. This point is firing at the cellular level informs a theory of obvious enough. If I want to know what color cognitive representation. my socks are, looking at them would be a useful Consider a slightly different question, source of evidence, but it is certainly not a however: If a scholar wishes to understand the necessary source of evidence. I could buy a influence of Uncle Tom’s Cabin on American camera, hire a film crew to shoot a movie of my attitudes towards slavery, would it be useful for socks, show it to my wife and ask her what color her to read the book—i.e., to interact directly she sees. But even if it isn’t necessary for me to with it as a physical object? Presumably it look at my own socks to determine their color, it would, simply because this would provide could certainly be useful. relevant information about its contents, which Finally, it might be the case that neurosci- would in turn inform a theory of its influence. ence is a possible source of evidence without Reading the book might not be necessary. The being a practical one. For instance, the film- scholar could rely on indirect sources of crew approach is a possible method of attaining evidence about its content, and many of the evidence about the color of my socks, but it isn’t inferences she would wish to draw about its a very practical one. Although practicality is influence wouldn’t depend on knowing its important, I won’t address it much further. precise contents anyway.
Recommended publications
  • M.S. and Ph.D. Sequences in Neuroscience and Physiology
    Neuroscience and Physiology are distinct but overlapping disciplines. • M.S. and Ph.D. students take three core Whereas Neuroscience investigates courses in neuroscience, physiology and neural substrates of behavior, Physiology biostatistics, and elective courses in more studies multiple functions. However, specific areas of these fields, as well as in M.S. and Ph.D. both seek to understand at an integrated related fields, such as cellular and level across molecules, cells, tissues, molecular biology, behavior, chemistry Sequences in whole organism, and environment. and psychology The workings of our brain and body • The curriculum provides a canonical Neuroscience and define us. When problems occur, results conceptual foundation for students can be devastating. According to the pursuing master’s and doctoral research in Physiology National Institutes of Health, neurological neuroscience and physiology and heart disease are two of the largest world health concerns and more than 50 • Our sequences provide a “cohort” million people in this country endure experience for new students, by offering a School of Biological some problem with the nervous system. cohesive curriculum for those students interested in pursuing graduate study in Sciences Our graduate sequences in Neuroscience neuroscience and physiology. and Physiology provide an exciting and Illinois State University challenging academic environment by combining research excellence with a strong commitment to education. We offer a comprehensive curriculum to graduate students interested in Neuroscience and Physiology. Both M.S. For more information, contact Dr. Paul A. and Ph.D. programs are also tightly Garris ([email protected]) or visit integrated into laboratory research. bio.illinoisstate.edu/graduate and goo.gl/9YTs4X Byron Heidenreich, Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • The Creation of Neuroscience
    The Creation of Neuroscience The Society for Neuroscience and the Quest for Disciplinary Unity 1969-1995 Introduction rom the molecular biology of a single neuron to the breathtakingly complex circuitry of the entire human nervous system, our understanding of the brain and how it works has undergone radical F changes over the past century. These advances have brought us tantalizingly closer to genu- inely mechanistic and scientifically rigorous explanations of how the brain’s roughly 100 billion neurons, interacting through trillions of synaptic connections, function both as single units and as larger ensem- bles. The professional field of neuroscience, in keeping pace with these important scientific develop- ments, has dramatically reshaped the organization of biological sciences across the globe over the last 50 years. Much like physics during its dominant era in the 1950s and 1960s, neuroscience has become the leading scientific discipline with regard to funding, numbers of scientists, and numbers of trainees. Furthermore, neuroscience as fact, explanation, and myth has just as dramatically redrawn our cultural landscape and redefined how Western popular culture understands who we are as individuals. In the 1950s, especially in the United States, Freud and his successors stood at the center of all cultural expla- nations for psychological suffering. In the new millennium, we perceive such suffering as erupting no longer from a repressed unconscious but, instead, from a pathophysiology rooted in and caused by brain abnormalities and dysfunctions. Indeed, the normal as well as the pathological have become thoroughly neurobiological in the last several decades. In the process, entirely new vistas have opened up in fields ranging from neuroeconomics and neurophilosophy to consumer products, as exemplified by an entire line of soft drinks advertised as offering “neuro” benefits.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Neuroscience 1
    Cognitive Neuroscience 1 Capstone Cognitive Neuroscience Concentrators will additionally take either a seminar course or an independent research course to serve as their capstone experience. Cognitive neuroscience is the study of higher cognitive functions in humans and their underlying neural bases. It is an integrative area of Additional requirements for Sc.B. study drawing primarily from cognitive science, psychology, neuroscience, In line with university expectations, the Sc.B. requirements include a and linguistics. There are two broad directions that can be taken in greater number of courses and especially science courses. The definition this concentration - one is behavioral/experimental and the other is of “science” is flexible. A good number of these courses will be outside of computational/modeling. In both, the goal is to understand the nature of CLPS, but several CLPS courses might fit into a coherent package as well. cognition from a neural perspective. The standard concentration for the In addition, the Sc.B. degree also requires a lab course to provide these Sc.B. degree requires courses on the foundations, systems level, and students with in-depth exposure to research methods in a particular area integrative aspects of cognitive neuroscience as well as laboratory and of the science of the mind. elective courses that fit within a particular theme or category such as general cognition, perception, language development or computational/ Honors Requirement modeling. Concentrators must also complete a senior seminar course or An acceptable upper level Research Methods, for example CLPS 1900 or an independent research course. Students may also participate in the an acceptable Laboratory course (see below) will serve as a requirement work of the Brown Institute for Brain Science, an interdisciplinary program for admission to the Honors program in Cognitive Neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience
    NEUROSCIENCE SCIENCE OF THE BRAIN AN INTRODUCTION FOR YOUNG STUDENTS British Neuroscience Association European Dana Alliance for the Brain Neuroscience: the Science of the Brain 1 The Nervous System P2 2 Neurons and the Action Potential P4 3 Chemical Messengers P7 4 Drugs and the Brain P9 5 Touch and Pain P11 6 Vision P14 Inside our heads, weighing about 1.5 kg, is an astonishing living organ consisting of 7 Movement P19 billions of tiny cells. It enables us to sense the world around us, to think and to talk. The human brain is the most complex organ of the body, and arguably the most 8 The Developing P22 complex thing on earth. This booklet is an introduction for young students. Nervous System In this booklet, we describe what we know about how the brain works and how much 9 Dyslexia P25 there still is to learn. Its study involves scientists and medical doctors from many disciplines, ranging from molecular biology through to experimental psychology, as well as the disciplines of anatomy, physiology and pharmacology. Their shared 10 Plasticity P27 interest has led to a new discipline called neuroscience - the science of the brain. 11 Learning and Memory P30 The brain described in our booklet can do a lot but not everything. It has nerve cells - its building blocks - and these are connected together in networks. These 12 Stress P35 networks are in a constant state of electrical and chemical activity. The brain we describe can see and feel. It can sense pain and its chemical tricks help control the uncomfortable effects of pain.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuromechanics: from Neurons to Brain
    CHAPTER TWO Neuromechanics: From Neurons to Brain Alain Goriely*, Silvia Budday†, Ellen Kuhl{,1 *Mathematical Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom †Department of Mechanical Engineering, University of Erlangen-Nuremberg, Erlangen, Germany { Departments of Mechanical Engineering and Bioengineering, Stanford University, Stanford, United States 1Corresponding author: e-mail address: [email protected] Contents 1. Motivation 80 2. Neuroelasticity 82 2.1 Elasticity of Single Neurons 82 2.2 Elasticity of Gray and White Matter Tissue 90 2.3 Elasticity of the Brain 93 3. Neurodevelopment 96 3.1 Growth of Single Neurons 96 3.2 Growth of Gray and White Matter Tissue 103 3.3 Growth of the Brain 106 4. Neurodamage 116 4.1 Neurodamage of Single Neurons 116 4.2 Neurodamage of Gray and White Matter Tissue 119 4.3 Neurodamage of the Brain 126 5. Open Questions and Challenges 128 Acknowledgments 131 Glossary 132 References 133 Abstract Arguably, the brain is the most complex organ in the human body, and, at the same time, the least well understood. Today, more than ever before, the human brain has become a subject of narcissistic study and fascination. The fields of neuroscience, neu- rology, neurosurgery, and neuroradiology have seen tremendous progress over the past two decades; yet, the field of neuromechanics remains underappreciated and poorly understood. Here, we show that mechanical stretch, strain, stress, and force play a critical role in modulating the structure and function of the brain. We discuss the role of neuromechanics across the scales, from individual neurons via neuronal tissue to the whole brain. We review current research highlights and discuss challenges and potential future directions.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Neuroscience Sequence
    Neuroscience Major: Sequence in Cognitive Neuroscience If you are a student who is interested in the human brain, and how it links to the human mind and complex human behaviors-- from before birth through adulthood, then this is the Neuroscience track for you! Note: This track is approved as a multidisciplinary major for ISS scholars at CMC. I. Overview The CogNeuro sequence in the Neuroscience major equips students with the knowledge of how to relate information processing in the human brain to human mental processes and behavior. Mental processes include perception, attention, voluntary movement, memory, conceptual biases, language, imagery, emotions, problem solving, decision-making, and social judgment. There are 2 main tracks within the CogNeuro track: 1) one for students who plan to attend graduate school for cognitive, social, or cultural neuroscience, or who want to obtain a job in a research lab at graduation; and 2) one for all other students (e.g. those who plan to attend medical or nursing or veterinary or dentistry school or graduate school for clinical psychology or law school or business school As for all of the sequences you should be choosing your 4 courses with the advice of the faculty who will be your Neuroscience major advisor, who will most likely be your senior thesis first reader and primary mentor. Below we give some requirements and general guidelines. II. Cognitive Neuroscience Faculty Mentors Stacey Doan (CMC) Alison Harris (CMC) Cathy Reed (CMC, KSD) Timothy Justus (PZ) David Moore (PZ) Michael Spezio (SC, KSD) Stacey Wood (SC) III. Tier 2 Requirements for the CogNeuro Emphasis in the KSD Neuroscience Major * Psyc 109 CM (or equivalent), Basic Psychological Statistics * Psyc 110 & 111L CM (or equivalent), Research Methods Lecture & Laboratory * Psyc 91 PZ Psychological Statistics * Psyc 92/ 92P PZ Introduction to Research Methods * Psyc 103 SC Psychological Statistics * Psyc 104 SC & 104L SC Research Design in Psychology & Laboratory NOTE: students should take the versions of these courses through their home college.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology Department Biopsychology Specialization
    PSYCHOLOGY DEPARTMENT BIOPSYCHOLOGY SPECIALIZATION OVERVIEW In the Biopsychology Specialization, students will explore how biological mechanisms relate to a wide range of topics: sensation, cognition, sleep, motivation, emotion, addiction, and clinical disorders. This specialization will expose students to the interface between biology and psychology (e.g., neuroscience, health psychology, psychopharmacology, psychoneuroimmunology, and genetics) and will prepare students for careers in these fields as well as in clinical psychology, medicine, or pharmaceuticals. Students will be offered hands-on research opportunities (with humans and rats), and specialized courses may include cognitive neuroscience, health psychology, neuroanatomy, addiction, psychopharmacology, human neuropsychology, neurological disorders, animal behavior, behavioral pharmacology of drug abuse, and schizophrenia. Students in this specializations will develop credentials that facilitate challenging careers and graduate/professional studies. Students completing the biopsychology specialization will receive preparation for careers in fields including clinical psychology, medicine, neuropsychological testing, and pharmaceuticals. This specialization also prepares students for graduate studies in neuroscience, health psychology, psychopharmacology, genetics, clinical psychology and psychoneuroimmunology. Objectives: • To expose students to a variety of different areas within biopsychology (for example: neuroscience, health psychology, psychopharmacology, psychophysiology,
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience Café
    Neuroscience Café Thursday March 18, 2021 6:00 - 7:00 PM “ Sleep and Circadian Rhythms During a Pandemic” The overall goal of my research program is to investigate environmental modulation of circadian clock function in mammalian systems and the contribution of clock disruption to pathological disease. We are interested in how nutrition (high caloric diets, meal timing) and disease (obesity, neurodegeneration) influence clock-driven changes in physiology and behavior in brain regions. A second interest of the laboratory is translation from animal models to humans determining the impact of circadian misalignment on metabolic function under a shift work environment. I investigated chronobiological systems while training in the laboratories of Drs. Elliott Albers and Doug McMahon. I have conducted research in numerous species, including hamsters, mice, and humans. I received my PhD training at Georgia State University in Behavioral Neuroscience. My postdoctoral training began at Vanderbilt University in the McMahon laboratory, where I started using transgenic mouse models and learned electrophysiology and organotypic culture imaging in 2004. In this vibrant chronobiology community, I received mentoring and training from Drs. Carl Karen L Gamble, PhD Professor Johnson, Terry Page, Shin Yamazaki, and Randy Blakely. This training prepared me for my faculty position which began in 2009 at University of Alabama at Birmingham. At UAB, I enjoy the Psychiatry -Behavioral Neuroscience outstanding neuroscience community, inter-disciplinary collaborations in research and teaching, Vice Chair of Basic Research as well as opportunities to educate the next generation of scientists. I am currently serving as the Director of the Neuroscience theme in the Graduate Biomedical Sciences Program.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience of Self and Self-Regulation
    PS62CH14-Heatherton ARI 22 November 2010 9:19 Neuroscience of Self and Self-Regulation Todd F. Heatherton Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire 03766; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011. 62:363–90 Key Words The Annual Review of Psychology is online at self-awareness, theory of mind, need to belong, social neuroscience, psych.annualreviews.org neuroimaging, addiction This article’s doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131616 Abstract by Dartmouth College on 12/08/10. For personal use only. Copyright c 2011 by Annual Reviews. As a social species, humans have a fundamental need to belong that en- All rights reserved courages behaviors consistent with being a good group member. Being 0066-4308/11/0110-0363$20.00 a good group member requires the capacity for self-regulation, which Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2011.62:363-390. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org allows people to alter or inhibit behaviors that would place them at risk for group exclusion. Self-regulation requires four psychological com- ponents. First, people need to be aware of their behavior so as to gauge it against societal norms. Second, people need to understand how others are reacting to their behavior so as to predict how others will respond to them. This necessitates a third mechanism, which detects threat, es- pecially in complex social situations. Finally, there needs to be a mech- anism for resolving discrepancies between self-knowledge and social expectations or norms, thereby motivating behavior to resolve any con- flict that exists. This article reviews recent social neuroscience research on the psychological components that support the human capacity for self-regulation.
    [Show full text]
  • Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience
    Intro to Cognitive Neuroscience Cognition and the study thereof: An overview 1 Some definitions • Cognition - The acquisition, storage, transformation, and use of knowledge. • Neuroscience - Study of the structure and workings of the nervous system. • Cognitive neuroscience - Study of how cognitive processes can be Image courtesy of Euskalanato explained by the structure and function of the brain. 2 Cognitive Psychology • An approach to studying and explaining behavior that emphasizes mental processes and knowledge. • Often described as studying the “software” of the brain The Thinker, Auguste Rodin, 1889. Image courtesy of mharrsch. 3 Some history • Late 19th century, many psychologists worked by introspection Wilhelm Wundt, 1832 - 1920. “Father of psychology.” Image courtesy of Wikimeda Commons. • Early 20th century, move towards behaviorism - study of objective, observable phenomena. • Knowledge based in B. F. Skinner, 1904 - empirical data, rigorous 1990. Image courtesy of standards for definitions lauradahl. and experiment designs. 4 Some history • Growth of computers contributed to success of information- processing approach to cognition. • Respectable context for discussing mechanisms that produce behavior. (Like software!) Glen Beck and Betty Snyder program ENIAC, circa 1947 - 1955. Image courtesy of the U.S. Army. 5 And today? • Cognitive psychologists study • perception • learning • language • creativity • imagery • attention • decision-making • reasoning • meta-cognition. • Almost all psychologists say that mental representations are important in affecting behavior. 6 Three themes of cognition 1.Cognitive processes are active, not passive. 2.Cognitive processes are interconnected. 3.Most cognitive capabilities use both bottom-up and top- down processing. 7 Bottom-up vs. top-down • Bottom-up processing is stimulus-driven. • Top-down processing is expectation-driven.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Neuroscience: a Review of Core Systems
    C HAPTER 2 2 SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE: A REVIEW OF CORE SYSTEMS Bruce P. Doré, Noam Zerubavel, and Kevin N. Ochsner Descartes famously argued that the mind is both SOCIAL COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE everlasting and indivisible (Descartes, 1988). If he APPROACH was right about the first part, he is probably pretty In the past decade, the field of social cognitive neuro- impressed with the advance of human knowledge science (SCN) has attempted to fill this gap, integrat- on the second. Although Descartes’ position on ing the theories and methods of two parent the indivisibility of the mind has been echoed at disciplines: social psychology and cognitive neurosci- times in the history of psychology and neurosci- ence. Stressing the interdependence of brain, mind, ence (Flourens & Meigs, 1846; Lashley, 1929; and social context, SCN seeks to explain psychological Uttal, 2003), the modern field has made steady phenomena at three levels of analysis: the neural level progress in demonstrating that subjective mental of brain systems, the cognitive level of information life can be understood as the product of distinct processing mechanisms, and the social level of the functional systems. Today, largely because of the experiences and actions of social agents (Ochsner & success of cognitive neuroscience models, Lieberman, 2001). In contrast to scientific approaches researchers understand that people’s intellectual that grant near exclusive focus to a single level of anal- faculties emerge from the operation of core ysis (e.g., behaviorism, artificial
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Planner: COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE CONCENTRATION - Fall 2019/Spring 2020 Updated 5/2019
    Name _______________________________________________________ UIN ________________________ Date ___________________ Curriculum Planner: COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE CONCENTRATION - Fall 2019/Spring 2020 Updated 5/2019 Introductory/Foundation Core Courses Concentration Courses UIUC/LAS General Education Psychology [choose one] 200-Level Core [choose one] Composition I PSYC 100 – Intro to Psych (FA19/SP20) PSYC 103 – Intro Experimental Psych PSYC 204 – Intro to Brain & Cognition (FA19) (not offered FA19/SP20 ) PSYC 220 – Images of Mind (SP20) Advanced Composition Statistics [choose one] Research Methods [choose one] Quantitative Reasoning (2 courses) PSYC 235 – Intro to Statistics or equivalent PSYC 445 – Cognitive Neuroscience Lab (SP20) Concentration Electives [choose four] 1. PSYC statistics or equivalent (3 hrs) Equivalent Courses – STAT 100, 200, 212, 400; ECON BCOG 100 – Intro Brain & Cog Sci (FA19) 202, 203; EPSY 280, 480; SOC 280: ACE 261; CHLH 2. BCOG 301 – Intelligence & Brain (SP20) 244; PS 230; UP 116; SOCW 225 (formerly PSYC 396 – Intelligence and the Brain) Western/Comparative Culture(s) (1 course) These equivalent courses meet the statistics PSYC 204 or 220 if not used as “200-level Core” requirement but do not count toward PSYC hours PSYC 302 – Applied Neuroscience (SP20) Non-Western Culture(s) (1 course) PSYC 403 – Memory and Amnesia (FA19) 200-Level Foundation Courses PSYC 404 – Cognitive Neuroscience (SP20) PSYC 421 – Prin. of Psychophysiology US Minority Cultures (1 course) (not offered FA19/SP20 ) Biological/Cognitive [choose one] PSYC 427 – Language and the Brain (SP20) PSYC 433 – Evolutionary Neuroscience (FA19) Humanities and the Arts (6 hours) PSYC 210 – Behavioral Neuroscience (FA19/SP20) PSYC 450 – Cognitive Psychophysiology (FA19) PSYC 224 – Cognitive Psych (FA19) PSYC 453 - Cognitive Neuroscience of Vision (FA19) 1.
    [Show full text]