Illusory Contour Perception and Amodal Boundary Completion: Evidence of a Dissociation Following Callosotomy

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Illusory Contour Perception and Amodal Boundary Completion: Evidence of a Dissociation Following Callosotomy Illusory Contour Perception and Amodal Boundary Completion: Evidence of a Dissociation Following Callosotomy Paul M. Corballis and Robert Fendrich Dartmouth College Robert M. Shapley New York University Michael S. Gazzaniga Dartmouth College Abstract A fundamental problem in form perception is how the tion of illusory contours and amodal completion. Although visual system can link together spatially separated contour both cerebral hemispheres appear to be equally capable of fragments to form the percept of a unitary shape. Illusory perceiving illusory contours, amodal completion is more read- contours and amodal completion are two phenomena that ily utilized by the right hemisphere. These results suggest that demonstrate this linking process. In the present study we illusory contours may be attributable to low-level visual proc- investigate these phenomena in the divided hemispheres of esses common to both hemispheres, whereas amodal comple- two callosotomy (“split-brain”) patients. The data suggest that tion reects a higher-level, lateralized process. dissociable neural mechanisms are responsible for the genera- INTRODUCTION Figure 1a, no illusory contours are seen. In such cases, the grouping process responsible for the perception of To perceive objects in the environment as unied the rectangle has been termed “amodal completion” wholes, the visual system must often extrapolate from (Michotte, Thines, & Crabbe, 1964).1 incomplete contour or boundary information. Under cer- The relationship between amodal completion and the tain conditions extrapolated contours are perceived in formation of illusory contours has been controversial. It areas of homogeneous retinal stimulation. Because such has been suggested that a common mechanism may be contours are not present in the physical stimulus, they responsible for both of these phenomena (e.g., Kellman are referred to as “illusory” or “subjective.” Illusory con- & Loukides, 1987; Kellman & Shipley, 1991; Ringach & tours are often perceived when the contours or edges Shapley, 1996) or that amodal completion may be a of elements in the visual array are consistent with the prerequisite for the generation of illusory contours (Min- presence of a superimposed surface or object. An exam- guzzi, 1987). However, the fact that amodal completion ple is the “Kanisza rectangle” shown in Figure 1a, which and the perception of illusory contours can dissociate appears to most observers as a white rectangle superim- under certain conditions has caused some investigators posed on four black circles. There is an apparent bright- to argue that these linking processes are mediated by ness discontinuity between the inside and outside of the different neural mechanisms (e.g., Sambin, 1987). rectangle so that the inside appears brighter than the Our initial aim in this study was to investigate the outside. This apparent brightness transition forms the perception of illusory contours by the divided hemi- illusory contour (see Kanisza, 1976; 1979). spheres of two right-handed callosotomy patients, J.W. However, visual grouping processes can act to gener- and V.P. Evidence from functional neuroimaging studies, ate perceived shapes from incomplete boundary infor- investigations of clinical populations with unilateral mation without producing such illusory contours. For brain damage, and psychophysical investigations with example, in Figure 1b most observers perceive a white normal observers has suggested that the mechanism or rectangle mostly occluded by a surface with four holes mechanisms responsible for illusory-contour perception in it so that only the corners are visible. In contrast with may be preferentially lateralized to the right cerebral © 1999 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:4, pp. 459–466 Figure 1. (a) A Kanisza rec- tangle. This conguration is typically perceived as a white rectangle superimposed on four black circles. (b) An amo- dally completed rectangle. This conguration is typically perceived as a white rectangle viewed through four aper- tures in an occluding surface. Note that although the shape of the rectangle is still readily visible, no illusory contours are perceived. hemisphere (Atchley & Atchley, 1998; Hirsch et al., 1995; the strength of the binding processes that produce the Wasserstein, Zapulla, Rosen, Gerstman, & Rock, 1987). If perceived shape. In neurological normals, similar im- this is the case, the left hemisphere of a split-brain provements in performance are obtained in conditions patient ought to be signicantly impaired at the percep- in which illusory contours are perceived and when the tion of illusory contours relative to the right hemisphere. pacmen are modied (see Figure 2d) so that perceiving We therefore designed an experiment to directly inves- the illusory rectangle depends upon amodal completion tigate the perception of illusory contours by each of the (Ringach & Shapley, 1996). isolated hemispheres of two such patients. In our lateralized version of this task, illusory-contour A second goal of our investigation was to compare (IC), amodal-completion (AC), or local-orientation (LO) amodal boundary completion to illusory contour per- control stimuli were briey ashed to the left or right of ception in each hemisphere. We reasoned that if amodal a xation point, so that their processing would be completion and illusory-contour perception depend on conned to the hemisphere contralateral to the side of the same mechanisms, any relative hemispheric advan- xation to which they were presented. Two callosotomy tage for one of these effects should also be found for the patients, J.W. and V.P., were asked to judge whether the other. If, however, amodal completion depends on differ- Kanisza rectangle appeared thin or fat (IC and AC tasks) ent mechanisms than illusory-contour perception, these or whether the pacmen tilted up or down (LO control processes might exhibit different patterns of lateraliza- task). tion. We investigated illusory-contour perception and amo- RESULTS AND DISCUSSION dal boundary completion using a lateralized version of the shape-discrimination task introduced by Ringach and Separate analyses were conducted for the comparison Shapley (1996). In this task the observer is asked to between the IC and LO tasks and between the IC and judge whether a deformed illusory rectangle appears to AC tasks. Psychometric functions showing percentage have convex or concave vertical sides (i.e., appears “fat” correct as a function of angular displacement of the or “thin;” see Figure 2a and b). The deformed illusory pacman inducers for both patients are displayed in Fig- rectangles are created by rotating the “pacman” inducers ures 3 (IC versus LO tasks) and 4 (IC versus AC tasks). by a small amount so that the sides of the rectangle appear to bend, creating the appearance of a thin or fat Patient J.W. rectangle. Subjects’ ability to discriminate thin from fat illusory rectangles is compared to performance in a Illusory Contours vs. Local Orientation “local orientation” control task in which the inducers all Inspection of Figure 3 indicates that J.W.’s discrimination face in the same direction and the subjects judge performance was better for the IC task than the LO task whether they are tilted “up” or “down” (see Figure 2c). in both hemields. This suggests that both hemispheres Ringach and Shapley have shown that neurologically are capable of perceiving illusory contours. It is also normal observers can detect smaller pacman rotations apparent from Figure 3 that J.W.’s performance at both in the shape-discrimination task than in the control task. tasks was better in the left visual eld (LVF) than the The magnitude of this difference provides an index of right visual eld (RVF). 460 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 11, Number 4 Figure 2. Examples of the stimuli used in these experi- ments. (a) A “fat” Kanisza rec- tangle used in the IC task. The inducers in this gure are each rotated by 4° so that the vertical sides of the rectangle appear to be convex. (b) A “thin” Kanisza rectangle used in the IC task. The inducers in this gure are each rotated by 4° so that the vertical sides of the rectangle appear to be concave. (c) An example of a stimulus from the LO task. The pacmen in this gure are each rotated downward by 4°. (d) A “fat” stimulus from the AC task. The inducers in this gure are each rotated by 4° so that the vertical sides of the rectangle appear to be concave. Multidimensional-c 2 analyses (Winer, Brown, & and response reects J.W.’s superior performance in the Michels, 1991) were conducted to determine the effects IC task over the LO task (c 2(1) = 28.94, p < 0.01; of task (IC versus LO), hemield (LVF versus RVF), angle proportion correct = 0.778 for the IC task, 0.628 for the (1, 2, 3, or 4°) and stimulus condition (thin/up versus LO task). Finally, an interaction between hemield, con- fat/down) on response choice (thin/up versus dition, and response reects his overall superior perfor- fat/down). In this analysis, discrimination accuracy is mance when stimuli were presented in the LVF (c 2(1) = indexed by the interaction between stimulus condition 17.19, p < 0.01; proportion correct = 0.761 for the LVF, and response. The effects of other factors, or combina- 0.645 for the RVF). The four-way interaction between tions of factors, on accuracy are indexed by higher-order task, hemield, condition, and response was not sig- interactions involving both condition and response. The nicant (c 2(1) = 1.01, n.s.). This indicates that the differ- interaction between stimulus condition and response ence in discrimination accuracy between the IC and LO choice was signicant (c 2(1) = 212.25, p < 0.01), indicat- tasks was not signicantly different for LVF and RVF ing that that J.W.’s overall performance was signicantly stimuli. No interactions involving angle reached sig- better than chance (overall proportion correct was nicance. 0.703). A signicant interaction between task, condition, Overall, the results of this analysis suggest that al- Corballis et al.
Recommended publications
  • Neon Colour Spreading in Three-Dimensional Illusory Objects in Humans
    Neuroscience Letters 281 (2000) 119±122 www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Neon colour spreading in three-dimensional illusory objects in humans Marja Liinasuoa,*, Ilpo Kojob, Jukka HaÈ kkinenc, Jyrki Rovamod aInstitute of Biomedicine, Department of Physiology, P.O. Box 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland bThe Finnish Institute of Occupational Health, Brain Work Laboratory, Topeliuksenkatu 41 a A, 00250 Helsinki, Finland cDepartment of Psychology, General Psychology Division, P.O. Box 13, 00014 University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland dDepartment of Optometry and Vision Sciences, University of Wales, College of Cardiff, P.O. Box 905, Cardiff CF1 3XF, UK Received 11 October 1999; received in revised form 7 January 2000; accepted 10 January 2000 Abstract We studied whether neon spreading can be induced within three-dimensional illusory triangles. Kanizsa triangles were induced by black pacman disks consisting of red sectors with curved sides. Viewing our stimuli monocularly produced two-dimensional illusory contours and surfaces as well as neon spreading in each ®gure. Triangles appeared concave or convex under stereoscopical viewing. Neon colour spreading was induced within illusory ®gures bending in three-dimensional space, suggesting that neural contour completion and surface ®lling-in interact across depth. Surpris- ingly, neon spreading was induced above the intervening surface even when the inducers were below the surface. Neon colour and illusory con®guration were preserved behind the intervening surface only when it appeared transparent. q 2000 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Vision; Perception; Illusion; Three-dimensionality; Colour; Neon spreading Illusory ®gures with one to three dimensions are created zontal and vertical crossing lines so that some line elements by the neural processes of visual area V2 [15] between some are replaced by different colour or luminance, the surround- physically existing objects, known as inducers.
    [Show full text]
  • Introducing the Scintillating Starburst: Illusory Ray Patterns from Spatial Coincidence Detection
    Introducing the scintillating starburst: Illusory ray patterns from spatial coincidence detection 1 2 Michael W Karlovich ​ & Pascal Wallisch ​ ​ 1 Recursia Labs. 2 Department of Psychology, New York University Abstract Here, we introduce a novel class of visual illusion, the “Scintillating Starburst”, which features illusory scintillating rays or beams. We show empirically what combination of factors modulate how an observer experiences this kind of stimulus. We explain how the illusion arises from the interplay of known visual processes, specifically magnocellular and parvocellular dynamics and relate the effects of these ray patterns to other known visual illusions such as illusory contours. Introduction Illusions have played a key role in understanding the principles of perceptual processing (Purkinje, 1825; van Buren & Scholl, 2018), although this notion is not entirely without detractors (Braddick, 2018; Rogers, 2019). One important reason why illusions can be helpful is that they allow us to distinguish the mere sensation of physical object properties from the perceptual experience. This is perhaps clearest in the case of illusory contours. If contours are defined by a sharp change in luminance, it is hard to tell whether the observer “directly” perceives objects in the environment “as they are” or if the percept is constructed in the mind of the observer, because the output of a photometer and perceptual judgments agree (Gibson, 1978; Fodor & Pylyshyn, 1981). Conversely, illusory contours are not defined by changes in luminance, and would be invisible to a photometer, whereas human observers readily perceive them. One example of this is Kanisza’s triangle, which most observers perceive as a bright triangle occluding three black circles - as opposed to the three black pac-man-like shapes are actually defined by luminance (Kanisza, 1955).
    [Show full text]
  • Illusory Contours: a Window Onto the Neurophysiology of Constructing Perception
    Review Illusory contours: a window onto the neurophysiology of constructing perception 1,2,3 4,5 Micah M. Murray and Christoph S. Herrmann 1 The Functional Electrical Neuroimaging Laboratory, Neuropsychology and Neurorehabilitation Service, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois and University of Lausanne, 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland 2 Electroencephalography Brain Mapping Core, Center for Biomedical Imaging (CIBM), 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland 3 Radiology Department, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois and University of Lausanne, 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland 4 Experimental Psychology Laboratory, ‘Hearing4all’ Center of Excellence, European Medical School, Carl von Ossietzky University, 26111 Oldenburg, Germany 5 Neurosensory Science Research Center, Carl von Ossietzky University, 26111 Oldenburg, Germany Seeing seems effortless, despite the need to segregate Researchers are generally divided into three camps and integrate visual information that varies in quality, regarding the brain mechanisms responsible for IC per- quantity, and location. The extent to which seeing pas- ception based on the presumed role of feed-forward versus sively recapitulates the external world is challenged by feedback activity, as well as the locus of neural sensitivity phenomena such as illusory contours, an example of visual completion whereby borders are perceived de- Glossary spite their physical absence in the image. Instead, visual Binding:: (neurophysiologic) processes whereby different information is none- completion and seeing are increasingly conceived as theless coded to refer to the same object or event. Here, different information active processes, dependent on information exchange refers to spatially separated stimulus elements, such as inducers of ICs. across neural populations. How this is instantiated in the Bottom-up versus top-down processes:: bottom-up processes rely exclusively on feed-forward architectures, whereas top-down processes incorporate brain remains controversial.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 3. Visual Attention and Visual Awareness
    Section I. Anatomy and physiology of the human visual system. CHAPTER 3. VISUAL ATTENTION AND VISUAL AWARENESS. Rufin VanRullen1 and Christof Koch2. 1. CNRS Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition 133 Route de Narbonne 31062 Toulouse Cedex (France) 2. California Institute of Technology Division of Biology and Division of Engineering and Applied Sciences MC 139-74 Pasadena CA 91125 (USA) 1 Intuitively, vision appears an overall easy process, effortless, almost automatic, and so efficient that a simple glance at a complex scene is sufficient to produce immediate awareness of its entire structure and elements. Unfortunately, much of this is a grand illusion (O’Regan & Noe, 2001). Proper manipulations will reveal that many often essential aspects of the visual scene can go purely unnoticed. For example, human subjects will often fail to notice an unexpected but quite large stimulus flashed right at the center of gaze during a psychophysical experiment (a phenomenon known as “inattentional blindness”, Mack and Rock, 1998). In more natural environments, observers can fail to notice the appearance or disappearance of a large object (Fig 3.1; Rensink et al, 1997; O’Regan et al, 1999), the change in identity of the person they are conversing with (Simons and Levin, 1998), or the passage of a gorilla in the middle of a ball game (Simons and Chabris, 1999). As a group, these visual failures are referred to as “change blindness” (Rensink, 2002). Such limitations are rarely directly experienced in real life –except as one of the main instrument of magician tricks; yet they shape much of our visual perceptions.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Luminance Contrast on Color Spreading and Illusory Contour In
    Perception & Psychophysics 1989, 45 (4), 427-430 Effects ofluminance contrast on color spreading and illusory contour in the neon color spreading effect TAKEO WATANABE University of Tokyo, Tokyo, Japan and TAKAOSATO ATR Auditory and Visual Perception Research Laboratories, Kyoto, Japan The present study examined whether color spreading and illusory contours in the neon color spreading effect ofEhrenstein figures are governed by different mechanisms. In the experiment, Ehrenstein figures with colored crosses inserted in the central gaps were used. There were three luminance conditions: the luminance ofthe Ehrenstein figures was lower than, the same as, or higher than the luminance ofthe background. In each condition, 16 trials (2 sets of instructions x 8 repetitions) were conducted in a random order. Subjects were required to adjust the luminance ofthe colored crosses according to one ofthe two sets ofinstruction given before each trial. One was to adjust the upper and lower thresholds in the luminance of the colored crosses such that their color was seen to spread out of the crosses. The other was to adjust the thresholds such that circular illusory contours were visible. It was found that illusory contours disappeared and the color spreading remained when the crosses and the Ehrenstein figures were in or nearly in isoluminance or when the Ehrenstein figures and the background were in isoluminance. These results suggest that color spreading and illusory contours are governed by different mechanisms. Redies and Spillmann (1981) showed that van Tuijl's vary the luminance of the inner segment, the outer seg­ (1975) neon color spreading effect occurred in the Ehren­ ments, and the background.
    [Show full text]
  • A Model of the Illusory Contour Formation Based on Dendritic Computation
    ARTICLE IN PRESS Neurocomputing 70 (2007) 1977–1982 www.elsevier.com/locate/neucom A model of the illusory contour formation based on dendritic computation Drazˇen DomijanÃ, Mia Sˇetic´, Domagoj Sˇvegar Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Rijeka, I. Klobucˇaric´a 1, 51000 Rijeka, Croatia Available online 9 November 2006 Abstract We proposed a new model of illusory contour formation based on the properties of dendritic computation. The basic elements of the network are a single-excitatory cell with two dendritic branches and an inhibitory cell. Both dendritic branches behave as an independent linear unit with a threshold. They sum all excitatory input from the nearby collinear cells, and the inhibition from one collateral of the corresponding inhibitory cell. Furthermore, the output of dendritic branches multiplicatively interacts before it is sent to the soma. The multiplication allows the excitatory cell to be active only if both of its branches receive enough excitation to reach the threshold. Computer simulations showed that the presented model of the illusory contour formation is able to perform perceptual grouping of nonadjacent collinear elements. It shows a linear response relationship with the input magnitude because dendritic inhibition counteracts recurrent excitation. The model can explain why illusory contours are stronger with irregular placement of inducing elements rather than regular placement and why top-down influences may prevent the illusory contour formation. r 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Keywords: Contour integration; Dendritic computation; Illusory contours; Recurrent excitation; Top-down processing 1. Introduction graded strength or clarity of illusory contours with the relative size of inducing elements [3].
    [Show full text]
  • The Generic-Viewpoint Assumption and Illusory Contours
    Perception, 2000, volume 29, pages 303 ^ 312 DOI:10.1068/p3016 The generic-viewpoint assumption and illusory contours Marc K Albertô Vision Sciences Laboratory, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02114, USA Donald D Hoffman Department of Cognitive Sciences University of California at Irvine, Irvine, CA 92697, USA Received 16 February 1999, in revised form 17 August 1999 Abstract. Visual images are ambiguous. Any image, or collection of images, is consistent with an infinite number of possible scenes in the world. Yet we are generally unaware of this ambiguity. During ordinary perception we are generally aware of only one, or perhaps a few of these possibilities. Human vision evidently exploits certain constraintsöassumptions about the world and images formed of itö in order to generate its perceptions. One constraint that has been widely studied by researchers in human and machine vision is the generic-viewpoint assumption. We show that this assumption can help to explain the widely discussed fact that outlines of blobs are ineffective inducers of illusory contours. We also present a number of novel effects and report an experiment suggesting that the generic-viewpoint assumption strongly influences illusory-contour perception. 1 Introduction Illusory contours ICs) are contours seen where there are no corresponding gradients in the stimulus. Although ICs were first reported around the turn of the century, interest in this phenomenon dramatically increased after Kanizsa 1955) presented his striking demonstrations. Since then ICs have received a great deal of attention both experi- mentally and theoretically for reviews see Parks 1984; Purghe¨ and Coren 1992; Lesher 1995). Figure 1 is a typical example.
    [Show full text]
  • The Watercolor Illusion and Neon Color Spreading: a Unified Analysis of New Cases and Neural Mechanisms
    B. Pinna and S. Grossberg Vol. 22, No. 10/October 2005/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1 The watercolor illusion and neon color spreading: a unified analysis of new cases and neural mechanisms Baingio Pinna Facoltà di Lingue e Letterature Straniere, Università di Sassari, via Roma 151, I-07100, Sassari, Italy Stephen Grossberg Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems and Center for Adaptive Systems, Boston University, 677 Beacon Street, Boston, Massachusetts 02215 Received January 18, 2005; accepted April 14, 2005 Coloration and figural properties of neon color spreading and the watercolor illusion are studied using phe- nomenal and psychophysical observations. Coloration properties of both effects can be reduced to a common limiting condition, a nearby color transition called the two-dot limiting case, which clarifies their perceptual similarities and dissimilarities. The results are explained by the FACADE neural model of biological vision. The model proposes how local properties of color transitions activate spatial competition among nearby per- ceptual boundaries, with boundaries of lower-contrast edges weakened by competition more than boundaries of higher-contrast edges. This asymmetry induces spreading of more color across these boundaries than con- versely. The model also predicts how depth and figure–ground effects are generated in these illusions. © 2005 Optical Society of America OCIS codes: 330.0330, 330.1720, 330.4060, 330.4270, 330.5020, 330.5510, 330.7310. 1. INTRODUCTION: A CURRENT VIEW OF One timely piece of evidence was the Thorell et al.1 study, A SEMINAL DISCOVERY OF DEVALOIS which supported the early prediction that these bound- aries and surfaces are processed by the interblob and blob Russell DeValois and his colleagues discovered many streams, respectively, from V1 to V4.
    [Show full text]
  • Sparse Models for Computer Vision
    Chapter 14 Sparse models for Computer Vision BibTex entry This chapter appeared as (Perrinet, 2015): @inbook{Perrinet15sparse, Author = {Perrinet, Laurent U.}, Chapter = {Sparse models for Computer Vision}, Citeulike-Article-Id = {13514904}, Editor = {Crist{\'{o}}bal, Gabriel and Keil, Matthias S. and Perrinet, Laurent U.}, Keywords = {bicv-sparse, sanz12jnp, vacher14}, month = nov, chapter = {14}, Priority = {0}, isbn = {9783527680863}, title = {Sparse Models for Computer Vision}, DOI = {10.1002/9783527680863.ch14}, url={http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9783527680863.ch14/summary}, publisher = {Wiley-VCH Verlag GmbH {\&} Co. KGaA}, booktitle = {Biologically inspired computer vision}, Year = {2015} } arXiv:1701.06859v1 [cs.CV] 24 Jan 2017 1 Perrinet (2015) Sparse models doi:10.1002/9783527680863.ch14 2 Contents 14 Sparse models for Computer Vision 1 14.1 Motivation . .3 14.1.1 Efficiency and sparseness in biological representations of natural images . .3 14.1.2 Sparseness induces neural organization . .4 14.1.3 Outline: Sparse models for Computer Vision . .5 14.2 What is sparseness? Application to image patches . .6 14.2.1 Definitions of sparseness . .6 14.2.2 Learning to be sparse: the SparseNet algorithm . .8 14.2.3 Results: efficiency of different learning strategies . .9 14.3 SparseLets: a multi scale, sparse, biologically inspired representation of natural images 10 14.3.1 Motivation: architecture of the primary visual cortex . 10 14.3.2 The SparseLets framework . 12 14.3.3 Efficiency of the SparseLets framework . 14 14.4 SparseEdges: introducing prior information . 17 14.4.1 Using the prior in first-order statistics of edges . 17 14.4.2 Using the prior statistics of edge co-occurences .
    [Show full text]
  • Kanizsa Illusory Contours Appearing in the Plasmodium Pattern of Physarum Polycephalum
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 28 February 2014 CELLULAR AND INFECTION MICROBIOLOGY doi: 10.3389/fcimb.2014.00010 Kanizsa illusory contours appearing in the plasmodium pattern of Physarum polycephalum Iori Tani 1*, Masaki Yamachiyo 1, Tomohiro Shirakawa 2 and Yukio-Pegio Gunji 1,3 1 Department of Earth and Planetary Science, Graduate School of Science, Kobe University, Kobe, Japan 2 Department of Computer Science, School of Electrical and Computer Engineering, National Defense Academy of Japan, Yokosuka, Japan 3 The Unconventional Computing Centre, University of the West England, Bristol, UK Edited by: The plasmodium of Physarum polycephalum is often used in the implementation of Kevin B. Clark, Veterans Affairs non-linear computation to solve optimization problems, and this organismal feature was Greater Los Angeles Healthcare not used in this analysis to compute perception and/or sensation in humans. In this paper, System, USA we focused on the Kanizsa illusion, which is a well-known visual illusion resulting from the Reviewed by: Jeff Jones, University of the West of differentiation-integration of the visual field, and compared the illusion with the adaptive England, UK network in the plasmodium of P.polycephalum. We demonstrated that the network pattern Werner Baumgarten, Universität mimicking the Kanizsa illusion can be produced by an asynchronous automata-fashioned Rostock, Germany model of the foraging slime mold and by the real plasmodia of P.polycephalum. Because *Correspondence: the protoplasm of the plasmodium is transported depending on both local and global Iori Tani, Department of Earth and Planetary Science, Graduate School computation, it may contain differentiation-integration processes. In this sense, we can of Science, Kobe University, Science extend the idea of perception and computation.
    [Show full text]
  • HOW DOES the CEREBRAL CORTEX WORK? Learning, Attention, and Grouping by the Laminar Circuits of Visual Cortex
    HOW DOES THE CEREBRAL CORTEX WORK? Learning, Attention, and Grouping by the Laminar Circuits of Visual Cortex Stephen Grossberg* Department of Cognitive and Neural Systems and Center for Adaptive Systems Boston University 677 Beacon Street Boston, MA 02215 April, 1998 Spatial Vision, in press To appear in the special issue on Long-Range Visual Mechanisms Technical Report CAS/CNS-97-023 Running Title: Cortical Circuits for Learning, Attention, and Grouping * Supported in part by the Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency, the National Science Foundation (NSF IRI-97-20333), and the Office of Naval Research (ONR N00014-95-1-0409 and ONR N00014-95-1-0657). S. Grossberg Acknowledgments: The author wishes to thank Robin Amos and Diana Meyers for their valuable assistance in the preparation of the manuscript. 1 S. Grossberg The organization of neocortex into layers is one of its most salient anatomical features. These layers include circuits that form functional columns in cortical maps. A major unsolved problem concerns how bottom-up, top-down, and horizontal interactions are organized within cortical layers to generate adaptive behaviors. This article models how these interactions help visual cortex to realize: (1) the binding process whereby cortex groups distributed data into coherent object representations; (2) the attentional process whereby cortex selectively processes important events; and (3) the developmental and learning processes whereby cortex shapes its circuits to match environmental constraints. New computational ideas about feedback systems suggest how neocortex develops and learns in a stable way, and why top-down attention requires converging bottom-up inputs to fully activate cortical cells, whereas perceptual groupings do not.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereoscopic Illusory Contours—Cortical Neuron Responses and Human Perception
    Stereoscopic Illusory Contours—Cortical Neuron Responses and Human Perception Barbara Heider1, Lothar Spillmann2, and Esther Peterhans3 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/14/7/1018/1757651/089892902320474472.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & In human perception, figure–ground segregation sug- signal such illusory contours and can be selective for certain gests that stereoscopic cues are grouped over wide areas of figure–ground directions that human observers perceive at the visual field. For example, two abutting rectangles of these contours. The results suggest that these neurons equal luminance and size are seen as a uniform surface group stereoscopic cues over distances up to 88. In addition, when presented at the same depth, but appear as two we compare these results with human perception and show surfaces separated by an illusory contour and a step in that the mean stimulus parameters required by these depth when presented with different retinal disparities. neurons also induce optimal percepts of illusory contours Here, we describe neurons in the monkey visual cortex that in human observers. & INTRODUCTION representations of such contours have been identified Illusory contours are perceived in visual scenes where in the human visual cortex by means of imaging objects occlude one another and produce overlapping techniques such as positron emission tomography images on the retina. If these objects have equal (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging luminance, their retinal images merge because their (fMRI). Representations have been localized early in overlapping borders lack luminance contrast. The visual visual processing, in area V2 (Larsson et al., 1999; system has developed methods to recover the segrega- ffytche & Zeki, 1996; Hirsch et al., 1995), and in higher tion of figure and ground in such situations—it gen- areas such as V3A, V4v, V7, and V8 (Mendola, Dale, erates illusory contours that complete the occluding Fischl, Liu, & Tootell, 1999).
    [Show full text]