The Social Neuroscience of Empathy

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Social Neuroscience of Empathy The Social Neuroscience of Empathy edited by Jean Decety and William Ickes A Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge, Massachusetts London, England © 2009 Massachusetts Institute of Technology All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form by any electronic or mechani- cal means (including photocopying, recording, or information storage and retrieval) without permission in writing from the publisher. For information about special quantity discounts, please e-mail special_sales@mitpress.mit.edu This book was set in Stone Sans and Stone Serif by SNP Best-set Typesetter Ltd., Hong Kong. Printed and bound in the United States of America. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data The social neuroscience of empathy / edited by Jean Decety and William Ickes. p. cm.—(Social neuroscience) “A Bradford book.” Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-0-262-01297-3 (hardcover : alk. paper) 1. Empathy. 2. Neurosciences. 3. Social psychology. I. Decety, Jean. II. Ickes, William John. BF575.E55S63 2009 155.2′32—dc22 2008034814 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Subject Index Abstract reasoning, 132–133 Altruistic motivation, 9 Academic achievement, 87–88 Amygdala, 128, 141, 174, 228 teacher empathy and, 88–89 cognitive empathy and, 224 Accurate empathy, 57. See also Empathic conditioning and, 144 accuracy emotion representation and, 188 benefi ts and limitations of, 154–156 facial expressions and, 140, 141, 174, 201, 207 Acquaintanceship effect, 64 fear and, 140, 141, 204 Actualizing tendency, 102 and mediation of emotional experiences, 227 organismic infl uence of, 102–103 morality and, 144 Adolescent adjustment, empathic accuracy in pain and, 77 peer relations and, 63 selective sociality, neuropeptides, and, 178, Aesthetic empathy, 6, 7 179 Affective component of empathy, 153–154. sex differences and, 179 See also Empathy, components/dimensions simulation and, 226, 228 Affective empathy, 6, 215, 218–220, 226, 227. Analogical reasoning, 209 See also Emotional contagion; Emotional Anger, emotional empathic responses to empathy; Simulation and reasoning about conventions, 145–146 Affective mentalizing, 223. See also Mentalizing Anger-mediated aggressive behavior, 87 Affective resonance, 71, 77, 184, 185, 200, 201. Angry expressions, 141 See also Empathic resonance Animals. See also Evolution Affective responsiveness, 87 empathy and emotional contagion in, 25, 71, Affective sharing, 109, 185, 189 169 Affi nity continuum, 160–161 Anterior attention system, 74 Agency, sense of, 206 Anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), 38, 77, 78, 127, Aggression, 86–87. See also Anger; Violent 186, 189, 201, 224–227 husbands Anterior insula, 38, 77, 186, 187, 189, 190, 201, regulation of, 86 225. See also Insula Alexithymia, 160 Anterior paracingulate cortex, 141 Altruistic behavior, 85, 203. See also Imitation, Anterior temporal cortex, 128 prosocial effects of being imitated Antisocial behavior, 143–144 Altruistic concern, 161 regulation of, 86 246 Subject Index Anxiety. See also Fear Caring, 86 social, 224, 225 (see also Social phobia) “Chameleon effect,” 184 Appeasement displays, empathic responses to, Cheek muscle region, 21 147–148 Cingulate cortex, 201, 226. See also Anterior Appraisal theory, 202 cingulate cortex Approach-avoidance reactions, 176, 177 Citizen Curriculum, 91 “As if” condition, 103, 125, 126 Classroom, empathy-stimulating experiences in, Asperger syndrome (AS), 219, 224, 225 90. See also Student empathy, fostering Attachment processes, 78–79 Client-centered therapy, 109–110. See also Attentional disengagement, 61 Psychotherapy; Rogerian empathy Attentional resources, 141 conditions of the therapeutic process, 102–104 Audience design hypothesis, 47, 52 empathy in, 103–104 Audition and social communication, 172 and activities of therapist, 104–105 Auditory communication, 172 theory, 101–102 Auditory mirror neuron systems, 131 therapeutic relationship, 105, 132 Autism, 26, 60, 146–147, 180, 190–192, 224, 225 Client empathic resonance. See Empathic Automatic imitation, 32 resonance, in psychotherapy Automatic mimicry, 200 Clinical empathy. See Psychotherapy, empathy Automatic/uncontrolled vs. deliberative/ in; Rogerian empathy refl ective/controlled empathic reactions, 157 Cognition Automatic vigilance, 113 embodied, 209 Autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity. social, 188, 191, 200, 216, 217 See also Evolving autonomic and social Cognitive-affective model of empathy, 85, 86, 91 nervous system Cognitive appraisal and reappraisal, 207 facial feedback and, 23 Cognitive-behavioral therapies, 120 Cognitive capacities related to empathy, Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (BEES), 189 132–133 Basal ganglia, 205 Cognitive component of empathy, 153. Belief-reasoning errors, 220 See also Cognitive empathy; Empathy, Bilateral insulae, 77 components/dimensions Bilateral insular cortices, 204 Cognitive-emotional empathy, 133 Bipolar disorder, 145, 224, 225 Cognitive empathy, 4, 7, 33, 218, 224. Body-oriented perspective. See Embodied See also Projection; Theory of mind empathy defi ned, 141 Borderline personality disorder (BPD), 61, 224, and moral and conventional reasoning, 225 146–147 Brain damage and empathic abilities, 217–219. nature of, 215 See also specifi c regions of brain and other types of empathy, 133, 139, 215, Brow muscle region, 21 218–220 “Burnout,” 156 Cognitive fl exibility, 132, 133, 217–219 Cognitive mentalizing, 223. See also Mentalizing Canonical neurons, 24 Cognitive perspective taking. See Perspective Caregiving, interest in, 153 taking Subject Index 247 Cognitive (processing) style, 36–37 Dialogue, empathic, 116 and interpersonal behavior, 37 Domain-specifi c modules, 186 Cognitive psychology and empathic accuracy, 62 Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, 77 Common ground, 47 Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLC), 218, 219, Common-ground assumptions, 44 225 in communication, 47–48 Common knowledge, 48, 52 Effortful control, 71, 74–76, 79 Compassion, 8, 86, 117, 153, 161 Egocentrism, 203 Comprehension, 47–48 overprojection as, 49 Concept, empathy of. See Aesthetic empathy Egoistic motivation, 9 Conceptualizing skills, 119 Einfühlung, 6, 31, 38, 183 Concern, empathic/other-oriented, 131, 183, Electromyography (EMG), 20 188, 189, 199, 202, 208. See also Sympathy Embarrassment, 147, 148 Confl ict monitoring, 38 Embodied cognition, 209 Congruence in psychotherapy, 102, 103 Embodied empathy, 113–114, 116–117, 120. Congruence with other person’s welfare, 8 See also Resonating, bodily; Somatic Conscientiousness, 76 resonance Contact refl ections, 116 Emotion, 19 Contagion, 24–26 Emotional arousal, 11 Context/contextual factors, 129–130. See also Emotional competence, 86 Relationship between empathizer and target Emotional contagion, 6, 19, 24–26. See also of empathy Affective empathy Cooperation, 86, 170, 173 in animals, 25, 169 Cooperative learning curriculum, 90 defi ned, 19 Corrugator supercilii (brow) muscle region, 21 facial expression and, 32, 225 Corticobasal degeneration, 223 implications of existing research regarding, 26 Corticospinal motor representations, 201 in infants and toddlers, 185 Counterempathic responding, 208 mechanisms and stages in the process of, 20–26 Countertransference (psychotherapy), 119, 132 merging and, 131 Cross-target consistency, 59 mimicry and, 32 Curriculum Transformation Project, 92–94 vs. other types of empathy, 73, 206 pain and, 78, 155 Decentering, 7, 131 primitive, 19, 20, 27 Defensiveness, 178 questions regarding, 26–27 Deliberate/intentional empathic responses, 157 shared affect and, 184 Development, moral, 139. See also Moral Emotional Contagion Scale, 20, 189 reasoning Emotional countercontagion, 61 Developmental approaches to empathy, 185 Emotional empathy, 133, 139–141, 189, 228. Developmental issues, 76–77 See also Affective empathy; Simulation Developmental psychology and empathic Emotional intelligence, 67, 88 accuracy, 62–63 Emotional Regulation Scale, 189 Dialogical perspective, 113, 114, 120. See also Emotional resonance. See Affective resonance Psychotherapy Emotional responding, 139 248 Subject Index Emotion “catching,” 5, 6, 20. See also Emotional Empathic processing, 215, 227–228. contagion See also specifi c topics ways emotions can be caught, 20–26 Empathic resonance, 134. See also Affective Emotion matching, 5, 6 resonance; Resonating Emotion regulation, 73, 115, 119, 189, 206–207, in psychotherapy, 114–118, 125 216. See also Self-regulation Empathic responding, modulation of, 206–209 Emotions, primary vs. secondary, 132 Empathic understanding, 102–104, 109, 119, Empathic ability, 60, 65. See also Empathic 134, 155 accuracy Empathic Understanding Responses, 101 Empathic accuracy, 4, 57, 67–68, 115, 156. Empathy See also Pain assessment, accuracy in as answer to two different questions, 3–4, 8–11 in clinical psychology, 59–62 components/dimensions of, 91, 103, 117–118, cognitive psychology and, 62 133, 153–154 (see also Cognitive-affective contributions of different information channels model of empathy) to, 62 conditions for (see under Psychotherapy) defi ned, 57 consequences of showing, 33–34 developmental psychology and, 62–63 (see also Imitation) individual differences in perceivers’, 59 defi nitions/uses of the term, 19, 31, 43, 71, 85, physiological psychology and, 65–66 103, 104, 125, 126, 139, 183–185 social psychology and, 64–65 concept 1: knowing another’s internal state, Empathic accuracy construct, “theory neutral” 4, 8–11 nature of, 67 concept 2: adopting the posture/expression of Empathic accuracy measures, 58–59, 155. an observed other, 4, 8–10 See
Recommended publications
  • University of Groningen Empathic Accuracy and Oxytocin After
    University of Groningen Empathic accuracy and oxytocin after tryptophan depletion in adults at risk for depression Hogenelst, Koen; Schoevers, Robert A.; Kema, Ido; Sweep, Fred C G J; aan het Rot, Marije Published in: Psychopharmacology DOI: 10.1007/s00213-015-4093-9 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2016 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Hogenelst, K., Schoevers, R. A., Kema, I. P., Sweep, F. C. G. J., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2016). Empathic accuracy and oxytocin after tryptophan depletion in adults at risk for depression. Psychopharmacology, 233(1), 111-120 . DOI: 10.1007/s00213-015-4093-9 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 11-02-2018 Psychopharmacology (2016) 233:111–120 DOI 10.1007/s00213-015-4093-9 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Empathic accuracy and oxytocin after tryptophan depletion in adults at risk for depression Koen Hogenelst1,2 & Robert A.
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy, Mirror Neurons and SYNC
    Mind Soc (2016) 15:1–25 DOI 10.1007/s11299-014-0160-x Empathy, mirror neurons and SYNC Ryszard Praszkier Received: 5 March 2014 / Accepted: 25 November 2014 / Published online: 14 December 2014 Ó The Author(s) 2014. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract This article explains how people synchronize their thoughts through empathetic relationships and points out the elementary neuronal mechanisms orchestrating this process. The many dimensions of empathy are discussed, as is the manner by which empathy affects health and disorders. A case study of teaching children empathy, with positive results, is presented. Mirror neurons, the recently discovered mechanism underlying empathy, are characterized, followed by a theory of brain-to-brain coupling. This neuro-tuning, seen as a kind of synchronization (SYNC) between brains and between individuals, takes various forms, including frequency aspects of language use and the understanding that develops regardless of the difference in spoken tongues. Going beyond individual- to-individual empathy and SYNC, the article explores the phenomenon of syn- chronization in groups and points out how synchronization increases group cooperation and performance. Keywords Empathy Á Mirror neurons Á Synchronization Á Social SYNC Á Embodied simulation Á Neuro-synchronization 1 Introduction We sometimes feel as if we just resonate with something or someone, and this feeling seems far beyond mere intellectual cognition. It happens in various situations, for example while watching a movie or connecting with people or groups. What is the mechanism of this ‘‘resonance’’? Let’s take the example of watching and feeling a film, as movies can affect us deeply, far more than we might realize at the time.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neural Components of Empathy: Predicting Daily Prosocial Behavior
    doi:10.1093/scan/nss088 SCAN (2014) 9,39^ 47 The neural components of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior Sylvia A. Morelli, Lian T. Rameson, and Matthew D. Lieberman Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563 Previous neuroimaging studies on empathy have not clearly identified neural systems that support the three components of empathy: affective con- gruence, perspective-taking, and prosocial motivation. These limitations stem from a focus on a single emotion per study, minimal variation in amount of social context provided, and lack of prosocial motivation assessment. In the current investigation, 32 participants completed a functional magnetic resonance imaging session assessing empathic responses to individuals experiencing painful, anxious, and happy events that varied in valence and amount of social context provided. They also completed a 14-day experience sampling survey that assessed real-world helping behaviors. The results demonstrate that empathy for positive and negative emotions selectively activates regions associated with positive and negative affect, respectively. Downloaded from In addition, the mirror system was more active during empathy for context-independent events (pain), whereas the mentalizing system was more active during empathy for context-dependent events (anxiety, happiness). Finally, the septal area, previously linked to prosocial motivation, was the only region that was commonly activated across empathy for pain, anxiety, and happiness. Septal activity during each of these empathic experiences was predictive of daily helping. These findings suggest that empathy has multiple input pathways, produces affect-congruent activations, and results in septally mediated prosocial motivation. http://scan.oxfordjournals.org/ Keywords: empathy; prosocial behavior; septal area INTRODUCTION neuroimaging studies of empathy, 30 focused on empathy for pain Human beings are intensely social creatures who have a need to belong (Fan et al., 2011).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neural Bases of Empathic Accuracy
    The neural bases of empathic accuracy Jamil Zaki1, Jochen Weber, Niall Bolger, and Kevin Ochsner Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027 Edited by Michael I. Posner, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR, and approved May 14, 2009 (received for review March 12, 2009) Theories of empathy suggest that an accurate understanding of remains unknown, because extant methods are unable to address another’s emotions should depend on affective, motor, and/or this question. To explore the neural bases of accurate interper- higher cognitive brain regions, but until recently no experimental sonal understanding, a method would need to indicate that method has been available to directly test these possibilities. Here, activation of a neural system predicts a match between perceiv- we present a functional imaging paradigm that allowed us to ers’ beliefs about targets’ internal states and the states targets address this issue. We found that empathically accurate, as com- report themselves to be experiencing (21). pared with inaccurate, judgments depended on (i) structures Instead, extant studies have probed neural activity while within the human mirror neuron system thought to be involved in perceivers passively view or experience actions and sensory shared sensorimotor representations, and (ii) regions implicated in states or make judgments about fictional targets whose internal mental state attribution, the superior temporal sulcus and medial states are implied in pictures or fictional vignettes. In either case, prefrontal cortex. These data demostrate that activity in these 2 comparisons between perceiver judgments and what targets sets of brain regions tracks with the accuracy of attributions made actually experienced cannot be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Theory of Mind and Empathy As Multidimensional Constructs Neurological Foundations
    Top Lang Disorders Vol. 34, No. 4, pp. 282–295 Copyright c 2014 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Theory of Mind and Empathy as Multidimensional Constructs Neurological Foundations Jonathan Dvash and Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory Empathy describes an individual’s ability to understand and feel the other. In this article, we review recent theoretical approaches to the study of empathy. Recent evidence supports 2 possible empathy systems: an emotional system and a cognitive system. These processes are served by separate, albeit interacting, brain networks. When a cognitive empathic response is generated, the theory of mind (ToM) network (i.e., medial prefrontal cortex, superior temporal sulcus, temporal poles) and the affective ToM network (mainly involving the ventromedial prefrontal cortex) are typically involved. In contrast, the emotional empathic response is driven mainly by simulation and involves regions that mediate emotional experiences (i.e., amygdala, insula). A decreased empathic response may be due to deficits in mentalizing (cognitive ToM, affective ToM) or in simulation processing (emotional empathy), with these deficits mediated by different neural systems. It is proposed that a balanced activation of these 2 networks is required for appropriate social behavior. Key words: emotion, empathy, inferior frontal gyrus, mirror neurons, simulation, theory of mind, ventromedial prefrontal cortex NE of the core functions of individu- have provided evidence of the multidimen- O als living within a society is the attribu- sional nature of ToM. In this article, we review tion of mental states to others. This function, main approaches to the study of the neural ba- known as theory of mind (ToM) or “mental- sis of ToM and empathy (including tasks used izing” (Frith, 1999), enables an individual to to elicit them), describe the neurological un- understand or predict another person’s be- derpinnings for the multidimensional nature havior and to react accordingly.
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy: a Social Cognitive Neuroscience Approach Lian T
    Social and Personality Psychology Compass 3/1 (2009): 94–110, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00154.x Empathy: A Social Cognitive Neuroscience Approach Lian T. Rameson* and Matthew D. Lieberman Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles Abstract There has been recent widespread interest in the neural underpinnings of the experience of empathy. In this review, we take a social cognitive neuroscience approach to understanding the existing literature on the neuroscience of empathy. A growing body of work suggests that we come to understand and share in the experiences of others by commonly recruiting the same neural structures both during our own experience and while observing others undergoing the same experience. This literature supports a simulation theory of empathy, which proposes that we understand the thoughts and feelings of others by using our own mind as a model. In contrast, theory of mind research suggests that medial prefrontal regions are critical for understanding the minds of others. In this review, we offer ideas about how to integrate these two perspectives, point out unresolved issues in the literature, and suggest avenues for future research. In a way, most of our lives cannot really be called our own. We spend much of our time thinking about and reacting to the thoughts, feelings, intentions, and behaviors of others, and social psychology has demonstrated the manifold ways that our lives are shared with and shaped by our social relationships. It is a marker of the extreme sociality of our species that those who don’t much care for other people are at best labeled something unflattering like ‘hermit’, and at worst diagnosed with a disorder like ‘psychopathy’ or ‘autism’.
    [Show full text]
  • Neural Correlates of Empathic Accuracy in Adolescence
    Title: Neural correlates of empathic accuracy in adolescence Running title: fMRI of empathic accuracy in adolescence Authors: Tammi RA Kral1,2,3, Enrique Solis1, Jeanette A Mumford1, Brianna S Schuyler1, Lisa Flook1, Katharine Rifken1, Elena G Patsenko1, Richard J Davidson1,2,3 1. Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin – Madison, 625 West Washington Avenue, Madison, WI, USA 53703 2. Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin – Madison, 1202 West Johnson Street, Madison, WI, USA 53706 3. Waisman Center, University of Wisconsin – Madison, 1500 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI, USA 53705 Corresponding author: Richard J Davidson, Center for Healthy Minds, University of Wisconsin – Madison, 625 West Washington Avenue, Madison, WI, USA 53703; Phone: (608) 265-8189; Email: rjdavids@wisc.edu. Tables: 1 Figures: 4 Supplementary Figures: 2 Words in Abstract: 200 Words in Manuscript: 5527 © The Author (2017). Published by Oxford University Press. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non- Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits unrestricted noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/scan/nsx099/4107538/Neural-correlates-of-empathic-accuracy-in by University of Wisconsin-Madison Libraries user on 18 September 2017 Abstract Empathy, the ability to understand others’ emotions, can occur through perspective taking and experience sharing. Neural systems active when adults empathize include regions underlying perspective taking (e.g. medial prefrontal cortex; MPFC), and experience sharing (e.g. inferior parietal lobule; IPL). It is unknown whether adolescents utilize networks implicated in both experience sharing and perspective taking when accurately empathizing.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neural Correlates of Empathy: Experience, Automaticity, and Prosocial Behavior
    The Neural Correlates of Empathy: Experience, Automaticity, and Prosocial Behavior Lian T. Rameson, Sylvia A. Morelli, and Matthew D. Lieberman Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/24/1/235/1780305/jocn_a_00130.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Abstract ■ Empathy is a critical aspect of human emotion that influences conditions: watching naturally, under cognitive load, and while the behavior of individuals as well as the functioning of society. empathizing. Across conditions, higher levels of self-reported ex- Although empathy is fundamentally a subjective experience, no perienced empathy were associated with greater activity in medial studies have yet examined the neural correlates of the self-reported PFC (MPFC). Activity in MPFC was also correlated with daily help- experience of empathy. Furthermore, although behavioral re- ing behavior. Self-report of empathic experience and activity in search has linked empathy to prosocial behavior, no work has empathy-related areas, notably MPFC, were higher in the empathize yet connected empathy-related neural activity to everyday, real- condition than in the load condition, suggesting that empathy is world helping behavior. Lastly, the widespread assumption that not a fully automatic experience. Additionally, high trait empathy empathy is an automatic experience remains largely untested. participants displayed greater experienced empathy and stronger It is also unknown whether differences in trait empathy reflect MPFC responses than low trait empathy individuals under cog- either variability in the automaticity of empathic responses or nitive load, suggesting that empathy is more automatic for indi- the capacity to feel empathy. In this study, 32 participants com- viduals high in trait empathy.
    [Show full text]
  • DOCTORAL THESIS the Empathy Fillip: Can Training in Microexpressions of Emotion Enhance Empathic Accuracy? Eyles, Kieren
    DOCTORAL THESIS The empathy fillip: Can training in microexpressions of emotion enhance empathic accuracy? Eyles, Kieren Award date: 2016 General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. • Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the public portal for the purpose of private study or research. • You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain • You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the public portal ? Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 04. Oct. 2021 The empathy fillip: Can training in microexpressions of emotion enhance empathic accuracy? Presented by: Kieren Eyles A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of PsychD, Department of Psychology, University of Roehampton 2015 Abstract Empathy is a central concern in the counselling process. Though much researched, and broadly commented upon, empathy is still largely understood through the words within a client-counsellor interaction. This semantic focus continues despite converging lines of evidence that suggest other elements of an interaction – for example body language – may be involved in the communication of empathy. In this thesis, the foundations of empathy are examined, focusing on empathy’s professional instantiation.
    [Show full text]
  • Empathic Accuracy and Cognitive and Affective Empathy in Young Adults with and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder
    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-05093-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Empathic Accuracy and Cognitive and Afective Empathy in Young Adults With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder K. McKenzie1 · A. Russell1 · D. Golm2 · G. Fairchild3 Accepted: 14 May 2021 © The Author(s) 2021 Abstract This study investigated whether young adults with ASD (n = 29) had impairments in Cognitive Empathy (CE), Afective Empathy (AE) or Empathic Accuracy (EA; the ability to track changes in others’ thoughts and feelings) compared to typically- developing individuals (n = 31) using the Empathic Accuracy Task (EAT), which involves watching narrators recollecting emotionally-charged autobiographical events. Participants provided continuous ratings of the narrators’ emotional intensity (indexing EA), labelled the emotions displayed (CE) and reported whether they shared the depicted emotions (AE). The ASD group showed defcits in EA for anger but did not difer from typically-developing participants in CE or AE on the EAT. The ASD group also reported lower CE (Perspective Taking) and AE (Empathic Concern) on the Interpersonal Reactivity Index, a self-report questionnaire. Keywords Autism · ASD · Empathy · Afective empathy · Empathic accuracy · Alexithymia Introduction and “trait empathy”, a personality tendency which is rela- tively stable over time (Song et al., 2019). Empathy can be defned as the ability to share others’ feel- A wide range of methods have been used to measure CE ings or ‘put yourself in their shoes’ (Singer & Lamm,
    [Show full text]
  • The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Presents Structural Variations Associated with Empathic Capacity in Psychotherapists
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.01.02.425096; this version posted January 30, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-ND 4.0 International license. The Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Presents Structural Variations Associated with Empathic Capacity in Psychotherapists Marcos E. Domínguez-Arriola1,&, Víctor E. Olalde-Mathieu1,&, Eduardo A. Garza-Villareal1, Fernando A. Barrios1, * 1 Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Instituto de Neurobiología, Querétaro, México & Authors with the same amount of responsibility *Corresponding Author Instituto de Neurobiología Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México Campus Juriquilla Blvd. Juriquilla 3001, Querétaro, Querétaro, 76230, México +52(442)2381-53 barrios@inb.unam.mx Abstract Empathic capacity has been shown to be correlated with brain structural variations, such as cortical thickness. Since psychotherapists have a constant demand to modulate their empathic response, in this study we compared cortical thickness between a group of psychotherapists and a control group at prefrontal and cingulate brain regions, and investigated how this is correlated with their empathic skills. Eighteen psychotherapists and eighteen healthy controls underwent 3-Tesla MRI scanning and completed empathy-related psychometric assessments. Cortical thickness (CT) measures were estimated for each participant. We evaluated how these measures differed between groups, and if they were associated with individual empathy-related scores in a series of regions of interest. Our analysis shows that psychotherapists display a significantly greater CT at a region in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC; p < 0.05, FDR corrected).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neural Networks Underlying Reappraisal of Empathy for Pain
    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2020, 733–744 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa094 Advance Access Publication Date: 23 July 2020 Original Manuscript The neural networks underlying reappraisal of empathy for pain Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/scan/article/15/7/733/5875520 by guest on 11 January 2021 Navot Naor,1 Christiane Rohr,2 Lina H Schaare,2 Chirag Limbachia,1 Simone Shamay-Tsoory,3 and Hadas Okon-Singer3 1University of Maryland, Department of Psychology, College Park, MD 20742-5031, USA, 2Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Department of Neurology, Leipzig 04103, Germany and 3University of Haifa, Department of Psychology, Haifa 3498838, Israel Correspondence should be addressed to Navot Naor, Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Biology/Psychology Building, 4094 Campus Dr., College Park, MD 20742, USA. E-mail: navotnaor@gmail.com. Abstract Emotion regulation plays a central role in empathy. Only by successfully regulating our own emotions can we reliably use them in order to interpret the content and valence of others’ emotions correctly. In an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)-based experiment, we show that regulating one’s emotion via reappraisal modulated biased emotional intensity ratings following an empathy for pain manipulation. Task-based analysis revealed increased activity in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) when painful emotions were regulated using reappraisal, whereas empathic feelings that were not regulated resulted in increased activity bilaterally in the precuneus, supramarginal gyrus and middle frontal gyrus (MFG), as well as the right parahippocampal gyrus. Functional connectivity analysis indicated that the right IFG plays a role in the regulation of empathy for pain, through its connections with regions in the empathy for pain network.
    [Show full text]