NARCISSISM and ENVY 1 Lange*, J., Crusius*, J., & Hagemeyer, B

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

NARCISSISM and ENVY 1 Lange*, J., Crusius*, J., & Hagemeyer, B Running head: NARCISSISM AND ENVY 1 Lange*, J., Crusius*, J., & Hagemeyer, B. (2016). The Evil Queen's dilemma: Linking narcissistic admiration and rivalry to benign and malicious envy. European Journal of Personality, 30, 168-188. doi:10.1002/per.2047 accepted manuscript before copyediting The Evil Queen’s dilemma: Linking narcissistic admiration and rivalry to benign and malicious envy Jens Langea, Jan Crusiusa, & Birk Hagemeyerb a University of Cologne, b Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena Author Note Jens Lange and Jan Crusius contributed equally to this research. This work was supported by a grant from the German Research Foundation (CR 489/1-1). We would like to thank Felix Speckmann, Carola Bloch, Eileen Pauels, Jannik Süselbach, Lucia Gehlen, and Jana Harzem for collecting the data for Study 5. We are also grateful to the Social Cognition Center Cologne and the Department of Personality Psychology and Psychological Assessment in Jena for valuable feedback. Correspondence should be addressed to either Jens Lange, Social Cognition Center Cologne, University of Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Köln, Germany, E-Mail: [email protected], or Jan Crusius, Social Cognition Center Cologne, University of Cologne, Richard-Strauss-Str. 2, 50931 Köln, Germany, E-Mail: [email protected]. NARCISSISM AND ENVY 2 Abstract It is widely assumed that narcissists are envious. Nevertheless, evidence supporting this claim has remained elusive. In five studies (combined N = 1225), we disentangle how grandiose narcissism predicts divergent envious inclinations. Specific facets of narcissism and forms of envy shared the same underlying motivational orientations (Study 1) and distinctively related to each other (Studies 1 to 5) via differences in emotional appraisal (Study 4). Moreover, envy was linked to opposing social consequences of different narcissism facets (Study 5). Specifically, hope for success related to narcissistic admiration, predicting benign envy, which entails the motivation to improve performance, translating into the ascription of social potency by the self and others. In contrast, fear of failure related to narcissistic rivalry, predicting malicious envy, which entails hostility, translating into the ascription of a proneness for social conflict by others. These results converged with envy measured as a trait (Studies 1 and 5) or state in recall tasks (Studies 2 and 4) and as response to an upward standard in the situation (Study 3). The findings provide important insights into narcissists’ emotional complexities, integrate prior isolated and conflicting evidence, and open up new avenues for research on narcissism and envy. Keywords: grandiose narcissism; narcissistic admiration and rivalry; benign and malicious envy; social comparison; social perception NARCISSISM AND ENVY 3 The Evil Queen’s dilemma: Linking narcissistic admiration and rivalry to benign and malicious envy One day when the queen asked her mirror: Mirror, mirror, on the wall, Who in this land is fairest of all? It answered: You, my queen, are fair; it is true. But Snow-White is a thousand times fairer than you. The queen took fright and turned yellow and green with envy. (Grimm & Grimm, 1857/2002) Are narcissists envious? According to a widely held assumption, envy is a central element of narcissism. In fact, this notion has been endorsed so strongly that it even used to be one of the criteria to diagnose narcissistic personality disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and was incorporated in measures of grandiose narcissism (Raskin & Terry, 1988). However, the empirical link of envy and narcissism remains to be elusive. In fact, the few investigations of this question have concluded that narcissists might not suffer from the green-eyed monster. If anything, previously, only vulnerable and not grandiose narcissists have been found to be envious of others’ qualities, achievements, and possessions (Gold, 1996; Krizan & Johar, 2012). Indeed, should not narcissists’ grandiosity protect them from threatening upward comparisons (Neufeld & Johnson, in press)? Here, we show that narcissists are in fact highly prone to react with envy. Importantly, however, the relationship of narcissism and envy is more complex than previously assumed. To unravel it, it is necessary to take distinct motivational dynamics within both envy and narcissism into account. NARCISSISM AND ENVY 4 Even though narcissists1 share the conviction that they are superior to others, we contend that this belief does not inoculate them against upward social comparisons. In many situations, objective sources of information, such as exam results, promotion decisions, or truth-telling mirrors, may undeniably reveal one’s inferiority relative to others. If high status is narcissists’ central concern, any defect substantiated by comparisons with superior others should be a particularly strong elicitor of emotion and spur them to deal with their disadvantage. We posit that, despite the weakness such envious action may uncover, narcissists respond to situational inferiority with envy—the Evil Queen’s dilemma. The goal of the present research is to investigate whether narcissists are prone to feel envy and how this emotional process is connected to the social consequences of the narcissistic personality. We link the two facets of grandiose narcissism—narcissistic admiration and rivalry (Back et al., 2013)—to the two distinct forms of envy—benign and malicious envy (Lange & Crusius, 2015a; Van de Ven, Zeelenberg, & Pieters, 2009). Moreover, we investigate how these emotional links illuminate the paradox of narcissism, the enigmatic opposing effects of narcissism on perceived (un-)popularity. Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Narcissists hold highly inflated, unrealistically positive, grandiose views of themselves (Gebauer, Sedikides, Verplanken, & Maio, 2012; Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). This biased self-perception has a profound impact on narcissists’ social behavior. Much of the recent interest in narcissism research has been fed by the paradoxical nature of these outcomes. On the one hand, narcissists are socially attractive (Back, Schmukle, & Egloff, 2010; Vazire, Naumann, Rentfrow, & Gosling, 2008) and seen as confident, intelligent, and competent by their peers (Back et al., 2013; Paulhus, 1998). As a consequence, narcissists have high mating success (Dufner, Rauthmann, Czarna, & Denissen, 2013, Grosz, Dufner, Back, & Denissen, 2015), are more often chosen as leaders of social groups (Brunell et al., NARCISSISM AND ENVY 5 2008), and have a higher probability to become famous (Dufner et al., 2015; Young & Pinsky, 2006). On the other hand, narcissists also show hostile behaviors (Bushman & Baumeister, 1998; Morf & Rhodewalt, 1993) leading others to see them as aggressive, less trustworthy, or bragging towards competitors (Back et al., 2013; Paulhus, 1998) which undermines their social relationships (Back et al., 2013; Campbell, Foster, & Finkel, 2002; Carlson & DesJardins, 2015; Leckelt, Küfner, Nestler, & Back, in press; Paulhus, 1998). The paradox culminates in the finding that narcissism can simultaneously contribute to being popular and unpopular at zero acquaintance (Back et al., 2013; Küfner, Nestler, & Back, 2013). Back and colleagues (2013; Leckelt et al., in press) have recently developed the Narcissistic Admiration and Rivalry Concept (NARC) as a process model that accounts for these diverging behavioral and social outcomes. They posit that—at the trait level—these paradoxical effects are differentially connected to two narcissistic facets. According to their model, these facets are fed by distinct motivational dynamics determining the way in which narcissists pursue their goal of maintaining a grandiose self. The behavioral pattern of narcissistic admiration is driven by hope for greatness and success, causing a self-regulatory strategy of assertive self-enhancement. In line with this conceptualization, narcissistic admiration comprises feelings of grandiosity, striving for uniqueness, and perceived charmingness. As social interaction outcomes, these tendencies result in narcissists’ social potency—desirable social consequences such as admiration and praise. The behavioral pattern of narcissistic rivalry, in contrast, is driven by fear of failure, prompting a self-regulatory strategy of antagonistic self-protection. In this vein, narcissistic rivalry is conceptualized as comprising striving for supremacy, active devaluation of others, as well as aggressiveness. As social interaction outcomes, these tendencies result in social conflict—negative social consequences such as perceived aggressiveness and unpopularity. In several studies, Back and colleagues (2013) developed a reliable and valid measure of the two facets. Their data indicates that narcissistic admiration and rivalry capture the diversity of narcissistic behavior NARCISSISM AND ENVY 6 in a more comprehensive manner than narcissism as assessed by the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988). Despite the importance of identifying the mechanisms underlying social effects of personality (Back, 2015; Back et al., 2011), the evidence about the processes driving the paradoxical effects of narcissism is still limited (see Leckelt et al., in press for initial findings). In line with previous researchers (e.g., Tracy & Robins, 2007), we argue that specific emotions may have much explanatory power in narcissistic reactions. Often, narcissists show more intense emotional responses than other
Recommended publications
  • Subtypes, Dimensions, Levels, and Mental States in Narcissism and Narcissistic Personality Disorder
    Subtypes, Dimensions, Levels, and Mental States in Narcissism and Narcissistic Personality Disorder Kenneth N. Levy Pennsylvania State University Various conceptualizations of subtypes, levels, and dimensions of narcissism and narcissistic person- ality disorder (NPD) are considered with a particular focus on overt grandiose presentations and covert vulnerable presentations. Evidence supporting this distinction and clinical vignettes to illustrate it are presented as well as their implications for clinical work with NPD patients. The research and clinical evidence points to the conclusion that these broad categorical subtypes are better conceptualized as dimensions on which individual patients vary on relative levels, thus suggesting that grandiose and vul- nerable presentations represent two sides of the same coin. A case example and clinical implications are provided and discussed. C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J. Clin. Psychol: In Session 00:1–12, 2012. Keywords: narcissism; narcissistic personality disorder; grandiose subtype; vulnerable subtype Beginning with its inclusion in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM; 1968, 1980, 1994, 2000), narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) has been conceptualized predominately by its overt grandiose features. However, the definition of NPD articulated in the DSM-III and its successors, DSM-III-R and DSM-IV, has been criticized for failing to fully capture the intended clinical phenomena (Cooper & Ronningstam, 1992; Gabbard, 1989; Gunderson et al., 1991). These authors have noted that the DSM criteria have focused narrowly on aspects of the conceptual approaches of Kernberg and Millon, emphasizing the more overt form of narcissism. However, theoretical and empirical work is now converging to suggest that NPD is not a homogenous disorder and subtypes likely exist within this group.
    [Show full text]
  • Parallel Syndromes: Two Dimensions of Narcissism and the Facets of Psychopathic Personality in Criminally Involved Individuals
    Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 2, No. 2, 113–127 1949-2715/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0021870 Parallel Syndromes: Two Dimensions of Narcissism and the Facets of Psychopathic Personality in Criminally Involved Individuals Michelle Schoenleber, Naomi Sadeh, and Edelyn Verona University of Illinois at Urbana–Champaign Little research has examined different dimensions of narcissism that may parallel psychopathy facets in criminally involved individuals. In this study, we examined the pattern of relationships between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, assessed using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory–16 and the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale, respec- tively, and the four facets of psychopathy (interpersonal, affective, lifestyle, and antisocial) assessed via the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version. As predicted, grandiose and vulnerable narcissism showed differential relationships to psychopathy facets, with gran- diose narcissism relating positively to the interpersonal facet of psychopathy and vulnerable narcissism relating positively to the lifestyle facet of psychopathy. Paralleling existing psychopathy research, vulnerable narcissism showed stronger associations than grandiose narcissism to (a) other forms of psychopathology, including internalizing and substance use disorders, and (b) self- and other-directed aggression, measured with the Life History of Aggression and the Forms of Aggression Questionnaire. Grandiose narcissism was none- theless associated
    [Show full text]
  • Differences in Narcissistic Presentation in Abused and Non- Abused Children and Adolescents
    The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Dissertations Summer 8-2014 Differences in Narcissistic Presentation in Abused and Non- Abused Children and Adolescents Mallory Laine Malkin University of Southern Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/dissertations Part of the Applied Behavior Analysis Commons, Child Psychology Commons, Clinical Psychology Commons, School Psychology Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Malkin, Mallory Laine, "Differences in Narcissistic Presentation in Abused and Non-Abused Children and Adolescents" (2014). Dissertations. 274. https://aquila.usm.edu/dissertations/274 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by The Aquila Digital Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of The Aquila Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The University of Southern Mississippi DIFFERENCES IN NARCISSISTIC PRESENTATION IN ABUSED AND NON-ABUSED CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS by Mallory Laine Malkin Abstract of a Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Southern Mississippi in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy August 2014 ABSTRACT DIFFERENCES IN NARCISSISTIC PRESENTATION IN ABUSED AND NON-ABUSED CHILDREN AND ADOLESCENTS by Mallory Laine Malkin August 2014 The present study examined whether children and adolescents who have been victims of sexual or physical abuse report higher levels of narcissistic tendencies than children and adolescents who have not been victims of abuse. Inaddition to narcissism, internalizing symptoms, externalizing behaviors, and risky behaviors were evaluated, as such issues have been associated with both maltreatment (Baer & Maschi, 2003) and narcissism (Barry & Malkin, 2010; Bushman & Baumeister, 1998).
    [Show full text]
  • Running Heading: NARCISSISM and INTERPERSONAL SITUATIONS
    Running Heading: NARCISSISM AND INTERPERSONAL SITUATIONS Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissistic States in Interpersonal Situations Elizabeth A. Edershile Aidan G.C. Wright University of Pittsburgh This manuscript is currently under review, and therefore should not be treated as the final report. The authors would appreciate critical feedback and suggestions for how to improve the study or its writeup. Word Count: 5,356 Aidan Wright’s effort on this project was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (L30 MH101760). The opinions expressed are solely those of the authors and not those of the funding source. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elizabeth Edershile, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3213 Sennott Square, 210 S. Bouquet St., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260. E-mail: [email protected] NARCISSISM AND INTERPERSONAL SITUATIONS Abstract Clinicians have noted that narcissistic individuals fluctuate over time in their levels of grandiosity and vulnerability. However, these fluctuations remain poorly understood from an empirical perspective. Interpersonal theory asserts that interpersonal situations are central to the expression of personality and psychopathology, and therefore are a key context in which to understand state narcissism’s dynamic processes. The present study is the first to examine state narcissism assessed during interpersonal situations. Specifically, perceptions of others’ warmth and dominance, momentary grandiosity and vulnerability, and one’s own warm and dominant behavior were assessed across situations in daily life in a large sample (person N=286; occasion N=6,837). Results revealed that more grandiose individuals perceived others as colder and behaved in a more dominant and cold fashion, on average. But in the moment, relatively higher grandiosity was associated with perceiving others as warmer and more submissive and resulted in more dominant and warm behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Narcissism, Confidence, and Risk Attitude
    Journal of Behavioral Decision Making J. Behav. Dec. Making, 17: 297–311 (2004) DOI: 10.1002/bdm.475 Narcissism, Confidence, and Risk Attitude W. KEITH CAMPBELL*, ADAM S. GOODIE and JOSHUA D. FOSTER University of Georgia, USA ABSTRACT The present research addresses whether narcissists are more overconfident than others and whether this overconfidence leads to deficits in decision making. In Study 1, nar- cissism predicted overconfidence. This was attributable to narcissists’ greater confi- dence despite no greater accuracy. In Study 2, participants were offered fair bets on their answers. Narcissists lost significantly more points in this betting task than non- narcissists, due both to their greater overconfidence and greater willingness to bet. Finally, in Study 3, narcissists’ predictions of future performance were based on per- formance expectations rather than actual performance. This research extends the litera- ture on betting on knowledge to the important personality dimension of narcissism. Copyright # 2004 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. key words risk taking; overconfidence; decision making; narcissism; personality; adults Individual differences matter in decision making. Gigerenzer and Hoffrage (1995), among many others, have pointed out that average decision strategies can be misleading, as they may reflect decision strategies that no single decision-maker employs. It is often better to explore the decisions that individuals reach. Furthermore, reliable differences in which kinds of people make which kind of decisions are important. Stanovich and West (2000), for example, have defended the reality of various cognitive illusions by demonstrating that people who violate conventionally defined norms tend to be those who perform less well in other domains. Looking at the same base of data, Funder (2000) argued that the observed correlation constitutes standard validation of the test items, and that the ability of some people to solve the problems correctly indicates an absence of systematic irrationality.
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Psychopathy Relate to Humor and Laughter? Dispositions Toward Ridicule and Being Laughed At, the Sense of Humor, and Psychopathic Personality Traits
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2012 How does psychopathy relate to humor and laughter? Dispositions toward ridicule and being laughed at, the sense of humor, and psychopathic personality traits Proyer, Rene T ; Flisch, Rahel ; Tschupp, Stefanie ; Platt, Tracey ; Ruch, Willibald Abstract: This scoping study examines the relation of the sense of humor and three dispositions toward ridicule and being laughed at to psychopathic personality traits. Based on self-reports from 233 adults, psychopathic personality traits were robustly related to enjoying laughing at others, which most strongly related to a manipulative/impulsive lifestyle and callousness. Higher psychopathic traits correlated with bad mood and it existed independently from the ability of laughing at oneself. While overall psychopathic personality traits existed independently from the sense of humor, the facet of superficial charm yielded a robust positive relation. Higher joy in being laughed at also correlated with higher expressions in superficial charm and grandiosity while fearing to be laughed at went along with higher expressions in a manipulative life-style. Thus, the psychopathic personality trait could be well described in its relation to humor and laughter. Implications of the findings are highlighted and discussed with respect to the current literature. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.04.007 Posted at the Zurich Open Repository and Archive, University of Zurich ZORA URL: https://doi.org/10.5167/uzh-62966 Journal Article Accepted Version Originally published at: Proyer, Rene T; Flisch, Rahel; Tschupp, Stefanie; Platt, Tracey; Ruch, Willibald (2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Intelligence Is Used by Dark Personalities to Emotionally Manipulate Others ⇑ ⇑ Ursa K.J
    Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2014) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Is there a ‘‘dark intelligence’’? Emotional intelligence is used by dark personalities to emotionally manipulate others ⇑ ⇑ Ursa K.J. Nagler a, ,1, Katharina J. Reiter a, ,1, Marco R. Furtner a, John F. Rauthmann b a Institute of Psychology, Leopold-Franzens Universität Innsbruck, Austria b Institute of Psychology, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Germany article info abstract Article history: Potential ‘‘darker sides’’ of socio-emotional intelligence (SEI) have been repeatedly noted. We examine Available online xxxx whether SEI is associated with emotional manipulation of others when used by dark personalities (Dark Triad: narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy). In N = 594 participants, narcissism was positively, Keywords: Machiavellianism negatively, and psychopathy positively and negatively associated with SEI. Moreover, Emotional manipulation narcissism and psychopathy moderated links between facets of emotional intelligence and emotional Dark Triad manipulation. Findings are discussed in context of a ‘‘dark intelligence’’ used for malicious intents. Narcissism Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Machiavellianism Psychopathy Emotional intelligence Social intelligence 1. Introduction & Sachse, 2010), including communication competence (e.g., Diez, 1984), social intelligence (e.g., Cantor & Kihlstrom, 1987; Gardner, Are social and emotional skills always used for good intentions? 1993; Guilford, 1967; Thorndike, 1920), and emotional intelligence Potential ‘‘dark sides’’ of socio-emotional intelligence (SEI), such as (e.g., Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Not only the emotional manipulation of others (Austin, Farrelly, Black, & interpersonal (e.g., encoding and decoding social information) Moore, 2007), have garnered interest during the last years.
    [Show full text]
  • Narcissistic Personality Disorder – Has It Become an Epidemic? 3 CE Hours
    Chapter 7: Narcissistic Personality Disorder – Has It Become an Epidemic? 3 CE Hours By: Kathryn Brohl, MA, LMFT Learning objectives Upon completion of this course, the learner should be able to: Describe at least three relationship characteristics of individuals Define narcissistic personality disorder (NPD) as it relates to the with NPD. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, Identify at least five diagnostic criteria for NPD as they relate to Fifth Edition (DSM-5) (2013). the DSM-5. Identify personality disorder as it relates to the DSM-5. Recognize challenges to treating individuals with NPD. Compare NPD to other Cluster B Personality Disorders, outlined Describe treatment for individuals with NPD. in the Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders, Fifth Edition, Compare and describe ICD Revision 10 personality disorder (2013). criteria to DSM-5 personality disorder criteria. Explain the male and female prevalence of NPD. Explain how NPD affects relationships. Introduction Narcissism has been examined by mental health professionals for years, society is more accepting of persons with NPD, or does the trend in and yet something has occurred recently within our society that seems to cultivating entitlement as it relates to expressing one’s opinions and have blurred narcissism traits with the clinical diagnosis of narcissistic behaviors more loudly simply mean that people are feeling more personality disorder (NPD) and accepted societal behavior. Recently, a empowered? And as a result, are people becoming desensitized to the shift has occurred in shared cultural values toward narcissism and self- feelings of others? Furthermore, are mental health professionals seeing admiration and some of these traits have become social norms.
    [Show full text]
  • Fluctuations in Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissistic States: a Momentary Perspective
    1 Fluctuations in grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic states: A momentary perspective Elizabeth A. Edershile Aidan G.C. Wright University of Pittsburgh This manuscript reflects a portion of the first author’s Master’s thesis. Aidan Wright’s effort on this project was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (L30 MH101760). Participant recruitment was supported by the University of Pittsburgh’s Clinical and Translational Science Institute, which is funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Clinical and Translational Science Award (CTSA) program (UL1 TR001857). The CTSA program is led by the NIH's National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS). The opinions expressed are solely those of the authors and not those of the funding source. This manuscript and supporting data and analyses has been posted on Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://osf.io/c9uea/ Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Elizabeth Edershile, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3137 Sennott Square, 210 S. Bouquet St., Pittsburgh, PA, 15260. E-mail: [email protected] 2 Abstract Theories of narcissism emphasize the dynamic processes within and between grandiosity and vulnerability. Research seeking to address this has either not studied grandiosity and vulnerability together or has used dispositional measures to assess what are considered to be momentary states. Emerging models of narcissism suggest grandiosity and vulnerability can further be differentiated into a three-factor structure – Exhibitionistic Grandiosity, Entitlement, and Vulnerability. Research in other areas of maladaptive personality (e.g., borderline personality disorder) has made headway in engaging data collection and analytic methods that are specifically meant to examine such questions.
    [Show full text]
  • Origins of Narcissism in Children
    Origins of narcissism in children Eddie Brummelmana,b,1, Sander Thomaesb,c, Stefanie A. Nelemansd, Bram Orobio de Castrob, Geertjan Overbeeka, and Brad J. Bushmane,f aResearch Institute of Child Development and Education, Department of Educational Sciences, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1001 NG, The Netherlands; bDepartment of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; cCenter for Research on Self and Identity, Department of Psychology, University of Southampton, Southampton SO17 1BJ, England; dResearch Centre Adolescent Development, Department of Youth and Family, Utrecht University, Utrecht 3584 CS, The Netherlands; eDepartment of Communication and Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210-1339; and fDepartment of Communication Science, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1081 HV, The Netherlands Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved February 12, 2015 (received for review November 7, 2014) Narcissism levels have been increasing among Western youth, and (9) and “are under a compulsion to ascribe every perfection to contribute to societal problems such as aggression and violence. the child—which sober observation would find no occasion to The origins of narcissism, however, are not well understood. Here, do” (10). Consequently, children might internalize the belief that we report, to our knowledge, the first prospective longitudinal they are special individuals who are entitled to privileges. In evidence on the origins of narcissism in children. We compared contrast, psychoanalytic theory holds that children are likely to two perspectives: social learning theory (positing that narcissism is grow up to be narcissistic when their parents lack warmth toward cultivated by parental overvaluation) and psychoanalytic theory them (11, 12).
    [Show full text]
  • Two Faces of Narcissism
    Two Faces of Narcissism Paul Wink Institute of Personality Assessment and Research University of California, Berkeley The present study examines the lack of strong correlations among existing self-report measures of narcissism. A principal-components analysis of 6 MMPI narcissism scales resulted in 2 orthogonal factors, 1 implying Vulnerability-Sensitivity and the other Grandiosity-Exhibitionism. Although unrelated to each other, these 2 factors were associated with such core features of narcissism as conceit, self-indulgence, and disregard of others. Despite this common core, however, Vulnerabil- ity-Sensitivity was associated with introversion, defensiveness, anxiety, and vulnerability to life's traumas, whereas Grandiosity-Exhibitionism was related to extraversion, self-assurance, exhibi- tionism, and aggression. Three alternative interpretations of these results are considered, and an argument for the distinction between covert and overt narcissism is made. The recent resurgence of interest in narcissism has led to the The NPDS (Ashby et al., 1979) is the only narcissism scale proliferation of self-report measures of the construct. Several of developed empirically by contrasting item endorsement rates the newly developed narcissism scales, including the most of diagnosed narcissists in treatment with control groups of widely researched Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; other patients and individuals not in treatment. Wink and Raskin & Hall, 1979, 1981), have shown high intercorrelations. Gough (1990) have shown that the NPDS correlates positively Surprisingly, however, the NPI does not correlate with the Nar- with Serkownek's (1975) narcissism-hypersensitivity and with cissistic Personality Disorder Scale (NPDS; Ashby, Lee, & Pepper and Strong's (1958) ego-sensitivity scales, two measures Duke, 1979), another widely used measure of the construct of narcissism derived from MMPI's (Hathaway & McKinley, (Emmons, 1987; Watson, Grisham, Trotter, & Biderman, 1940) Masculinity-Femininity scale.
    [Show full text]
  • How Narcissism Destroys
    Rune Fardal, studerer psykologi Personlighetsforstyrrelser med hovedvekt på narsissistisk problematikk i relasjon til barn http://www.sakkyndig.com mail: [email protected] How narcissism destroys 10 Desember, 2001, Oppdatert 10.02.2014 The development of the relation between the child and the parent is extremely important. This relationship is what forms the child’s personality. In order for this development be successful for the child, some important criteria must be met. Lets look at 10 of these traits. First Aggressive impulses from the infant child, must be neutralized by the adult, so that they don’t upset the child. The adult must be able to be a “container” for the child’s frustration and needs. At this stage the child’s communication can be described as projective identification! The child try to make the adult act in a preferable way. The child project its own frustration over in the adult, and identifies with this frustration in the adults processed expression of these traits. If the outcome of this processing is, in a form the child can not handle, then the adult has failed its role as container for the child’s aggression and frustration. An example would be fear projected in an adult, the adult reaction to this projected fear decides the child’s developmental brain structure. If the adult’s reaction to the projected fear is comfort, the child develop security in the relation with the adult and others. If the adult’s reaction is insecurity and frustration, then that’s what the child will experience. That’s what is mirrored back at the child.
    [Show full text]