<<

Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Counterstrategies

Ryne A. Palombit

Department of Anthropology, Center for Evolutionary Studies, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08901 Correspondence: [email protected]

One of the earliest recognized forms of sexual conflict was by males, which imposes serious costs on female reproductive success. Here I review two bodies of evidence addressing coevolved strategies of males and . The original hy- pothesis arguing that infanticide improves male mating success by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition has been generally supported in some taxa—notably, some , carnivores, , and cetaceans—but not in other taxa. One result of recent research has been to implicate other selective benefits of infanticide by males in various taxa from to to , such as acquisition of breeding status or improvement of the female breeding condition. In some cases, however, the adaptive sig- nificance of male infanticide remains obscure. The second body of data I review is arguably the most important result of recent research: clarifying the possible female counterstrategies to infanticide. These potential counterstrategies span diverse biological systems, ranging from sexual behavior (e.g., polyandrous mating), to physiology (e.g., the ), to individual behavior (e.g., maternal aggression), to social strategies (e.g., association with coalitionary defenders of either ). Although much remains to be studied, these current data provide compelling evidence of sexually antagonistic coevolution surrounding the phenomenon of infanticide.

t its most elemental level, infanticide is the and Hausfater 1984, p. xv). These definitions Akilling of a newborn individual by a conspe- highlight the heterogeneous and variable cific. With the growing appreciation of its bio- of the phenomenon, which can be perpetrated logical significance, however, infanticide came by either sex, by or other kin, by indi- to be defined more broadly as any “behavior viduals unrelated to the victim, in a wide variety that makes a direct and significant contribution of social and mating systems, under a range of to the immediate death of an embryo or newly seasonal or aseasonal breeding regimes, and hatched or born member of the performer’s across diverse taxa straddling vertebrates and own ” (Mock 1984, p. 4) or “any form invertebrates. of lethal curtailment of in One adaptive form of infanticide—the kill- offspring brought about by conspecifics” (Hrdy ing of infants by unrelated males—is arguably

Editors: William R. Rice and Sergey Gavrilets Additional Perspectives on The and of Sexual Conflict available at www.cshperspectives.org Copyright # 2015 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; all rights reserved; doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a017640 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640

1 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

the archetype of sexual conflict. In 450 BCE, as well as additional data from pri- Herodotus not only documented the behavior mates. among Egyptian , but explained it as a male “trick” to obtain sexual access to females other- Sexual Selection Hypothesis wise preoccupied with maternal duties (Delibes et al. 2012). Among the myriad ideas inaugurat- Although male infanticide clearly has no uni- ing in the 1970s, the hypothesis tary explanation, the biological relevance of in- that infanticide is a male strategy that improves trasexual selection in males makes the sexual reproductive success at the expense of female selection hypothesis a good starting point for fitness (Hrdy 1974) constituted one of the first discussion. As originally formulated for langurs demonstrations of the “battle of the ” the- (Semnopithecus entellus), Hrdy’s (1974, 1977) ory developed by Williams (1966) and Trivers argument is that the killing of an infant pre- (1972). Partly because of the controversy sur- maturely ends lactational amenorrhea (the ces- rounding the appearance of this hypothesis sation of ovulatory cycles during nursing) in (Rees 2009), however, subsequent research fo- its , thereby significantly advancing the cused more on male strategy than on the other time when she is available for subsequent fer- party in this sexual dialectic, the female. Thus, tilization by the perpetrator. The potential ben- field and laboratory research has helped to es- efit of this strategy to male fitness will depend tablish its many forms and conditional occur- on many variables, but salient among them is rence, describe its distribution across taxa, and the degree to which sexual access to fecund fe- clarify its adaptive significance, but it is only males is limited by two factors: male–male relatively recently that female counterstrategies competition and the reproductive life history have become the subjects of rigorous study, even of females. though their potential importance was grasped The latter of these factors provides a useful early on (Hrdy 1979). context for analyzing the problem of male in- In this article, I review selected aspects of fanticide. The L/G ratio, which compares the this body of data and analysis. My focus is durations of and gestation, summariz- on nonparental male infanticide targeting de- es the potential value (to males) and vulnera- pendent young—in mammals, nursing individ- bility (to females) of infanticide in mammals. uals—as opposed to older, weaned offspring, When lactation greatly exceeds gestation, post- the killing of which is variably rendered “juve- partum mating and early pregnancy impose nilicide,” “pedicide,” or “filicide” (e.g., Agora- potentially prohibitive costs on females in the moorthy and Mohnot 1988; Palombit 2014, in form of simultaneous support of two sets of press). offspring differing in ages, requirements, and competitive capacities (van Schaik 2000b). The adaptive solution to this female problem is lactational amenorrhea, which, however, gen- THE OF INFANTICIDE: erates the reproductive problem for males that MALE STRATEGIES infanticide can solve. Plainly expressed: An in- Several functional hypotheses have been pro- fant is a “perfect contraceptive,” and infanticide posed to explain male infanticide. Foremost simply serves to remove this effect (Altmann among them historically is the sexual selection et al. 1978, p. 1029). hypothesis. I consider this hypothesis first, with The taxonomic distribution of sexually se- special, although not exclusive, taxonomic fo- lected infanticide across mammals is not fully cus on the group of animals that originally gen- resolved, but current reports of infanticide erated the hypothesis—the primates. I then whose patterning is consistent with this hy- move on to several alternative hypotheses for pothesis are predictably concentrated among male infanticide, the discussion of which will taxa with relatively long L/G ratios— namely, incorporate consideration of data from other primates, fissiped carnivores, and odontocete

2 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

whales (Fig. 1) (van Schaik 2000b). Several ca- Schaik and Janson 2000; Ebensperger and veats warrant mention. First, this result does Blumstein 2007; Palombit 2012), but some not mean that male infanticide is uniformly general patterns can be highlighted. The largest expressed within these taxa; on the contrary, body of data supporting this hypothesis has there is often considerable variation over time come from study of the nonhuman primates. and across populations (see below). Second, it is In the last 25 years, the number of species in also not the case that male infanticide is un- which infanticide had been observed directly reported in other taxa with lower L/Gratios, or inferred from substantive indirect evidence but in those cases the sexually selected mecha- in the wild has risen from a dozen (Hiraiwa- nism has been less implicated than alternative Hasegawa 1988) to 54, including 84 distinct hypotheses (see below). Finally, L/G ratios are populations distributed across nine of the 14 important, but other factors mayexplain the rar- families of strepsirrhines, monkeys, and apes ity of infanticide in some mammals, for exam- (updated from Palombit 2012). ple, ecologically determined sexual segregation, The circumstances surrounding infanticide (KunzandEbensperger1999),caching ofinfants in primates are typically (although not invari- as an antipredator strategy (Schu¨lke 2005), need ably) consistent with the sexual selection hy- for hibernation (Thalmann 2001), or even phy- pothesis: (1) replacement of a male occupying logenetic inertia (Ebensperger 2001). the position of sole or dominant breeder in the It is not possible here to review comprehen- group by a male who is unlikely to have sired the sively the empirical tests of the sexual selec- infants he subsequently attacks; (2) resumption tion hypothesis (see Hausfater and Hrdy 1984; of ovulatory cycling by the mother; and (3) Parmigiani et al. 1994; Connor et al. 2000; van between the mother and perpetra-

I Primates* (N = 60)

I Fissiped carnivores* (N = 77)

I Odontocete cetaceans* (N = 12)

I? Sciurognath rodents (N = 192)

I? Perissodactyls (N = 8)

Insectivores* (N = 26)

Lagomorphs* (N = 16)

Artiodactyls* (N = 66)

Mysticete cetaceans* (N = 7)

Macroscelids† (N = 3)

Hystricognath rodents* (N = 24)

Edentates* (N = 7)

Pinniped carnivores* (N = 16)

–20–1.5 –1 –0.5 0.5 Mean Ln (L/G)

Figure 1. Mean lactation/gestation (L/G) ratios for selected mammalian groups. Log-transformed L/G ratios significantly different from 1 are indicted by Ã( p , 0.05) or †( p , 0.01). The letter “I” denotes taxa in which deliberate sexually selected infanticide by males has been recorded. Sample sizes (number of species) are indicated in parentheses. (Modified from data in van Schaik 2000b.)

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 3 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

tor, and thus shortening of the interbirth inter- in a manner reminiscent of (Pusey and val. Most but not all reports provide supportive Packer 1994b). One factor apparently promot- data for all three predictions, but cases in which ing infanticide in this social context is pro- behavioral data are unavailable or contradic- nounced mating skew toward dominant males tory more often concern the third prediction. (sensu Broom et al. 2004). Advances in field methodology have permitted Compelling evidence for sexually selected collection of ancillary (nonbehavioral) data to infanticide in the nonprimate mammals has further test the hypothesis. For example, DNA come from field studies of fissiped carnivores data establish that males are unrelated to the (particularly and well-studied lions); infants they attack (Pereira and Weiss1991; Bor- there are also some suggestive recent data for ries et al. 1999a; Soltis et al. 2000) or are fathers toothed whales (particularly ) (Table of the female’s next infant (Morelli et al. 2009), 1). Among rodents, the of young and whereas hormonal data provide direct evidence lengthy periods of female investment make sex- of maternal resumption of ovulatory cycling ually selected infanticide likely in many species, following infanticide (Harris and Monfort particularly in the murids (Wolff and MacDon- 2003) as well as elevated stress response in ald 2004). Male infanticide is known in 14–17 following heightened infanticide risk species of Muridae (Ebensperger and Blumstein or actual attacks (Beehner et al. 2005; see also 2007). The Norway rat (Rattus norvegicus), Brockman et al. 2009). Thus, as a general expla- house (Mus musculus/domesticus), and nation of infanticide, the sexual selection hy- (Meriones unguiculatus) have potheses has received more support than alter- been the subjects of extensive and well-con- native hypotheses (see below) (van Schaik trolled laboratory research (see references in Ta- 2000a; Palombit 2012). ble 1), which has shed important light on the Although originally observed for primates proximate mechanisms underlying infanticide living in unimale, polygynous groups, male in- and on female counterstrategies. These data for fanticide has been observed in almost all of the murids are largely consistent with the sexual other social systems found in this order, from selection hypothesis, but almost all of them dispersed societies, to multimale groups, to fis- come from the laboratory; only four murid spe- sion–fusion communities, and perhaps even cies have provided limited data on male infan- socially monogamous pairs. Its occurrence in ticide in the wild (see Caley and Boutin 1985; some multimale societies is noteworthy. The Wolff and Cicirello 1991; Ylo¨nen et al. 1997). It permanent presence of multiple males was ex- is partly because “virtually nothing is known” pected to hinder infanticide by increasing its about infanticide in natural populations of mu- costs (through male defense of infants) or de- rids (Berdoy and Drickamer 2007, p. 389) that creasing its potential benefits (through reduced the broader implications of the laboratory ability to monopolize matings). Thus, across results are debated, particularly vis-a`-vis the gregarious primates, infanticide rate is nega- predicted postinfanticide reproductive benefits. tively correlated with the number of males in Thus, Ebensperger and Blumstein (2007, groups (Janson and van Schaik 2000). Never- p. 178) conclude that although current data re- theless, significant male infanticide has been ject nonadaptive explanations, the sexual selec- documented in a number of multimale societies tion hypothesis “remains unproven in rodents” in two general contexts: (1) by new immigrants (see also below for sciurid rodents). (or, somewhat less commonly, by long-term residents) that rise to the alpha position in the Alternative Explanations for Male Infanticide male (e.g., Palombit et al. 2000; Soltis et al. 2000; Teichroeb and Sicotte Besides the sexual selection hypothesis, there is a 2008); or (2) by coalitionary cohorts of alien large number of alternative adaptive explana- males that collectively replace the entire male tions for male infanticide (Table 1). Only a membership of a group (Fedigan et al. 2008) few of these can be addressed here. It is perhaps

4 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Table 1. Adaptive and nonadaptive hypotheses for nonparental infanticide by males Hypothesis Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Benefits related to mating Sexual selection When sexual access to Arachnids; birds (Hirundo Crook and Shields 1985; I. Accelerate female fecund females is limited rustica, Passer domesticus); Veiga 1993; Pusey and return to breeding by intrasexual hippos (Hippopotamus Packer 1994b; condition competition, killing amphibius); horses (Equus Schneider and Lubin or infants accelerates caballus); dolphins 1997; Lewinson 1998; female return to (Tursiops spp.); lions Patterson et al. 1998; fertilizable condition (Panthera leo); bears (Ursus Swenson et al. 2001; (mammals: shortens arctos); murid rodents Dunn et al. 2002; Erez lactational amenorrhea) (M. musculus, et al. 2005; McLellan for subsequent R. norvegicus, Peromyscus 2005; Bellemain et al. insemination spp., M. unguiculatus, 2006a; Solomon and Myodes glareolus); tree Keane 2007; Dudzinski (Paraxerus et al. 2009; Gray 2009; cepapi); relatively many Zedrosser et al. 2009; species see references in Ebensperger and Blumstein 2007 and Palombit 2012 II. Improved female Premature termination of Arctic ground squirrels McLean 1983; Bartosˇ and condition and lactational amenorrhea (Spermophilus parryii); red Madlafousek 1994; future fertility improves female deer (Cervus elaphus); Wright 1995; Jolly et al. condition and thereby horses (E. caballus, E. ferus 2000; Cameron et al. enhances future przewalskii); ring-tailed 2003; Hoesli et al. 2009; reproductive success ( catta); Morelli et al. 2009 following insemination sifakas (Propithecus by infanticidal male edwardsi) Breeding social status Infanticide facilitates Howler monkeys (Alouatta Angst and Thommen acquisition acquisition of high rank arctoides); baboons (Papio 1977; Crockett 2003; or breeding status in hamadryas hamadryas); Rajpurohit et al. 2008 multimale groups for Hanuman langurs resident males or new (S. entellus) immigrants Breeding site Infanticide is part of a Burying beetles (Nicrophorus Freed 1986, 1987; acquisition strategy to take over a spp.); fish (Lamprologus Robertson and breeding site by callipterus); birds Stutchbury 1988; destroying existing (Tachycineta bicolor, Trumbo 1990a; offspring, and then Troglodytes aedon) Trumbo and Valletta mating with the resident 2007; Maan and female(s) or new Taborsky 2008 immigrant female(s) Mate acquisition Direct Infanticide by an extragroup Birds (H. rustica); colobine Crook and Shields 1985; male promotes female monkeys (Presbytis Watts 1989; Steenbeek transfer to the killer thomasi, Colobus 2000; Møller 2004; partly by advertising his polykomos); mountain Korstjens et al. 2005 superior ability to protect ( gorilla) against infanticide Continued

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 5 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Table 1. Continued Hypothesis Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Indirect In species with female Chimpanzees (Pan Goodall 1986 dispersal, infanticide by troglodytes) an extragroup male disrupts the mother– subadult daughter bond, facilitating the dispersal of latter to infanticidal male Sexual Infanticide induces female Chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) Hamai et al. 1992 monopolization to restrict future of female copulations to infanticidal males(s) than to other male(s) Removal of mating Selective killing of male Arctic ground squirrels McLean 1983; Sommer competitors infants: (a) eliminates (S. parryii); Hanuman 1994; Teichroeb and possible future sexual langurs (S. entellus); white- Sicotte 2008 competitors of thighed colobus monkeys infanticidal males or their (Colobus vellerosus) sons; (b) undermines coalitionary power of rival males? Decrease Infanticide removes genes of Tree (P. cepapi); fur de Villiers 1986; Enstam reproductive rival male(s) seals (Callorhinus ursinus); et al. 2002; Kiyota and success of rivals patas monkey Okamura 2005 (Erythrocebus patas) Benefits related to resources Removal of Infanticide improves access Gerbils (M. unguiculatus); Rudran 1973b; Clarke competitors for to limiting food resources purple-faced langurs 1983; Elwood and limiting resources to infanticidal male and/ (Semnopithecus vetulus); Ostermeyer 1984b; or his offspring howler monkeys (Alouatta Agoramoorthy and arctoides, A. palliata) Rudran 1995 Improved intergroup Killing of infants in (a) Chimpanzees (a) Watts et al. 2002; competition for neighboring groups (P. troglodytes); (b) guereza Williams et al. 2002, limited resources enhances access to (Colobus guereza) 2004; Mitani et al. 2010; resources by: (a) causing (b) Harris and Monfort their mothers to 2003 withdraw permanently from borders areas, thereby facilitating killers’ home range to expand; (b) facilitating intergroup dominance and thereby priority of access of resources in border areas Nutritional Infants are cannibalized to (Telmatherina Paul and Kupferschmidt exploitation obtain direct nutritional sarasinorum); yellow 1975; Svare and Bartke benefits sciurid rodents 1978; Sherman 1981; (Spermophilus beldingi, Elwood and Continued

6 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Table 1. Continued Hypothesis Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References S. tridecemlineatus, Ostermeyer 1984a; Cynomys parvidens); murid Vestal 1991; rodents (R. norvegicus, Ebensperger et al. 2000; M. musculus, Hoogland 2001; M. unguiculatus, Microtus Altmann et al. 2006; pennsylvanicus); baboons Gray et al. 2007 (Papio cynocephalus) Benefits related to paternal investment Avoidance of Infanticide prevents male Birds (T.aedon, P.domesticus); Daly and Wilson 1984, misdirected parental investment in (Homo sapiens) 1994; Rohwer 1986; paternal offspring sired by another Freed 1987; Pierotti investment male 1991; Veiga 1993 Benefits related to Acquisition of Inexperienced subadult Otariid seals (Mirounga spp., Le Boeuf and Campagna mating tactics and males lethally use pups as Callorhinus ursinus) 1994; Kiyota and skills proxies for adult females Okamura 2005 to acquire skill in mating tactics (e.g., female stealing and herding, maintenance) Nonadaptive hypotheses Social pathology Lethal attacks on infants are: Horses (E. ferus przewalskii); Jansen et al. 2002; (a) (a) abnormal behavior episodic in others (e.g., Dolhinow 1977; Curtin resulting from human- Hanuman langurs) and Dolhinow 1978; induced disturbance (S. entellus); Tibetan Feh and Munkhtuya (e.g., overcrowding) or macaques (Macaca 2008; (b) Boggess 1984; selection; (b) results of thibetana); chimpanzees Berman et al. 2007; changes in nature/rate of (P. troglodytes) Williams et al. 2008 aggression (e.g., takeover, intergroup hostility) caused by anthropogenic disturbance and habitat degradation Generalized Infant killing is an Otariid seals (Mirounga spp., Galat-Luong and Galat aggression incidental byproduct of Otaria byronia); horses 1979; Rijksen 1981; selection for overall intra- (E. ferus przewalskii); Goodall 1990; Bartlett or intersexual episodic in others (e.g., et al. 1993; Le Boeuf aggressiveness in males or baboons) (Papio and Campagna 1994; (for intergroup h. hamadryas); macaques Singh et al. 2006; Hoesli infanticide) of general (Macaca radiata); mona et al. 2009 male xenophobic monkey (Cercopithecus territoriality campbellii); chimpanzees (P. troglodytes)

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 7 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

unsurprising that the two taxa in Figure 1 that os- almost all of the sciurid data are from the wild, tensibly straddle the “high L/G” and “low L/G” particularly from studies of prairie dogs (Cyn- groupings have generated conflicting results and omys spp.), ground squirrels (Spermophilus debate about sexually selected infanticide. These spp.), and marmots (Marmota spp.). With few are the sciurid rodents and the perissodactyls. exceptions (e.g., nonseasonally breeding tree Among , the perissodactyls—par- squirrels) (Solomon and Keane 2007), these nat- ticularly equids—show the highest incidence of uralistic data are generally less consistent with the male infanticide (Pluha´cˇek and Bartosˇ 2005). sexual selection hypothesis than with alternative The majority of reports have come from cap- explanations (e.g., nutritional exploitation) (see tivity, however, which has lead to suggestions Table 1). Indeed, Hoogland (2007) has argued that the behavior is an artifact of captivity or that his large data set on wild black-tailed prairie domestication (Table1). Moreover, there is little dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus) and Utah prairie unambiguous evidence from either the captive dogs (C. parvidens) does not clearly support any or few wild studies that infanticide shortens in- of the existing hypotheses for male infanticide. terbirth intervals in females, as predicted by Perhaps Blumstein’s (2000, p. 195) appeal of the sexual selection hypothesis (e.g., Feh and more than a decade ago—“we simply need better Munkhtuya 2008). If there is an adaptive basis data”—remainsthe most compelling conclusion. to the behavior, a variant of this hypothesis may Another functional hypothesis for male in- be more relevant. This alternative hypothesis fanticide has arisen from avian research. Sexu- arose originally from an appreciation that the ally selected infanticide has been suggested for sexually selected mechanism is unlikely to op- at least 13 species of birds (Veiga 2000). It has erate in seasonal breeders, because infanticide been reported in polygynous species (as cannot easily accelerate female return to fertil- expected theoretically), but notably also in so- izable condition when mating is determined cially monogamous breeders as well, where primarily by photoperiodicity or other cyclical more often than advancing the female’s next cues. However, premature termination of lacta- fecund period, infanticide functions as part of tion can potentially benefit males in seasonal a takeover strategy for achieving breeding space breeders if it improves the female condition and/or acquiring a mate in the first place. This and thus her future reproductive success (Hrdy role of infanticide has also been suggested for and Hausfater 1984; Hoogland 1985). Bartosˇ several species of mammals (see Table 1). and Madlafousek’s (1994) experimental study Perhaps no single species has yielded more of red deer (Cervus elaphus) was the first to pro- proposed explanations for male infanticide vide some empirical support for this hypothe- than the chimpanzee (P. troglodytes) (Table 1). sis, which has been invoked for other mammals, Much about infanticide in this ape has long including some primates, rodents, and equids been puzzling (Arcadi and Wrangham 1999). (see Sexual selection II in Table1). Another con- This is particularly true of the violent attacks text in which sexually selected infanticide may on the infants of neighboring groups, which operate among seasonal breeders is, of course, have seemed to produce no obvious mating- when interbirth intervals exceed 1 yr, as in lan- or resource-related benefits for the male perpe- gurs living at high elevations (Borries 1997). trators. Intriguing new data, however, suggest a Under this scenario, infanticide can accelerate “range expansion” function, whereby infanti- the return of females to fertilizable condition, cide induces victimized mothers to abandon and thereby enhance the reproductive success their home ranges to the killers, which in one of a male (assuming he retains breeding status Ugandan community facilitated a 22% increase in the subsequent reproductive season). in their territory size (Mitani et al. 2010). Among the Sciuridae, male infanticide is Finally, two hypotheses explain infanti- known to occur in 11–14 species (Ebensperger cide as maladaptive behavior or accidental by- and Blumstein 2007). In stark contrast to the product (Table 1). Inspired partly by laboratory mostly laboratory-based research on murids, demonstrations of infanticide arising from

8 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

debilitating overcrowding (e.g., Myers and Poole occurrence may vary significantly across popu- 1961; Calhoun 1962), the social pathology lations of the same species. Forexample, in chac- hypothesis links the behavior in the wild to an- ma baboons of southern Africa (Papio hamadry- thropogenic disturbance of either a direct (e.g., as ursinus), male infanticide accounts for an provisioning) or indirect (e.g., habitat degra- average of 38% of infant mortality, but among dation) nature. Although this explanation may east African yellow (P.h. cynocephalus) and olive apply to some individual cases (especially in baboons (P. h. anubis), it is not only much less captivity), it has largely failed as a general expla- demographically significant but less clearly a nation, partly because the prediction that “the male reproductive strategy (Palombit 2003; for infanticidal individual is behaving in a manner other examples, see also Butynski 1990; Arcadi that indicates malfunction” (Pierotti 1991, and Wrangham 1999). Even in populations with p. 1141) has not been consistently supported. relatively high rates of infanticide, it is not in- Sterck (1998) argues that rather than produce variably expressed by all males experiencing the infanticide by disrupting “normal” regulatory putative causal conditions. This has led some pathways of behavior, local human disturbance researchers to propose that infanticide is a fac- often alters rates of infanticide through its ef- ultative strategy (a behavioral polymorphism?) fects on important causal variables and condi- (e.g., McLean 1983; Palombit et al. 2000; Cords tions, such as the occurrence of male takeovers. and Fuller 2010), as also suggested by theoretical Some anthropogenic intervention has also pro- models (e.g., Chapman and Hausfater 1979; vided difficult-to-obtain “experimental” sup- Glass et al. 1985; Yamamura et al. 1990; Broom port of the sexual selection hypothesis (e.g., et al. 2004). Consequently, rates of occurrence in the selective removal of males through trophy an “infanticidal” population may vary consid- hunting in fissiped carnivores) (e.g., Wielgus erably over time. For example, although male and Bunnell 2000; Loveridge et al. 2007; Wielgus infanticide accounted for an average of 38% of et al. 2013; but see Miller et al. 2003). infant mortality in the previously mentioned The second nonadaptive hypothesis, the chacma baboon population, this figure ranged “generalized aggression” hypothesis, suggests from nearly 0% to as high as 75% in certainyears that infanticide arises accidentally as a byprod- (Cheneyet al. 2004). The reasons for variation in uct of the intra- or intersexual aggression sur- infanticide are not well understood, but one rel- rounding male mating competition (Bartlett evant variable for interpopulation differences in et al. 1993). Again, although some individual primates as well as carnivores isthe rate of breed- episodes are consistent with this model (Table ing male turnover (Packer et al. 1988; Sterck 1), it has not proved an adequate general expla- 1998; Crockett and Janson 2000). nation of larger data sets, which typically estab- Long-term studies of populations in which lish the highly “directed” nature of infanticidal male infanticide occurs have generally suggested attacks and their predictable long-term pattern- it is a major—if not the major—source of in- ing. One notable exception seems to be the well- fant mortality. Among Serengeti lions (P. leo), studied otariid seals, in which evidence suggests it accounts for 27% of cub mortality overall, thatinfants(andjuveniles)aretrampledtodeath but nearly 100% of the mortality of cubs under accidentallybyhugemales(althougheveninthis the age of 9 mo (Pusey and Packer 1994b). In a case, adaptive hypotheses have been proffered sample of 29 primate populations in which but not tested) (see Table 1). infanticide is known, 19 had relative rates 30% (Palombit 2012). In some of these taxa, male replacement produces a 3-, 11-, or 32-fold The Occurrence of Male Infanticide as increase in infant mortality (Beehner and Berg- a Potential Selective Agent for Female man 2008; Fedigan et al. 2008; Pave´ et al. 2012). Counterstrategies In sciurid rodents, male (as well as female) in- A defining feature of infanticide is its variability, fanticide is a major cause of infant and juvenile expressed across taxa and over time. Rates of mortality (Ebensperger and Blumstein 2007).

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 9 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

In total, the empirical evidence for adap- compared to 11% of nonvulnerable species (van tive strategies of male infanticide provides a Noordwijk and van Schaik 2000). Recent data compelling rationale for evaluating its possible have even suggested a heretofore unappreciated importance as a selective agent on the evolution potential for this benefit to accrue to females of female biology. living in unimale groups, through copulations with extragroup males (Palombit 2012). Simi- larly, Wolff and MacDonald (2004) argue on THE SEXUAL CONFLICT OF INFANTICIDE: the basis of their correlative analysis that multi- FEMALE COUNTERSTRATEGIES male mating in rodents is best understood as Avery large number of possible female counter- anti-infanticidal confusion of paternity, but it strategies to male infanticide have been pro- is important to remember that multimale mat- posed and studied to varying degrees. These ing has many other possible functions which can be grouped into three broad categories ad- are implicated in numerous species (e.g., Bryja dressing: (1) sexual behavior and reproduction; et al. 2008; Lane et al. 2008). Notably, Hoogland (2) individual prevention and deterrence; and (2007) rejects this hypothesis for Utah prairie (3) social counterstrategies. Space limitations dogs based on observations of males attacking preclude discussion of all of these phenomena, some of the pups of females with whom they and so I focus on some of the major empirically previously mated. However, students of species supported patterns. in which females produce multiply sired litters have speculated that males may be able to rec- ognize kin phenotypically and then selectively Sexual and Reproductive Counterstrategies target unrelated infants in a litter (e.g., Vestal of Females 1991; Berdoy and Drickamer 2007). If true, Among the numerous potential counterstrate- this outcome would imply a possible male gies related to sex and reproduction (Table 2), counterstrategy to counteract the female anti- arguably the best known is “confusing paterni- infanticide counterstrategy of ty” through copulation with males (Hrdy (Table 2). 1979). Direct tests of this hypothesis are limited A large number of laboratory tests of this primarily to laboratory murids (see below), but hypothesis in murid rodents suggest that copu- field data have generally suggested that infanti- lation inhibits subsequent infanticidal behav- cide is perpetrated by males who have no sexual ior of males in at least seven species (reviewed history with the victimized infants’ mothers. In in Ebensperger and Blumstein 2007). These re- one of the few direct tests in primates, DNA sults have not been replicated in several studies, paternity data showed that male attacks on in- however, which sometimes indicate the impor- fants were directed at eight times the rate toward tance of not just copulation but a period of unrelated individuals than toward offspring postcopulatory cohabitation in facilitating inhi- (Soltis et al. 2000). bition of infanticidal tendencies (e.g., Elwood A logical extension of this hypothesis is and Kennedy 1994). that females vulnerable to infanticide will pur- More intriguingly, a series of experiments sue a strategy of “promiscuous” or “multimale” on the house mouse suggest that this female mating, which Hrdy (1979, 1981) invoked to counterstrategy of promiscuity may have itself explain the pronounced sexual assertiveness of selected for additional male counterstrategies. haplorrhine primates. Broad comparative anal- Copulation is known to inhibit infanticide yses provide indirect support for this hypo- in males (vom Saal and Howard 1982). That in- thesis. For example, in eutherian mammals, hibition, however, does not commence until multimale mating is more pronounced in spe- about 12 d after the sexual encounter, and it cies characterized by male infanticide; among terminates spontaneously 60 d later, a schedule primates alone, 62% of species with female that corresponds roughly to the periods of ges- susceptibility to infanticide show promiscuity, tation and lactation, respectively (vom Saal

10 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Table 2. Sexual and reproductive counterstrategies of females with selected example taxa Proposed counterstrategy Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Strategies obscuring paternity among males Promiscuity/ Elevated sexual receptivity Birds (Tachycineta bicolor); Bertram 1975; Robertson multimale mating and proceptivity and/or murid rodents (Mus 1990; Connor et al. polyestrous cycling musculus); prairie dogs 1996; 2000; Dahle and promotes insemination (Cynomys parvidens); bears Swenson 2003; by multiple males, (Ursus arctos); lions Ebensperger and thereby confusing (Panthera leo); badgers Blumstein 2007; paternity (Meles meles); dolphins Hoogland 2007; (Tursiops spp.); primates Dugdale et al. 2011; see references in Palombit 2012 Multiply sired litters In polytocous breeders, Mice (M. musculus); voles Bellemain et al. 2006a; females achieve (Myodes rufocanus); bears Ishibashi and Saitoh fertilizations by multiple (U. arctos) 2008; Thonhauser et al. males in single litters 2013 Extended estrus Lengthened periods of Dolphins (Tursiops); Connor et al. 1996; van period estrus or polyestrous catarrhine primates with Schaik et al. 2000; cycling increase sexual swellings, langurs Heistermann et al. 2001 opportunities for (S. entellus) copulation with multiple males Concealed Paternity is obscured by Hanuman langurs Andelman 1987; absence of reliable signals (S. entellus); vervet Heistermann et al. of ovulation and/or by monkeys (Chlorocebus 2001; Carnegie et al. variability in the timing pygerythrus); capuchin 2006 of ovulation monkeys (Cebus capucinus) Postconception Anovulatory copulations Many catarrhine primates, See references in Palombit sexual behavior (and when relevant, some platyrrhine primates 2012 signaling of “ovulation”) occur regularly in early gestation Situation-dependent Exposure to new (or (a) Lemurs (Propithecus (a) Struhsaker and Leland sexual receptivity extragroup) males verreauxi); capuchin 1985; Fairgrieve 1995; (“pseudoestrus”) facultatively causes (a) monkeys (Cebus apella); Brockman and Whitten “deceptive” anovulatory blue monkeys 1996; Zinner and sexual behavior in (Cercopithecus mitis); Deschner 2000; Pazol pregnant females; or (b) baboons (Papio hamadryas 2003; Ramı´rez-Llorens longer receptive periods hamadryas); et al. 2008; (b) Cords among cycling females (Procolobus badius); (b) 1984; Takahata et al. cercopithecine monkeys 1994; Pazol 2003 (C. mitis, Macaca fuscata) Strategies focusing copulations and/or conceptions on particular male(s) Female Females mate preferentially Murid rodents (Lemmus, O’Brien 1991; Agrell et al. targeting with males most likely to Microtus, Mus, Myodes); 1998; Bellemain et al. infanticidal commit infanticide bears (U. arctos); capuchin 2006b; Izar et al. 2009 male(s) because of factors such as monkeys (C. nigritus, dominance status or C. olivaceous) spatial proximity Continued

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 11 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Table 2. Continued Proposed counterstrategy Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Female mate choice Females limit periovulatory Hamadryas baboons Swedell and Saunders targeting protector matings to a male likely (P. h. hamadryas) 2006 male(s) (direct) to be a future protector against infanticide Female mate choice Females incite male mate Catarrhine primates living in See references in Palombit targeting protector guarding by advertising multimale societies 2012 male(s) (indirect) ovulation or copulations (e.g., through sexual swellings, vocalizations), which reduces polyandry at ovulation and thereby concentrates paternity in future, high-quality male defender of infant Strategies manipulating reproduction Pregnancy block and Direct or indirect exposure Equids (E. burchelli, Berger 1983; Pereira 1983; pregnancy to a new replacement E. caballus); baboons Agoramoorthy et al. termination (Bruce male results in failure of (P. h. hamadryas, 1988; Colmenares and effect) implantation or P. h. cynocephalus); langurs Gomendio 1988; absorption of fetus (S. entellus); geladas Pluha´cˇek and Bartosˇ (Theropithecus gelada) 2000; Bartosˇ et al. 2011; Roberts et al. 2012 Superfetation Conception occurs during European badger (Meles Yamaguchi et al. 2006 gestation accompanied meles) by fertilized eggs entering embryonic diapause Abandonment of Mothers abandon a healthy Colobine monkeys (Colobus Hrdy 1977; Sicotte et al. infant infant or an infant vellerosus, S. entellus) 2007 injured in infanticidal attack Acceleration of Following male takeover, Vervet monkeys (Chlorocebus Fairbanks and McGuire weaning mothers wean older pygerythrus); baboons 1987; Colmenares and infants sooner than usual (P. h. hamadryas) Gomendio 1988 Temporarily reduced Following male takeover, Lions (Panther leo); leaf Pusey and Packer 1994b; fecundity females temporarily monkeys (P. thomasi) Steenbeek 1999b suspend ovulatory cycles until new male(s) are established and unlikely to be replaced again Breeding synchrony Among group-living Lions (P. leo); lemurs Bertram 1975; Cords females, greater ovulatory (Eulemur fulvus rufus); 1986; Altmann 1990; synchronization prevents baboons (Papio spp.); blue Ostner and Kappeler single males from monkeys (Cercopithecus 2004 monopolizing matings, mitis) generating multimale groups that are generally less vulnerable to takeover infanticide

12 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

1985; Perrigo et al. 1990, 1992). This timing sug- lation) and promiscuously at other times in gests that the female (sexual) counterstrategy their cycle (when ovulation is less likely to oc- has, in turn, selected for counteradaptations cur) (reviewed Palombit 2012). Moreover, in in males that “fine-tune” the expression of in- catarrhine primates exhibiting sexual swellings, fanticidal inhibition to periods when pups are the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle— most likely to be their genetic offspring, and during which follicles mature in the ovary and that thereby avoid the “evolutionary mistake” ending with ovulation—is significantly longer of failing to attack unrelated pups. A further in species in which females are subject to higher step in this coevolutionary scenario is suggested risk of male infanticide (van Schaik et al. 2000). by the fact that aggressive mothers protecting One interpretation of this pattern is that longer their pups attack males who have copulated phases provide greater opportunities for mat- with them very recently (within the last 24 h) ings to confuse paternity. with the same severity they direct toward males A recently suggested angle on this coevolu- who have not copulated with them at all (Par- tionary dialectic is that males may themselves migiani et al. 1988b). Taken together, these pat- exploit this female counterstrategy to protect terns imply the spiraling, antagonistic dynamic their offspring. Evidence from chacma baboons characterizing sexual conflict. suggests that alpha males may “cede” a pro- Recognition of the potential importance of portion of mating opportunities to other male protectors in deterring infanticide in some males and thereby reduce the risk of infanticidal species (see below) has led to modification of attack (Boyko and Marshall 2009; Henzi et al. this hypothesized counterstrategy of paternity 2010). confusion. Under these conditions, females may benefit most from pursuing a dual sexual Individual Counterstrategies of Females strategy that concentrates paternity in the best available male defender and also dilutes (con- A number of possible deterrent counterstrate- fuses) it in other males (e.g., Clarke et al. 2009). gies are based on individual action (Table 3). Cues of ovulation, such as the sexual swellings Maternal (or postpartum) aggression, the characterizing many species of Old Worldmon- “fierce and persistent” hostility of pregnant or keys and apes, may provide a mechanism for lactating females to intruders is a conspicuous achieving what seem at first glance to be incom- feature of reproductive biology (Lonstein patible reproductive goals. The “graded signal and Gammie 2002, p. 869) and is argued to hypothesis” holds that: (1) the probability of underlie female territoriality in some species ovulation is highest at the time of maximal tu- (Wolff and Peterson 1998; but see Ebensper- mescence, and thus sexual swellings can pro- ger 1998a). Although maternal aggression is a mote fertilization by high-quality (protector) heterogeneous phenomenon subserving multi- males; but (2) the probability of ovulation at ple functions, a link to infanticide prevention other times is inherently variable and nonzero, is currently suggested in at least eight murid and, hence, paternity can be confused among species and possibly several sciurid species (re- other males (Nunn 1999). Hormonal data from viewed Ebensperger and Blumstein 2007; langurs confirmed that the timing of ovulation McGuire and Bemis 2007; Weber and Olsson was sufficiently variable that low-ranking males 2008). Once again, numerous detailed data occasionally achieved fertilizations despite the have emerged from laboratory investigations of dominant male’s sexual monopolization of fe- murids. In species such as the house mouse, the males during periods when ovulation was stat- aggressiveness of mothers is striking in light istically more likely (Heistermann et al. 2001). of the virtual absence of aggression when fe- Further support for this hypothesis is provided males are in other reproductive states (Heiming by observations that female primates of several et al. 2013). Aggression is sensitive to the pres- species mate selectively at periovulatory periods ence of pups (Elwood et al. 1990), increases with (coinciding with increased probability of ovu- litter size (Maestripieri and Alleva 1990; see also

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 13 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Table 3. Individual counterstrategies of females with selected example taxa Proposed counterstrategy Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Maternal Females increase in Birds (H. rustica, Tachycineta Møller 1988; Robertson aggression heterosexual bicolor); many murid 1990; Wolff and Peterson aggressiveness during rodents, gerbils (Gerbilliscus 1998; Ebensperger and lactation and/or directly leucogaster); shrews (Sorex Blumstein 2007; Lo¨tter defend infants from araneus) and Pillay 2012; infanticidal attacks by Oleinchenko 2012 males; females become territorial during periods of offspring rearing Female Female maintains a Gerbils (M. unguiculatus); Pereira and Weiss 1991; dominance relationship of social ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur Elwood and Kennedy dominance over males catta) 1994; Ichino 2005 either during lactation or more permanently Maternal Under conditions of Horses (E. caballus); many Cameron et al. 2003, 2009; protectiveness heightened infanticide primates see references in Palombit risk, mothers increase 2012 visual monitoring of potentially infanticidal male and/or offspring, and restrain and/or maintain proximity to infants more Chemical Pregnant females produce Rats (R. norvegicus) Mennella and Moltz 1988 suppression pheromone that disrupts neurochemical pathways mediating infanticidal behavior in males Sexual Lactating females (a) alter (a) Lions (Panthera pardus); (a) Wrangham and Smuts segregation individual ranging to bears (U. arctos); spider 1980; Packer and Pusey avoid areas with monkeys (Ateles spp.); 1983, 1994b; McComb potentially infanticidal chimpanzees (P. troglodytes); et al. 1993; Sakura 1994; males; or (b) for (b) baboons (P. h. ursinus); Matsumoto-Oda 1999; gregarious females, adjust capuchin monkeys Wielgus and Bunnell intragroup spatial (C. capucinus) 2000; Dahle and Swenson relations to decrease 2003; Ben-David et al. proximity to infanticidal 2004; Aureli et al. 2008; males or reduce (b) Palombit et al. 2001; participation in Crofoot 2007 intergroup encounters

Koskela et al. 2000), and wanes as pups mature they do so in aggressive interactions with (less (Parmigiani et al. 1994). Moreover, in their de- infanticidal) virgin females (Parmigiani et al. fensive responses to the infanticidal threat posed 1988a, 1990). Mothers can also differentiate by sexually naı¨ve males, lactating females direct among males on the basis of infanticidal poten- “severe” bites to vulnerable body parts (such as tial and respond accordingly (Elwood et al. the head and ventrum) at 3–5 times the rate 1990). A possible indication of sexually antago-

14 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

nistic coevolution in this domain is that the Social Counterstrategies of Females magnitudes of male infanticidal behavior and maternal aggression are positively correlated A large set of potential counterstrategies involve with one another in genetic strains of mice (Par- deterrence through cooperation with conspecif- migiani et al. 1999). ics. These can be grouped into two sets of strat- The actual effectiveness of maternal aggres- egies involving social interactions with other sion in deterring male infanticide in rodents is females and with males, respectively (Table 4). debated, however. In the laboratory, male mice Female gregariousness has traditionally retaliate with extreme aggression, particularly been viewed as an adaptive solution to the prob- against females with whom they have not mated lems of and feeding ecology (e.g., (or have mated only recently), often overcoming Alexander 1974; Wrangham 1980), and it is maternal defense and successfully committing only recently that the selective role of infanticide infanticide, leading Parmigiani et al. (1989) to risk has begun to be formally incorporated into conclude that maternal aggression may simply these models (e.g., Sterck et al. 1997). There are “delay,”rather than prevent, infanticide (see also several possible mechanisms through which Ebensperger 1998b). Ebensperger and Blum- group living potentially reduces infanticide stein (2007), however, suggest that under more risk: dilution effects (sensu Hamilton 1971), naturalistic conditions, such a delay would early detection through communal vigilance probably often translate into maternal success. and alarm-based avoidance of infanticidal On the other hand, these researchers point out males; female–female coalitionary aggression that ineffectiveness does seem to characterize to extragroup males to prevent takeover or to maternal aggression in several species of squir- resident males to thwart attacks on infants. As rel, and they explicitly raise the possibility that with ecological models, related females may be this may reflect different outcomes or phases in more likely to join together in this manner (e.g., sexually antagonistic coevolution. lions, see below), but aggregations of unrelated Similarly, primate mothers mount concert- females to reduce infanticidal threat have also ed and protracted defense of their infants been suggested (e.g., Maestripieri and Rossi-Ar- against infanticidal males, but once again evi- naud 1991; Cameron et al. 2009). dence suggests that such efforts succeed in only Although female gregariousness has been a handful of species (Palombit 2012), some of invoked as a counterstrategy for a number of which are characterized by the relatively rare species (Table 4), direct evidence for the pre- social arrangement of stable female dominance dicted inhibitory effect on infanticide is still rel- over males (which also applies to some rodent atively rare. In a study of a seminatural pop- examples, Elwood and Kennedy 1994). The rea- ulation of house mice, Manning et al. (1995) sons for this general ineffectiveness remain un- reported that female communal nests experi- clear, but they may partly reflect a greater reli- enced less than half the rate of infanticide (by ance of female primates on socially based both sexes) than did single-mother nests. A pos- counterstrategies (see below). sible mechanism underlying this outcome is A final individual counterstrategy has been suggested by laboratory experiments demon- suggested in several mammalian species charac- strating that paired females effectively repel terized by solitariness or fission–fusion/dis- male (and female) intruders (Parmigiani 1986; persed gregariousness. In these species, lactating Maestripieri and Rossi-Arnaud 1991). Similar- females may pursue a strategy of evasion based ly, when food was experimentally manipulated on sexual segregation (Table 2). There is evi- to promote increased aggregation of female dence that even females in highly gregarious bank voles (Myodes glareolus), pup survival in- species may adopt this counterstrategy on a creased, which Re´my et al. (2013) attributed to smaller spatial scale by adjusting behavior to improved anti-infanticide protection. decrease proximity to potentially infanticidal There are several lines of evidence that males residing in their groups (see Table 2). group living among lionesses (Panthera leo)is

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 15 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Table 4. Social counterstrategies of females with selected example taxa Proposed counterstrategy Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References Social strategies involving other females Female–female (a) Females living in (a) Voles(Microtus oeconomus, (a) McLean 1982; Pusey association permanent groups with Myodes glareolus); house and Packer 1994a,b; other females reduce mice (M. musculus); Arctic Brereton 1995; Manning infanticide risk through ground squirrels et al. 1995; Treves and derive dilution, vigilance, (S. parryii); horses Chapman 1996; Connor and/or defensive (E. caballus); lions (P. leo); 2000; Le Galliard et al. benefits; (b) lactating dolphins (Tursiops spp.); 2006; Cameron et al. members of aggregate langurs (S. entellus); 2009; Re´my et al. 2013; temporarily with other primates generally; (b) (b) Cowlishaw 1999; females baboons (P. hamadryas Palombit et al. 2001 ursinus) Female coalitionary Females collectively attack Lions (Panthera leo); sifakas Butynski 1982; Packer et al. defense infanticidal males and/or (Propithecus edwardsi, 1990; Richard et al. deter male immigration P. verreauxi); chimpanzees 1993; Starin 1994; with relative success (P. troglodytes); bonobo Grinnell and McComb (Pan paniscus); blue 1996; de Waal 1997; monkeys (Cercopithecus Morelli et al. 2009; mitis); red colobus Cords and Fuller 2010 (Procolobus badius); bonobos (P. paniscus) Residency in small If risk of male takeover Lions (P. leo); many species of Pusey and Packer 1987; see (female) groups increases with female colobine monkeys, howler references in Palombit group size, females monkeys, gorillas 2012 maintain small groups via secondary transfer, group fission, and/or aggression to female immigrants Social strategies involving males Transfer with In species in which Capuchin monkeys (Cebus Rudran 1973a; Hrdy 1977; evicted male infanticide is preceded by capucinus); colobine Davies 1987; Jack and male takeover, lactating monkeys (Semnopithecus Fedigan 2009 females temporarily vetulus, S. entellus, Presbytis accompany ousted male rubicunda) until infant is weaned Male–female Females associate with Burying beetles (Nicrophorus McLean 1983; Freed 1986; association individual male spp.); birds (Troglodytes Watts 1989; Trumbo defender(s) as: (a) aedon); Arctic ground 1990b, 2007; Gust 1994; relatively loose squirrels (S. parryii); Pusey and Packer 1994b; association in dispersed muroid rodents; bears Kappeler 1997; Sommer societies; (b) temporary (U. arctos); lions (P. leo); 1997; Agrell et al. 1998; relationship with dolphins (Tursiops spp.); Borries et al. 1999b, particular males in many primates including 2011; Steenbeek 1999a; multimale groups during mouse lemurs Connor et al. 2000; lactation; or (c) (Cheirogaleus major); Crockett and Janson permanent male–female howler monkeys (Alouatta 2000; Delgado and van pairs or unimale groups spp.); mangabeys Schaik 2000; Weingrill Continued

16 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Table 4. Continued Proposed counterstrategy Theoretical rationale Proposed example taxa References (Cercocebus atys, 2000; Stokes 2004; C. galeritus); baboons Bellemain et al. 2006b; (P. hamadryas hamadryas, Swedell and Saunders P. h. ursinus); colobine 2006; Teichroeb and monkeys (Colobus Sicotte 2008; Palombit vellerosus, Procolobus 2009; Yamagiwa et al. badius, P. thomasi, 2009; Fruteau et al. 2010; S. entellus); gibbons Opie et al. 2013 (Hylobates spp.); great apes (Gorilla gorilla subspp., Pongo pygmaeus) Residency in In female-dispersal systems, Lemurs (Eulemur fulvus, Crockett and Janson 2000; multimale females immigrate to P. verreauxi); howler Treves 2001; Isbell et al. groups groups with more males; monkeys (Alouatta 2002; Ostner and in female-philopatric arctoidea, A. caraya, Kappeler 2004; Yamada systems, females promote A. palliata); cercopithecine and Nakamichi 2006; multimale group monkeys (Chlorocebus Lewis 2008; Robbins structure pygerythrus, Macaca et al. 2009a; Pave´ et al. fuscata); gorillas (Gorilla 2012 gorilla beringei) Residency in Primarily among females Colobine monkeys (Colobus Marsh 1979; Starin 1994; groups with living in unimale vellerosus, Procolobus Sterck 1997; Stokes et al. better male societies, cycling females badius, P. rufomitratus, 2003; Robbins et al. defender immigrate to a new group P. thomasi); gorillas (Gorilla 2009b; Teichroeb et al. with a better protector gorilla subspp.) 2009; Yamagiwa et al. male 2009

an effective deterrent to infanticide (Packer and data suggest that pride size does not maximize Pusey 1983). Cub mortality is much lower for individual food intake through communal females living in groups of two or more than hunting, but rather infanticidal protection for solitary mothers (Pusey and Packer 1994b). (Packer et al. 1990). This is partly achieved through coalitionary at- Somewhat surprisingly, female coalitionary tack on alien males, which successfully protects defense against infanticidal males in most pri- cubs from attack by single individuals or small mates appears as generally unsuccessful as indi- groups of males, who “may have to fight for vidual maternal defense (Palombit 2012). It is their lives” against lionesses capable of inflicting possible that the anti-infanticide advantage fatal injuries (Pusey and Packer 1994b, p. 297). conferred by gregariousness involves the hard- The counterstrategic response of males is to use er-to-test mechanism of improved vigilance and vocal cues to avoid groups of females (Grinnell nonaggressive intervention (e.g., retrieving in- and McComb 1996) or to travel in large male fants from dangerous situations before attacks cohorts, against which female defense is less can be launched, as has been noted in a number effective. Like house mouse dams, however, li- of field studies). In some primate species, how- onesses appear capable of discriminating ever, an alternative version of this female–fe- among males on the basis of infanticide threat male counterstrategy may operate. and may use evasion to reduce such interactions This alternative version is based on the idea (McComb et al. 1993). Finally, socioecological that larger groups of females are predicted to be

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 17 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

more attractive as takeover targets to potentially The ecological and social costs of intergroup infanticidal males. Thus, females may theoret- transfer vary considerably for females, but they ically garner increased protection for their in- are generally expected to be higher for frugivo- fants by breeding in relatively small groups rous species than for folivores. This is because (Janson and Goldsmith 1995; Treves and Chap- the greater rarity of fruits (relative to leaves) in man 1996). This goal can be achieved via sev- most environments engenders elevated feed- eral possible mechanisms, such as the fissioning ing competition and greater difficulties in find- of large groups, individual female transfer to ing food in the unfamiliar habitats that a trans- smaller groups, or the aggressive deterrence of ferring female would necessarily encounter. alien female immigration by resident females. Moreover, the social costs of female transfer are Supportive evidence for this counterstrategy potentially higher for frugivores because the has been found in a number of primate species “female-bonded” social organization of many in which: (1) the predicted negative correlation fruit-eating primates (particularly Old World between infant mortality or infanticide rates monkeys) is centered on networks of social rela- and female group size has been documented; tionships among related females. Thus, female and (2) one or more of the above three mech- transfer may necessarily require cessation of co- anisms has been identified (reviewed Palombit operative interactions with female kin, which 2012; see also Pusey and Packer 1987 for lions). can impose costs on both individual and inclu- This counterstrategy carries a potentially im- sive fitness. It is for these reasons that transfer is portant implication that may help to explain less of an option for females in the female-phil- the surprising ineffectiveness of female coali- opatric, multimale societies. Inthis case, ananti- tionary aggression against infanticide in many infanticide counterstrategy based on coopera- primates. The individual female transfer that tion with resident males would be more likely. underlies this counterstrategy in primates tends Onesuch possible counterstrategy isto maintain to produce small groups of females who share a temporary relationship with a male protector relatively low genetic relatedness with one an- (or a small number of adult males) during the other (cf. lions). Such conditions may impede period of infant vulnerability (Table4), as exem- the formation of the kinds of female–female plifiedby thechacmababoonofsouthern Africa. coalitions that are effective deterrents to in- When a female gives , she establishes such a fanticidal males (Palombit 2012). This hypoth- relationship, or “friendship,” with a particular esis has not been tested directly, but it under- male, which lasts as long as she has her nursing scores the possibility that various female infant; if the infant weans or dies, the relation- counterstrategies may be incompatible with shipends. Observational andexperimental play- one another, such that adoption of one may back data suggest that male friends are signifi- preclude another. cantly more predisposed than other males to A final set of counterstrategies has been in- come to the aid of females under attack by the spired by a growing body of data demonstrating potentially infanticidal alpha male (Palombit the important role males may play in preventing et al. 1997), possibly because of paternity of the infanticide. The majority of relevant data on infants involved (Moscovice et al. 2010). Immi- these counterstrategies has come from studies gration of a new male who attains alpha status of nonhuman primates, possibly because other results in elevated cortisol among the group’s animals, such as rodents, are less likely to rely on mothers (but not among cycling females); cor- socially based counterstrategies to infanticide tisol levels climb even higher if an infanticide (Blumstein 2000). There are several variants is successfully perpetrated or, notably, among on this strategic theme, which can be distin- the (very few) mothers who lack a male friend guished by general features of social system, (Beehner et al. 2005; Engh et al. 2006). Lactating such as the feasibility of transfer as a strategic females compete for male friends, such that a option for females at risk of infanticide (re- mother’s rank is positively correlated with the viewed in Palombit 2012). rank of her male friend (Palombit et al. 2001).

18 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

It is partly because the vast majority of mothers act sporadically, female association with a male have male friends that it is difficult to assess may nevertheless afford some protection against directly the fitness benefits of friendship for fe- infanticide (Table 4). For example, ousted male males, and data are still too few to evaluate how lions will re-establish or maintain residency variation in the characteristics of friendships near a pride that offers few mating opportuni- or male friends affect infant survival. Neverthe- ties if it contains vulnerable cubs they likely less, the evidence is suggestive that this system sired (Pusey and Packer 1994b). Male–female reflects an anti-infanticidal counterstrategy. pairs of house mice repulse infanticidal intrud- For folivorous primates, the costs of female ers (Palanza et al. 1996) so successfully that Par- transfer are predicted to be lower, opening the migiani et al. (1994) suggest that the apparent way for dispersal-based counterstrategies, such failure of maternal aggression to thwart infan- as the previously described transfer to smaller ticide in previous laboratory experiments may female groups. Many of these species are char- derive from the artificial absence of the stud acterized by unimale breeding units, which are male. particularly prone to takeover infanticide. Fe- Finally, infanticide has been suggested to males in these societies may reduce the risk of be the primary selective force behind the evolu- infanticide by choosing a group with a high- tion of social monogamy (Table 4) (reviewed in quality male protector. This hypothesis has Palombit 2000). Perhaps the most compelling not been subject to rigorous testing, partly be- evidence for this hypothesis comes from re- cause of the difficulties in measuring variation search on burying beetles (Nicrophorus spp.). in male quality as protector. There are sugges- In a manner reminiscent of avian monogamy, tive supportive data for a few species, however, a male–female pair of beetles maintains a long notably Thomas’s leaf monkeys (P. thomasi), period of postcopulatory association with one in which infanticide is not perpetrated as part another on a vertebrate carcass that provides of a male “takeover” by a new immigrant, but nourishment for developing larvae (Eggert and rather by extragroup males. Victimized females Mu¨ller 1997). Both parents provision the larvae respond to infanticide by abandoning their by regurgitating predigested carrion obtained group and joining the killer. And, as predicted, from this carcass, although the paternal con- cycling females who transfer from older males to tribution is not crucial for offspring success “prime” (younger) males subsequently experi- (Trumbo 1991; Mu¨ller et al. 1998). On the other ence fewer infanticidal attacks. hand, male presence significantly reduces the For relatively folivorous primates in which riskof takeover infanticide byanother male, sug- female transfer remains an ecologically viable gesting an anti-infanticide benefit of social mo- option, but in which the number of males in nogamy in some populations (Trumbo1990a,b, groups varies, an alternative counterstrategy is 1991; Robertson 1993; Trumbo and Valletta to preferentially target groups with more males. 2007). Similarly, in some birds such as tropical This hypothesis is supported in several species, house wrens (Troglodytes aedon), protection such as howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.) and against infanticide appears to be a more impor- mountain gorillas (G. gorilla beringei) in which tant advantage of social monogamy than bipar- there is empirical evidence that infanticide oc- ental provisioning of offspring (Freed 1986; see curs at predictably lower rates in groups with also Veiga 2000). This hypothesis as it applies more males (independently of female group to primates, notably the monogamous gibbons size). Even so, puzzles remain, such as the ap- (Hylobates spp.) (van Schaik and Dunbar 1990), lack of preference among female gorillas has been questioned on the basis of comparative for multimale groups as destinations for trans- data (Palombit 1999; Fuentes 2000; Brockelman fer (Robbins et al. 2009a). 2009), but recentindirect evidence of infanticide The above examples have focused on gregar- in one population of white-handed gibbons sug- ious animals, but even in dispersed societies in gests an intriguing possibility (Borries et al. which the sexes are relatively solitary and inter- 2011). Contradictory conclusions about the

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 19 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

selective role of infanticide in the evolution of infanticide to would-be perpetrators (e.g., pro- mammalian social monogamy have emerged miscuity, maternal aggression, coalitionary de- from recent phylogenetic and life history analy- fense with other females or males), to those that ses (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013; Opie et al. decrease its benefits (promoting small female 2013). group sizes or multimale social structure), to some that simply “cut the losses” of infanticide (e.g., pregnancy termination). It is also striking CONCLUSIONS that females in the well-studied populations Male infanticide appears to be an arresting ex- appear multiple times throughout Tables 2–4, ample of interlocus conflict (sensu Arnqvist and suggesting that females at risk of infanticide rely Rowe 2005). There is still a great deal we do not not on one counterstrategy but on an array of understand about the array of behaviors consti- counterstrategies. Lions provide an excellent ex- tuting this system. The adaptive significance, ample of this principle. When cycling, lionesses occurrence, and variability of male infanticide mate promiscuously to confuse paternity (Table remains unclear in manyspecies, and westill lack 2). During lactation, females with older cubs enough data to compare directly the lifetime (10 mo) capable of independent locomotion reproductive success of the “infanticidal” versus opt for a strategy of evasion, living largely soli- alternative “noninfanticidal” strategies. Never- tarily on the periphery of their home ranges theless, evidence accumulated over the last sev- (sexual segregation) (Table 3). Should alien eral decades yields several general conclusions. males be encountered visually or aurally, fe- First, in the well-studied populations, it seems males attempt to escape or resort to individual clear that infanticide is not simply an aberration maternal aggression (Table 3) (although the lat- or byproduct of other biological characters or ter counterstrategy carries potentially high conditions, but represents an adaptive strategy costs). For females with younger, less mobile for males in some circumstances. Second, the cubs, however, sexual segregation is less feasible, adaptive significance of the strategy is multifar- and they rely on membership in a pride of fe- ious; male infanticide is not unitary. The sexual male relatives (especially if it is a larger pride) selection hypothesis may be the best known ex- (Table 4). They thereby benefit from the coali- planation, but there is sufficient evidence to tionary defense that is “always able to overcome consider a multiplicity of other functions (Table or drive off one or a few males” (Pusey and 1). Third, whatever the benefit of infanticide to Packer 1994b, p. 297). But if prides become males, there is little doubt that it usually imposes too large and thus more attractive for male take- a substantive fitness cost on females (as well as over, then females may disperse (Table 4). The infants, of course), particularly in the slowly re- more general conclusion suggested here is that producing species in which one form of infanti- sexually antagonistic coevolution may be a cide—sexually selected—is expected to operate. mechanism accounting for much of the great This provides a strong rationale for expecting diversity of behavior. that male infanticide has acted as a selective There is an unquestionable need for further agent in the evolution of female biology in these research on possible female counterstrategies. species, perhaps operating as stronglyasthe con- There are alternative explanations for most of ventionally recognized contingencies related to the behaviors listed in Tables 2–4, which must predation and feeding ecology. be carefully tested (and rejected) to substantiate Female counterstrategies to infanticide have the anti-infanticide hypothesis. Analyses are been the subject of rigorous study for a relatively additionally complicated by the fact that pre- short time, however. What is perhaps most dictions of the anti-infanticide hypotheses are striking about current data is the great variety often similar to those of alternatives based on of possibilities that have been proposed (Tables ecology. Finally, although it seems likely that 2–4). These mechanisms range from counter- female counterstrategies will help to explain strategies that increase the potential costs of variation in the occurrence of male infanticide,

20 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

data from the wild are insufficient to address Agoramoorthy G, Mohnot SM, Sommer V, Srivastava A. this issue quantitatively. 1988. Abortions in free-ranging Hanuman langurs (Pres- bytis entellus)—A male induced strategy? Hum Evol 3: It should also be noted that the implications 297–308. for sexually antagonistic coevolution may ex- Agoramoorthy G, Rudran R. 1995. Infanticide by adult and tend well beyond infanticide and its immediate subadult males in free-ranging red howler monkeys, counterstrategies. A case in point concerns the Alouatta seniculus, in Venezuela. 99: 75–88. suite of behaviors collectively known as male Agrell J, Wolff JO, Ylo¨nen H. 1998. Counter-strategies to infanticide in mammals: Costs and consequences. Oikos “sexual coercion” of females. Those aspects of 83: 507–517. sexual coercion that involve restricting multi- Alexander RD. 1974. The evolution of . Annu male mating by females may be best understood Rev Ecol Syst 5: 325–383. as the male evolutionary response to promis- Altmann J. 1990. Primate males go where the females are. cuity as a female counterstrategy to the male Anim Behav 39: 192–195. strategy of infanticide (Wolff and MacDonald Altmann J, Altmann SA, Hausfater G. 1978. Primate infant’s effects on mother’s future reproduction. Science 201: 2004; Palombit 2014). Similarly, female promis- 1028–1029. cuity may have exerted selection on systems of Altmann J, Sayialel S, Bayes M, Bruford MW, Alberts SC. among males in some taxa, 2006. in baboons: Sexual selection versus which in turn may have then “counter selected” hungry hunters. Int J Primatol 27 (Suppl 1): 315. for mechanisms of cryptic female choice (Kap- Andelman SJ. 1987. Evolution of concealed ovulation in vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). Am Nat 129: peler 2012). Thus, the potential cascading ef- 785–799. fects of the sexual conflict originating with in- Angst W, Thommen D. 1977. New data and discussion of fanticide may be extensive, broad, and diverse. infant killing in Old World monkeys and apes. Folia Pri- In many ways, the study of male infanticide matol 27: 198–229. and female counterstrategies does not lend itself Arcadi CA, Wrangham RW. 1999. Infanticide in chimpan- zees: Review of cases and a new within-group observation well to the kinds of methodologies and reproduc- from Kanyawara study group in Kibale National Park. tive profiles that allow direct assessment of sexu- Primates 40: 337–351. ally antagonistic coevolution (Palombit 2010). Arnqvist G, Rowe L. 2005. Sexual conflict. Princeton Univer- The relevant organisms often reproduce slowly, sity Press, Princeton, NJ. are not easily amenable to experimental manipu- Aureli F, Schaffner CM, Boesch C, Bearder SK, Call J, Chap- man CA, Connor RC, di Fiore A, Dunbar RIM, Henzi SP, lation, and are difficult to study in the wild. De- et al. 2008. Fission-fusion dynamics. Curr Anthropol 49: spite these difficulties, however, these systems of- 627–654. fer opportunities to study the action of sexual Bartlett TQ, Sussman RW,Cheverud JM. 1993. Infant killing conflict in the context of highly gregarious life in primates: A review of observed cases with specific ref- erence to the sexual selection hypothesis. Am Anthropol styles and, more generally, to broaden our appre- 95: 958–990. ciation of the diversity of pathways over which Bartosˇ L, Madlafousek J. 1994. Infanticide in a seasonal sexually antagonistic coevolution has operated. breeder: The case of the red deer. Anim Behav 47: 217– 219. Bartosˇ L, Bartosˇova´ J, Pluha´ cˇek J, Sˇindela´rˇova´ J. 2011. Pro- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS miscuous behaviour disrupts pregnancy block in domes- tic horse mares. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65: 1567–1572. I thank William Rice and Sergey Gavrilets for Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2008. Infant mortality following inviting me to contribute to this volume. I am male takeovers in wild geladas. Am J Primatol 70: 1152– also grateful to S. Alavi, M. Janiak, S. Kivai, 1159. Beehner JS, Bergman J, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Whitten A. Moldawer, D. Prasetyo, and E. Vogel for dis- PL. 2005. The effect of new alpha males on female stress in cussion of the material for this manuscript. free-ranging baboons. Anim Behav 69: 111–1221. Bellemain E, Swenson JE, Taberlet P. 2006a. Mating strate- gies in relation to sexually selected infanticide in a non- REFERENCES social carnivore: The brown . Ethology 112: 238–246. Bellemain E, Zedrosser A, Manel S, Waits LP, Taberlet P, Agoramoorthy G, Mohnot SM. 1988. Infanticide and juve- Swenson JE. 2006b. The dilemma of female mate selec- nilicide in hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) around tion in the , a species with sexually selected Jodhpur, . Hum Evol 3: 279–296. infanticide. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 273: 283–291.

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 21 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Ben-David M, Titus K, Beier L. 2004. Consumption of salm- Bryja J, Patzenhauerova´ H, Albrecht T, Mosˇansky´ L, Stanko on by Alaskan brown bears: A trade-off between nutri- M, Stopka P. 2008. Varying levels of female promiscuity tional requirements and the risk of infanticide? Oecologia in four Apodemus mice species. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63: 138: 465–474. 251–260. Berdoy M, Drickamer LC. 2007. Comparative social orga- Butynski TM. 1982. -male replacement and infanti- nization and life history of Rattus and Mus.InRodent cide in the blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) societies: An ecological and evolutionary perspective (ed. in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. Am J Primatol 3: 1–22. Wolff JO, Sherman PA), pp. 380–392. University of Chi- Butynski TM. 1990. Comparative ecology of blue monkeys cago Press, Chicago. (Cercopithecus mitis) in high and low density subpopu- Berger J. 1983. Induced abortion and social factors in wild lations. Ecol Monogr 60: 1–26. horses. Nature 303: 59–61. Caley J, Boutin S. 1985. Infanticide in wild populations of Berman C, Li J, Ogawa H, Ionica C, Yin H. 2007. Primate Ondatra zibethicus and Microtus pennsylvanicus. Anim tourism, range restriction, and infant risk among Macaca Behav 33: 1036–1037. thibetana at Mt. Huangshan, China. Int J Primatol 28: Calhoun JB. 1962. Population density and social pathology. 1123–1141. Sci Am 206: 139–148. Bertram BC. 1975. Social factors influencing reproduction Cameron EZ, Linklater WL, Stafford KJ, Minot EO. 2003. in wild lions. J Zool 177: 463–482. Social grouping and maternal behaviour in feral horses Blumstein DT.2000. The evolution of infanticide in rodents: (Equus caballus): The influence of males on maternal A comparative analysis. In Infanticide by males and its protectiveness. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53: 92–101. implications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 178– Cameron EZ, Setsaas TH, Linklater WL. 2009. Social bonds 197. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. between unrelated females increase reproductive success Boggess JE. 1984. Infant killing and male reproductive strat- in feral horses. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 13850–13853. egies in langurs (Presbytis entellus). In Infanticide: Com- Carnegie SD, Fedigan LM, Ziegler TE. 2006. Post-conceptive parative and evolutionary perspectives (ed. Hausfater G, mating in white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus:Hor- Hrdy SB), pp. 283–310. Aldine, New York. monal and sociosexual patterns of cycling, noncycling, Borries C. 1997. Infanticide in seasonally breeding multi- and pregnant females. In New perspectives in the study of male groups of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus)in Mesoamerican primates (ed. Estrada A, et al.), pp. 387– Ramnagar (South Nepal). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41: 139– 409. Springer, New York. 150. Chapman M, Hausfater G. 1979. The reproductive conse- Borries C, Laundhard K, Epplen C, Epplen JT, Winkler P. quences of infanticide in langurs: A mathematical model. 1999a. DNA analyses support the hypothesis that infan- Behav Ecol Sociobiol 5: 227–240. ticide is adaptive in langur monkeys. Proc Roy Soc Lond B Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Fischer J, Beehner J, Bergman T, 266: 901–904. Johnson SE, Kitchen DM, Palombit RA, Rendall D, Silk Borries C, Launhardt K, Epplen C, Epplen JT, Winkler P. JB. 2004. Factors affecting reproduction and mortality 1999b. Males as infant protectors in Hanuman langurs among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. Int J (Presbytis entellus) living in mulitmale groups: Defense Primatol 25: 401–428. pattern, paternity, and sexual behavior. Behav Ecol Socio- Clarke MR. 1983. Infant-killing and infant disappearance biol 46: 350–356. following male takeovers in a group of free-ranging howl- Borries C, Savini T, Koenig A. 2011. Social monogamy and ing monkeys (Alouatta palliata) in Costa Rica. Am J Pri- the threat of infanticide in larger mammals. Behav Ecol matol 5: 241–247. Sociobiol 65: 685–693. Clarke PMR, Pradhan GR, van Schaik CP.2009. Intersexual Boyko RH, Marshall AJ. 2009. The willing cuckold: Optimal conflict in primates: Infanticide, paternity allocation, and paternity allocation, infanticide and male reproductive the role of coercion. In Sexual coercion in primates and strategies in mammals. Anim Behav 77: 1397. humans (ed. Muller MN, Wrangham RW), pp. 42–77. Brereton AR. 1995. Coercion-defense hypothesis: The evo- Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. lution of primate sociality. Folia Primatol 64: 207–214. Colmenares F, Gomendio M. 1988. Changes in female re- Brockelman WY. 2009. Ecology and the social system of productive condition following male take-overs in a col- gibbons. In The gibbons (ed. Lappan S, Whittaker D), ony of hamadryas and hybrid baboons. Folia Primatol 50: pp. 211–239. Springer, New York. 157–174. Brockman DK, Whitten PL. 1996. Reproduction in free- Connor RC. 2000. Group living in whales and dolphins. In ranging Propithecus verreauxi: Estrus and the relationship Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (ed. between multiple partner matings and fertilization. Am J Mann J, et al.), pp. 199–218. University of Chicago Press, Phys Anthropol 100: 57–69. Chicago. Brockman DK, Cobden AK, Whitten PL. 2009. Birth season Connor RC, Richard AF, Smolker RA, Mann J. 1996. Pat- glucocorticoids are related to the presence of infants in terns of female attractiveness in Indian Ocean bottle- sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi). Proc Roy Soc Lond B 276: nosed dolphins. Behaviour 133: 37–69. 1855–1863. Connor RC, Read AJ, Wrangham RW.2000. Male reproduc- Broom M, Borries C, Koenig A. 2004. Infanticide and infant tive strategies and social bonds. In Cetacean societies: Field defence by males—Modelling the conditions in primate studies of dolphins and whales (ed. Mann J, et al.), pp. multi-male groups. J Theor Biol 231: 261–270. 247–269. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

22 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Cords M. 1984. Mating patterns and social structure in red- Dugdale HL, Griffiths A, MacDonald DW.2011. Polygynan- tail monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius). Z Tierpsychol 64: drous and repeated mounting behaviour in European 313–329. badgers, Meles meles. Anim Behav 82: 1287–1297. Cords M. 1986. Promiscuous mating among blue monkeys Dunn DG, Barco SG, Pabst A, McLellan WA.2002. Evidence in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Ethology 72: 214–226. for infanticide in bottlenose dolphins of the western north Atlantic. J Wildlife Dis 38: 505–510. Cords M, Fuller JL. 2010. Infanticide in Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya: Variation in Ebensperger LA. 1998a. Strategies and counterstrategies to the occurrence of an adaptive behavior. Int J Primatol 31: infanticide in mammals. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 73: 409–431. 321–346. Cowlishaw G. 1999. Ecological and social determinants of Ebensperger LA. 1998b. The potential effects of protected nests and cage complexity on maternal aggression in spacing behaviour in desert baboon groups. Behav Ecol house mice. Agg Behav 24: 385–396. Sociobiol 45: 67–77. Ebensperger LA. 2001. No infanticide in the hystricognath Crockett CM. 2003. Re-evaluating the sexual selection hy- rodent, Octodon degus: Does ecology play a role? Acta pothesis for infanticide by Alouatta males. In Sexual se- Ethologica 3: 89–93. lection and reproductive competition in primates: New per- Ebensperger LA, Blumstein DT. 2007. Nonparental infanti- spectives and directions (ed. Jones CB), pp. 327–365. cide. In Rodent societies: An ecological and evolutionary American Society of Primatologists, Norman, OK. perspective (ed. Wolff JO, Sherman PA), pp. 267–279. Crockett CM, Janson CH. 2000. Infanticide in red howlers: University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Female group size, male membership, and a possible link Ebensperger LA, Botto-Mahan C, Tamarin RH. 2000. Non- to folivory. In Infanticide by males and its implications parental infanticide in meadow voles, Microtus pennsyl- (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 75–98. Cambridge vanicus: The influence of nutritional benefits. Ethol Ecol University Press, Cambridge. Evol 12: 149–160. Crofoot MC. 2007. Mating and feeding competition in Eggert A-K, Mu¨ller JK. 1997. Biparental care and social evo- white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus): The impor- lution in burying beetles: Lessons from the larder. In The tance of short- and long-term strategies. Behaviour 144: evolution of social behavior in insects and arachnids (ed. 1473–1495. Choe JC, Crespi BJ), pp. 216–236. Cambridge University Crook JR, Shields WM. 1985. Sexually selected infanticide Press, Cambridge. by adult male barn swallows. Anim Behav 33: 754–761. Elwood RW, Kennedy HF. 1994. Selective allocation of pa- Curtin R, Dolhinow P. 1978. Primate social behavior in a rental and infanticidal responses in rodents: A review of changing world. Am Sci 66: 468–475. mechanisms. In Infanticide and parental care (ed. Parmi- giani S, vom Saal F), pp. 397–425. Harwood, Chur, Swit- Dahle B, Swenson JE. 2003. Seasonal range size in relation to zerland. reproductive strategies in brown bears Ursus arctos. J Anim Ecol 72: 660–667. Elwood RW, Ostermeyer MC. 1984a. The effects of food deprivation, aggression, and isolation on infanticide in Daly M, Wilson M. 1984. A sociobiological analysis of hu- the male Mongolian gerbil. Agg Behav 10: 293–301. man infanticide. In Infanticide: Comparative and evolu- Elwood RW, Ostermeyer MC. 1984b. Infanticide by male tionary perspectives (ed. Hausfater G, Hrdy SB), pp. 487– and female Mongolian gerbils: Ontogeny, causation, 502. Aldine, New York. and function. In Infanticide: Comparative and evolution- Daly M, Wilson M. 1994. Stepparenthood and the evolved ary perspectives (ed. Hausfater G, Hrdy SB), pp. 367–386. psychology of discriminative parental solicitude. In In- Aldine, New York. fanticide and parental care (ed. Parmigiani S, vom Saal F), Elwood RW, Nesbitt AA, Kennedy HF. 1990. Maternal ag- pp. 121–134. Harwood, Chur, Switzerland. gression in response to the risk of infanticide by male Davies G. 1987. Adult male replacement and group forma- mice, Mus musculus. Anim Behav 40: 1080–1086. tion in Presbytis rubicunda. Folia Primatol 49: 111–114. Engh AL, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Whitten PL, Hoffmeier Delgado RA, van Schaik CP. 2000. The RR, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2006. Female hierarchy and conservation of the (Pongo pygmaeus): A instability, male immigration and infanticide increase tale of two islands. Evol Anthropol 9: 201–218. glucocorticoid levels in female chacma baboons. Anim Behav 71: 1227–1237. Delibes M, Bla´zquez MC, Delibes-Mateos M. 2012. Histor- ical books in ethology: Sexual purpose used to explain Enstam KL, Isbell LA, de Maar TW.2002. Male demography, two ancient documentations of infanticide by males. female mating behavior, and infanticide in wild patas Ethol Ecol Evol 24: 294–300. monkeys (Erythrocebus patas). Int J Primatol 23: 85–104. Erez T, Schneider JM, Lubin Y. 2005. Is male cohabitation de Villiers DJ. 1986. Infanticide in the tree squirrel, Para- costly for females of the spider Stegodyphus lineatus (Ere- xerus cepapi. S Afr J Zool 21: 183–184. sidae)? Ethology 111: 693–704. de WaalFBM. 1997. Bonobo: The forgotten ape. University of Fairbanks LA, McGuire MT. 1987. Mother–infant relation- California Press, Berkeley, CA. ships in vervet monkeys: Response to new adult males. Int Dolhinow P. 1977. Normal monkeys? Am Sci 65: 266. J Primatol 8: 351–366. Dudzinski KM, Gregg JD, Ribic CA, Kuczaj SA. 2009. A Fairgrieve C. 1995. Infanticide and infant eating in the blue comparison of pectoral fin contact between two different monkey (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni) in the Budongo wild populations. Behav Proc 80: 182–190. Forest Reserve, Uganda. Folia Primatol 64: 69–72.

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 23 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Fedigan LM, Carnegie SD, Jack KM. 2008. Predictors of ovulation and paternity confusion in free-ranging Hanu- reproductive success in female white-faced capuchins man langurs. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 268: 2445–2451. (Cebus capucinus). Am J Phys Anthropol 137: 82–90. Henzi SP,Clarke PMR, van Schaik CP,Pradhan GR, Barrett Feh C, Munkhtuya B. 2008. Male infanticide and paternity L. 2010. Infanticide and reproductive restraint in a po- analyses in a socially natural herd of Przewalski’s horses: lygynous social . Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 2130– Sexual selection? Behav Proc 78: 335–339. 2135. Freed LA. 1986. Territory takeover and sexually selected in- Hiraiwa-Hasegawa M. 1988. Adaptive significance of infan- fanticide in tropical house wrens. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 19: ticide in primates. Trends Ecol Evol 3: 102–105. 197–206. Hoesli T, Nikowitz T, Walzer C, Kaczensky P. 2009. Moni- Freed LA. 1987. Prospective infanticide and protection of toring of agonistic behaviour and foal mortality in free- genetic paternity in tropical house wrens. Am Nat 130: ranging Przewalski’s horse harems in the Mongolian 948–954. Gobi. Equus, September, 113–138. Fruteau C, Range F,Noe¨ R. 2010. Infanticide risk and infant Hoogland JL. 1985. Infanticide in prarie dogs: Lactating defence in multi-male free-ranging sooty mangabeys, females kill offspring of close kin. Science 230: 1037– Cercocebus atys. Behav Proc 83: 113–118. 1040. Fuentes A. 2000. Hylobatid communities: Changing views Hoogland JL. 2001. Black-tailed, Gunninson’s, and Utah on pair bonding and social organization in Hominoids. prairie dogs reproduce slowly. J Mammal 82: 917–927. Am J Phys Anthropol 43: 33–60. Hoogland JL. 2007. Alarm calling, multiple mating, and Galat-Luong A, Galat G. 1979. Conse´quences comporte- infanticide among black-tailed, Gunnison’s, and Utah mentales des perturbations sociales repete´es sure une prairie dogs. In Rodent societies: An ecological and evolu- troupe de Mones de Lowe, Cercopithecus campbelli lowei, tionary perspective (ed. Wolff JO, Sherman PA), pp. 438– de Coˆte d’Ivoire [Behavioral consequences of repeated 450. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. social disturbance in a group of Cercopithecus campbelli Hrdy SB. 1974. Male-male competition and infanticide lowei monkeys in the ]. Terre et Vie 33: 4–57. among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Glass GE, Holt R, Slade NA. 1985. Infanticide as an evolu- Folia Primatol 22: 19–58. tionarily stable strategy. Anim Behav 33: 384–391. Hrdy SB. 1977. The langurs of Abu: Female and male strat- Goodall J. 1986. The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of egies of reproduction. Harvard University Press, Cam- behavior. Belknap, Cambridge, MA. bridge, MA. Goodall J. 1990. Through a window. Houghton Mifflin, Hrdy SB. 1979. Infanticide among animals: A review, clas- Boston. sification, and examination of the implications for repro- Gray ME. 2009. An infanticide attempt by a free-roaming ductive strategies of females. Ethol Sociobiol 1: 13–40. feral stallion (Equus caballus). Biol Lett 5: 23–25. Hrdy SB. 1981. The woman that never evolved. Harvard Uni- Gray SM, Dill LM, McKinnon JS. 2007. Cuckoldry incites versity Press, Cambridge, MA. cannibalism: Male fish turn to cannibalism when per- Hrdy SB, Hausfater G. 1984. Comparative and evolutionary ceived certainty of paternity decreases. Am Nat 169: perspectives on infanticide: Introduction and overview. 258–263. In Infanticide: Comparative and evolutionary perspectives Grinnell J, McComb K. 1996. Maternal grouping as a de- (ed. Hausfater G, Hrdy SB), pp. xiii–xxxv. Aldine, fense against infanticide by males: Evidence from field New York. playback experiments on African lions. Behav Ecol 7: Ichino S. 2005. Attacks on a wild infant ring-tailed lemur 55–59. (Lemur catta) by immigrant males at Berenty, Madagas- Gust D. 1994. Alpha-male sooty mangabeys differentiate car: Interpreting infanticide by males. Am J Primatol 67: between females’ fertile and their postconception maxi- 267–272. mal swellings. Int J Primatol 15: 289–301. Isbell LA, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RD. 2002. Why vervet mon- Hamai M, Nishida T, Takasaki H, Turner LA. 1992. New keys (Cercopithecus aethiops) live in multimale groups. In records of within-group infanticide in wild chimpanzees. The guenons: Diversity and adaptation in African monkeys Primates 33: 151–162. (ed. Glenn ME, Cords M), pp. 173–187. Kluwer, Hamilton WD. 1971. Geometry for the selfish herd. J Theor New York. Biol 31: 295–311. Ishibashi Y,Saitoh T.2008. Effect of local density of males on Harris TR, Monfort SL. 2003. Behavioral and endocrine the occurrence of multimale mating in gray-sided voles dynamics associated with infanticide in a black and white (Myodes rufocanus). J Mammal 89: 388–397. colobus monkey (Colobus guereza). Am J Primatol 61: Izar P, Stone A, Carnegie S, Nakai ES. 2009. Sexual selec- 135–142. tion, female choice and mating systems. In South Amer- Hausfater G, Hrdy SB. 1984. Infanticide: Comparative and ican primates: Comparative perspectives in the study of evolutionary perspectives. Aldine, New York. behavior, ecology, and conservation (ed. Garber PA, et Heiming RS, Mo¨nning A, Jansen F, Kloke V, Lesch K-P, al.), pp. 157–189. Springer, New York. Sachser N. 2013. To attack, or not to attack? The role of Jack KM, Fedigan LM. 2009. Female dispersal in a female- serotonin transporter genotype in the display of maternal philopatric species, Cebus capucinus. Behaviour 146: aggression. Behav Brain Res 242: 135–141. 471–497. Heistermann M, Ziegler TE, van Schaik CP, Launhardt K, Jansen T, Forster P,Levine MA, Oelke H, Hurles M, Renfrew Winkler P, Hodges JK. 2001. Loss of oestrus, concealed C, Weber J, Olek K. 2002. Mitochondrial DNA and the

24 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

origins of the domestic horse. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99: Maestripieri D, Alleva E. 1990. Maternal aggression and 10905–10910. litter size in the female house mouse. Ethology 84: 27–34. Janson CH, Goldsmith ML. 1995. Predicting group size in Maestripieri D, Rossi-Arnaud C. 1991. Kinship does not primates: Foraging costs and predation risks. Behav Ecol affect litter defence in pairs of communally nesting fe- 6: 326–336. male house mice. Agg Behav 17: 223–228. Janson CH, van Schaik CP.2000. The behavioral ecology of Manning CJ, Dewsbury DA, Wakeland EK, Potts WK. 1995. infanticide by males. In Infanticide by males and its im- Communal nesting and communal nesting in house plications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 469–494. mice, Mus musculus domesticus. Anim Behav 50: 741– Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 751. Kappeler PM. 1997. Determinants of primate social organi- Marsh CW. 1979. Female transference and mate choice zation: Comparative evidence and new insights from among . Nature 281: 568–569. Malagasy lemurs. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 72: 111–151. Matsumoto-Oda A. 1999. Mahale chimpanzees: Grouping Kappeler PM. 2012. Mate choice. In The evolution of primate patterns and cycling females. Am J Primatol 47: 197–207. societies (ed. Mitani JC, et al.), pp. 367–386. University of McComb K, Pusey A, Packer C, Grinnell J. 1993. Female Chicago Press, Chicago. lions can identify potentially infanticidal males from Kiyota M, Okamura H. 2005. Harassment, abduction, and their roars. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 252: 59–64. mortality of pups by nonterritorial male northern fur McGuire B, Bemis WE. 2007. Parental care. In Rodent soci- seals. J Mammal 86: 1227–1236. eties: An ecological and evolutionary perspective (ed. Wolff Korstjens A, Nijssen E, Noe¨ R. 2005. Intergroup relation- JO, Sherman PA), pp. 231–242. University of Chicago ships in western black-and-white colobus, Colobus poly- Press, Chicago. komos polykomos. Int J Primatol 26: 1267–1289. McLean IG. 1982. The association of female kin in the Arctic Koskela E, Juutistenaho P,Mappes T, Oksanen T. 2000. Off- ground squirrel Spermophilus parryii. Behav Ecol Socio- spring defence in relation to litter size and age: Experi- biol 10: 91–99. ment in the bank vole Clethrionomys glareolus. Evol Ecol McLean IG. 1983. Paternal behaviour and killing of young in 14: 99–109. arctic ground squirrels. Anim Behav 31: 32–44. Kunz TH, Ebensperger LA. 1999. Why does non-parental McLellan BN. 2005. Sexually selected infanticide in grizzly infanticide seem so rare in bats? Acta Chiropterol 1: 17– bears: The effects of hunting on cub survival. Ursus 16: 29. 141–156. Lane JE, Boutin S, Gunn MR, Slate J, Coltman DW. 2008. Mennella JA, Moltz H. 1988. Infanticide in rats: Male strat- Female multiple mating and paternity in free-ranging egy and female counter-strategy. Physiol Behav 42: 19–28. North American red squirrels. Anim Behav 75: 1927– Miller SD, Sellers RA, Keay JA. 2003. Effects of hunting on 1937. brown bear cub survival and litter size in Alaska. Ursus Le Boeuf BJ, Campagna C. 1994. Protection and abuse of 14: 130–152. young in pinnipeds. In Infanticide and parental care (ed. Mitani JC, Watts DP, Amsler SJ. 2010. Lethal intergroup Parmigiani S, vom Saal F), pp. 257–276. Harwood, Chur, aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chim- Switzerland. panzees. Cur Biol 20: R507–R508. Le Galliard J-F, Gundersen G, Andreassen HP,Stenseth NC. Mock DW. 1984. Infanticide, , and avian nestling 2006. Natal dispersal, interactions among siblings and mortality. In Infanticide: Comparative and evolutionary intrasexual competition. Behav Ecol 17: 733–740. perspectives (ed. Hausfater G, Hrdy SB), pp. 3–30. Al- Lewinson R. 1998. Infanticide in the hippopotamus: Evi- dine, New York. dence for polygynous ungulates. Ethol Ecol Evol 10: 277– Møller AP.1988. Infanticidal and anti-infanticidal strategies 286. in the swallow Hirundo rustica. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 22: Lewis RJ. 2008. Social influences on group membership in 365–371. Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi. Int J Primatol 29: 1249– Møller AP. 2004. Rapid temporal change in frequency of 1270. infanticide in a associated with change in Lonstein JS, Gammie SC. 2002. Sensory, hormonal, and population density and body condition. Behav Ecol 15: neural control of maternal aggression in laboratory ro- 462–468. dents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26: 869–838. Morelli TL, King SJ, Pochron ST,Wright PC. 2009. The rules Lo¨tter T, Pillay N. 2012. Social interactions associated with of disengagement: Takeovers, infanticide, and dispersal reproduction in the bushveld gerbil Gerbilliscus leu- in a rainforest lemur, Propithecus edwardsi. Behaviour cogaster. Acta Theriol 57: 29–39. 146: 499–523. Loveridge AJ, Searle AW, Murindagomo F, MacDonald DW. Moscovice LR, Di Fiore A, Crockford C, Kitchen DM, Wittig 2007. The impact of sport-hunting on the population R, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2010. Hedging their bets? dynamics of an African population in a protected Male and female chacma baboons form friendships based area. Biol Conserv 134: 548–558. on likelihood of paternity. Anim Behav 79: 1007–1015. Lukas D, Clutton-Brock TH. 2013. The evolution of social Mu¨ller JK, Eggert AK, Sakaluk SK. 1998. Carcass mainte- monogamy in mammals. Science 341: 526–530. nance and biparental brood care in burying beetles: Are Maan ME, Taborsky M. 2008. Sexual conflict over breeding males redundant? Ecol Entomol 23: 195–200. substrate causes female expulsion and offspring loss in a Myers K, Poole WE. 1961. A study of the biology of the wild cichlid fish. Behav Ecol 19: 302–308. , Orytolagus cuniculus (L.), in confined popula-

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 25 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

tions: II. The effects of season and population increase on Palombit RA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 1997. The adaptive behaviour. CSIRO Wildlife Res 6: 1–41. value of “friendships” to female baboons: Experimental Nunn CL. 1999. The evolution of exaggerated sexual swell- and observational evidence. Anim Behav 54: 599–614. ings in primates and the graded-signal hypothesis. Anim Palombit RA, Cheney DL, Fischer J, Johnson S, Rendall D, Behav 58: 229–246. Seyfarth RM, Silk JB. 2000. Male infanticide and defense O’Brien TG. 1991. Female-male social interactions in of infants in wild chacma baboons. In Infanticide by males wedge-capped capuchin monkeys: Benefits and costs of and its implications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. group living. Anim Behav 41: 555–567. 123–152. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Oleinchenko VY. 2012. Behavioral interactions of adult fe- Palombit RA, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. 2001. Female–fe- males of the common shrew (Sorex araneus)withcon- male competition for male “friends” in wild chacma ba- specifics on familiar territory. Biol Bull 39: 351–359. boons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Anim Behav 61: Opie C, Atkinson QD, Dunbar RIM, Shultz S. 2013. Male 1159–1171. infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates. Proc Parmigiani S. 1986. Rank order in pairs of communally Natl Acad Sci 110: 13328–13332. nursing female mice (Mus musculus domesticus) and ma- Ostner J, Kappeler PM. 2004. Male life history and the un- ternal aggression towards conspecific intruders of differ- usual adult sex ratios of redfronted lemur, Eulemur fulvus ing sex. Agg Behav 12: 377–386. rufus, groups. Anim Behav 67: 249–259. Parmigiani S, Brain PF, Mainardi D, Brunoni V. 1988a. Dif- Packer C, Pusey AE. 1983. Adaptations of female lions to ferent patterns of biting attack generated when lactating infanticide by incoming males. Am Nat 121: 716–728. female mice (Mus domesticus) encounter male and fe- Packer C, Herbst L, Pusey AE, Bygott JD, Hanby JP, Cairns male conspecific intruders. J Comp Psychol 102: 287–293. SJ, Mulder MB. 1988. Reproductive success in lions. In Parmigiani S, Sgoifo A, Mainardi D. 1988b. Parental ag- Reproductive success: Studies of individual variation in gression displayed by female mice in relation to the sex, contrasting breeding systems (ed. Clutton-Brock TH), pp. 363–383. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. reproductive status, and infanticidal potential of con- specific intruders. Monit Zool Ital 22: 193–201. Packer C, Scheel D, Pusey AE. 1990. Why lions form groups: Food is not enough. Am Nat 136: 9–19. Parmigiani S, Palanza P,Brain PF.1989. Intraspecific mater- nal aggression in the house mouse (Mus domesticus): Palanza P, Re L, Mainardi D, Brain PF, Parmigiani S. 1996. A counterstrategy to infanticide by male? Ethol Ecol Male and female competitive strategies of wild house mice pairs (Mus musculus domesticus) confronted with Evol 1: 341–352. intruders of different sex and age in artificial territories. Parmigiani S, Palanza P,Mainardi M, Mainardi D. 1990. Fear Behaviour 133: 863–882. and defensive components of maternal aggression in Palombit RA. 1999. Infanticide and the evolution of pair mice. In Fear and defense (ed. Brain PF, et al.), pp. 109– bonds in nonhuman primates. Evol Anthropol 7: 117– 126. Harwood, London. 129. Parmigiani S, Palanza P,Mainardi D, Brain PF. 1994. Infan- Palombit RA. 2000. Infanticide and the evolution of male- ticide and protection of young in house mice (Mus do- female bonds in animals. In Male infanticide and its im- mesticus): Female and male strategies. In Infanticide and plications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 239–268. parental care (ed. Parmigiani S, vom Saal F), pp. 341– Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 363. Harwood, Chur, Switzerland. Palombit RA. 2003. Male infanticide in wild savanna ba- Parmigiani S, Palanza P, Rodgers J, Ferrari PF. 1999. Selec- boons: Adaptive significance and intraspecific variation. tion, evolution of behavior and animal models in behav- In Sexual selection and reproductive competition in pri- ioral neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 23: 957–970. mates: New perspectives and directions (ed. Jones CB), Patterson IAP, Reid RJ, Wilson B, Grellier K, Ross HM, pp. 364–411. American Society of Primatologists, Nor- Thompson PM. 1998. Evidence for infanticide in bottle- man, OK. nose dolphins: An explanation for violent interactions Palombit RA. 2009. Friendships with males: A female coun- with harbour porpoises. Proc Roy Soc Lond B 265: terstrategy to infanticide in the Okavango chacma ba- 1167–1170. boons. In Sexual coercion in primates and humans (ed. Muller MN, Wrangham RW), pp. 377–409. Harvard Paul L, Kupferschmidt J. 1975. Killing of conspecific and University Press, Cambridge, MA. mouse young by male rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 88: 755–763. Palombit RA. 2010. Conflict and bonding between the sexes. In Mind the gap: Tracing the origin of human universals Pave´ R, Kowalewski M, Garber P, Zunino G, Fernandez V, (ed. Kappeler PM, Silk JB), pp. 53–84. Springer, Berlin. Peker S. 2012. Infant mortality in black-and-gold howlers Palombit RA. 2012. Infanticide: Male strategies and female (Alouatta caraya) living in a flooded forest in northeast- counterstrategies. In The evolution of primate societies ern Argentina. Int J Primatol 33: 937–957. (ed. Mitani JC, et al.), pp. 432–468. University of Chica- Pazol K. 2003. Mating in the Kakamega forest blue monkeys go Press, Chicago. (Cercopithecus mitis): Does female sexual behavior func- Palombit RA. Infanticide. In Encyclopedia of human sexual- tion to manipulate paternity assessment? Behaviour 140: ity (ed. Whelehan P, Bolin A). Wiley, Hoboken, NJ (in 473–499. press). Pereira M. 1983. Abortion following the immigration of an Palombit RA. 2014. Sexual conflict in nonhuman primates. adult male baboon (Papio cynocephalus). Am J Primatol 4: Adv Stud Behav 46: 191–280. 93–98.

26 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Pereira ME, Weiss ML. 1991. Female mate choice, male mi- Robertson IC. 1993. Nest intrusions, infanticide, and paren- gration, and the threat of infanticide in ringtailed lemurs. tal care in the , Nicrophorus orbicollis (Co- Behav Ecol Sociobiol 28: 141–152. leoptera, Silphidae). J Zool 231: 583–593. Perrigo G, Bryant WC, vom Saal FS. 1990. A unique neural Robertson RJ, Stutchbury BJ. 1988. Experimental evidence timing system prevents male mice from harming their for sexually selected infanticide in tree swallows. Anim own offspring. Anim Behav 39: 535–539. Behav 36: 749–753. Perrigo G, Belvin L, vom Saal F. 1992. Time and sex in the Rohwer S. 1986. Selection for adoption versus infanticide by male mouse: Temporal regulation of infanticide and pa- replacement mates in birds. Cur Ornithol 3: 353–395. rental behavior. Chronobiol Int 9: 421–433. Rudran R. 1973a. The reproductive cycles of two subspecies of purple-faced langurs (Presbytis senex) with relation to Pierotti R. 1991. Infanticide versus adoption: An intergen- enviornmental factors. Folia Primatol 19: 41–60. erational conflict. Am Nat 138: 1140–1158. Rudran R. 1973b. Adult male replacement in one-male ´ ˇ ˇ Pluhacek J, Bartos L. 2000. Male infanticide in captive plains troops of purple faced langurs (Presbytis senex senex) , Equus caballus. Anim Behav 59: 689–694. and its effect on population structure. Folia Primatol Pluha´cˇek J, Bartosˇ L. 2005. Further evidence for male infan- 19: 166–192. ticide and feticide in captive zebra, Equus burchelli. Folia Sakura O. 1994. Factors affecting party size and composition Zool 54: 258–262. of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) Bossou, Guinea. Pusey AE, Packer C. 1987. The evolution of sex-biased dis- Int J Primatol 15: 167–183. persal in lions. Behaviour 101: 275–310. Schneider JM, Lubin Y.1997. Infanticide by males in a spider Pusey AE, Packer C. 1994a. Non-offspring nursing in social with suicidal maternal care, Stegodyphus lineatus (Eresi- carnivores: Minimizing the costs. Behav Ecol 5: 362–374. dae). Anim Behav 54: 305–312. Pusey AE, Packer C. 1994b. Infanticide in lions: Conse- Schu¨lke O. 2005. Evolution of pair-living in Phaner furcifer. quences and counterstrategies. In Infanticide and paren- Int J Primatol 26: 903–919. tal care (ed. Parmigiani S, vom Saal F), pp. 277–299. Sherman PW. 1981. Reproductive competition and infanti- Harwood, Chur, Switzerland. cide in Belding’s ground squirrels and other animals. In and social behavior (ed. Alexander RD, Rajpurohit IS, Chhangani AK, Rajpurohit RS, Bhaker NR, Tinkle DW), pp. 311–331. Chiron, New York. Rajpurohit DS, Sharma G. 2008. Recent observations on resident male change followed by infanticide in Hanu- Sicotte P,Teichroeb J, Saj T. 2007. Aspects of male competi- man langurs (Semnopithecus entellus) around Jodhpur. tion in Colobus vellerosus: Preliminary data on male and female loud calling, and infant deaths after a takeover. Int Primate Rep 75: 33–40. J Primatol 28: 627–636. Ramı´rez-Llorens P, Di Bitetti MS, Baldovino MC, Janson Singh M, Kumara HN, Ananda KM, Singh M, Cooper M. CH. 2008. Infanticide in black capuchin monkeys (Cebus 2006. Male influx, infanticide, and female transfer in apella nigritus) in Iguazu´ National Park, Argentina. Am J Macaca radiata. Int J Primatol 27: 515–528. Primatol 70: 473–484. Solomon NG, Keane B. 2007. Reproductive strategies in Rees A. 2009. The infanticide controversy. University of Chi- female rodents. In Rodent societies: An ecological and evo- cago Press, Chicago. lutionary perspective (ed. WolffJO, Sherman PA), pp. 42– Re´my A, Odden M, Richard M, Stene MT, Le Galliard J-F, 56. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Andreassen HP.2013. Food distribution influences social Soltis J, Thomsen R, Matusabayshi K, Takenaka O. 2000. organization and population growth in a small rodent. Infanticide by resident males and female counter-strate- Behav Ecol 24: 832–841. gies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behav Richard AF,Rakotomanga P,Schwartz M. 1993. Dispersal by Ecol Sociobiol 48: 195–202. Propithecus verreauxi at Beza Mahafaly, Madagascar: Sommer V.1994. Infanticide among the langurs of Jodhpur: 1984–1991. Am J Primatol 30: 1–20. Testing the sexual selection hypothesis with a long-term Rijksen HD. 1981. Infant killing: A possible consequence of record. In Infanticide and parental care (ed. Parmigiani S, vom Saal F), pp. 155–198. Harwood, Chur, Switzerland. a disputed leader role. Behaviour 78: 138–167. Sommer S. 1997. Monogamy in Hypogeomys antimena,an Robbins AM, Stoinski T, Fawcett K, Robbins M. 2009a. So- endemic rodent of the deciduous dry forest in western cioecological influences on the dispersal of female moun- Madagascar. J Zool 241: 301–314. tain gorillas—Evidence of a second folivore paradox. Be- Starin ED. 1994. Philopatry and affiliation among red col- hav Ecol Sociobiol 63: 477–489. obus. Behaviour 130: 253–270. Robbins AM, Stoinski T, Fawcett K, Robbins MM. 2009b. Steenbeek R. 1999a. Tenure related changes in wild Leave or conceive: Natal dispersal and philopatry of fe- Thomas’s langurs: I. Between-group interactions. Behav- male mountain gorillas in the Virunga volcano region. iour 136: 595–625. Anim Behav 77: 831–838. Steenbeek R. 1999b. “Female choice and male coercion in Roberts EK, Lu A, Bergman TJ, Beehner JC. 2012. A Bruce wild Thomas’s langurs.” PhD dissertation, Utrecht Uni- effect in wild geladas. Science 335: 1222–1225. versity, Netherlands. Robertson RJ. 1990. Tactics and counter-tactics of sexually Steenbeek R. 2000. Infanticide by males and female choice selected infanticide in tree swallows. In Population biology in wild Thomas’s langurs. In Infanticide by males and its of passerine birds: An integrated approach (ed. Blondel J, et implications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 153– al.), pp. 381–390. Springer, Berlin. 177. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 27 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

R.A. Palombit

Sterck EHM. 1997. Determinants of female dispersal in Trumbo ST.1991. Reproductive benefits and the duration of Thomas langurs. Am J Primatol 42: 179–198. in a biparental burying beetle, Necrophorus Sterck EHM. 1998. Variation in langur social organization in orbicollis. Behaviour 117: 82–115. relation to the socioecological model, human habitat al- Trumbo ST. 2007. Defending young biparentally: Female teration, and phylogenetic constraints. Primates 40: 199– risk-taking with and without a male in the burying bee- 213. tle, Nicrophorus pustulatus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 61: Sterck EHM, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. 1997. The evolution 1717–1723. of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Trumbo ST, Valletta RC. 2007. The costs of confronting Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41: 291–309. infanticidal intruders in a burying beetle. Ethology 113: Stokes EJ. 2004. Within-group social relationships among 386–393. females and adult males in wild western lowland gorillas van Noordwijk MA, van Schaik CP.2000. Reproductive pat- (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Am J Primatol 64: 233–246. terns in eutherian mammals: Adaptations against infan- Stokes E, Parnell RJ, Olejniczak C. 2003. Female dispersal ticide. In Infanticide by males and its implications (ed. van and reproductive success in wild western lowland gorillas. Schaik CP, Janson CH), pp. 322–360. Cambridge Uni- Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54: 329–339. versity Press, Cambridge. Struhsaker TT, Leland L. 1985. Infanticide in a patrilineal van Schaik CP. 2000a. Infanticide by male primates: The society of red colobus monkeys. Z Tierpsychol 69: 89– sexual selection hypothesis revisited. In Infanticide by 132. males and its implications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson Svare B, Bartke A. 1978. Food deprivation induces conspe- CH), pp. 27–60. Cambridge University Press, Cam- cific pup-killing in mice. Agg Behav 4: 253–261. bridge. Swedell L, Saunders J. 2006. Infant mortality, paternity cer- van Schaik CP.2000b. Vulnerability to infanticide by males: tainty, and female reproductive strategies in hamadryas Patterns among mammals. In Infanticide by males and its baboons. In Reproduction and fitness in baboons: Behav- implications (ed. van Schaik CP,Janson CH), pp. 61–71. ioral, ecological, and life history perspectives (ed. Swedell L, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Leigh SR), pp. 19–51. Springer, New York. van Schaik CP, Dunbar RIM. 1990. The evolution of mo- Swenson JE, Brunberg S, Segerstro¨m P. 2001. Factors asso- nogamy in large primates: A new hypothesis and some ciated with loss of brown bear cubs in Sweden. Ursus 12: crucial tests. Behaviour 115: 30–62. 69–80. van Schaik CP,Janson CH (eds.). 2000. Infanticide by males Takahata Y,Sprague DS, Suzuki S, Okayasu N. 1994. Female and its implications. Cambridge University Press, Cam- competition, co-existence, and the mating structure of bridge. wild Japanese macaques on Yakushima Island, Japan. van Schaik CP, Hodges JK, Nunn CL. 2000. Paternity con- Physiol Ecol 29: 163–179. fusion and the ovarian cycles of female primates. In In- Teichroeb JA, Sicotte P. 2008. Infanticide in ursine colobus fanticide by males and its implications (ed. van Schaik CP, monkeys (Colobus vellerosus) in Ghana: New cases and a Janson CH), pp. 361–387. Cambridge University Press, test of the existing hypotheses. Behaviour 145: 727–755. Cambridge. Teichroeb JA, Wikberg EC, Sicotte P.2009. Female dispersal Veiga JP. 1993. Prospective infanticide and ovulation retar- patterns in six groups of ursine colobus (Colobus veller- dation in free-living sparrows. Anim Behav 45: 43–46. osus): Infanticide avoidance is important. Behaviour 146: Veiga JP. 2000. Infanticide by male birds. In Infanticide by 551–582. males and its implications (ed. van Schaik CP, Janson Thalmann U. 2001. Food resource characteristics in two CH), pp. 198–220. Cambridge University Press, Cam- nocturnal lemurs with different social behavior: Avahi bridge. occidentalis and Lepilemur edwardsi. Int J Primatol 22: 287–324. Vestal BM. 1991. Infanticide and cannibalism by male thir- teen-lined ground squirrels. Anim Behav 41: 1103–1104. Thonhauser KE, Raveh S, Hettyey A, Beissmann H, Penn DJ. 2013. Why do female mice mate with multiple males? vom Saal FS. 1985. Time-contingent change in infanticide Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67: 1961–1970. and parental behavior induced by ejaculation in male mice. Physiol Behav 34: 7–15. Treves A. 2001. Reproductive consequences of variation in the composition of howler monkey (Alouatta spp.) vom Saal FS, Howard LS. 1982. The regulation of infanticide groups. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50: 61–71. and parental behavior: Implications for reproductive suc- cess in male mice. Science 215: 1270–1272. Treves A, Chapman CA. 1996. Conspecific threat, predation avoidance, and resource defense: Implications for group- Watts DP.1989. Infanticide in mountain gorillas: New cases ing in langurs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 39: 43–53. and a reconsideration of the evidence. Ethology 81: 1–18. Trivers RL. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. Watts DP,Mitani JC, Sherrow HM. 2002. New cases of inter- In Sexual selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971 (ed. community infanticide by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Campbell B), pp. 136–179. Aldine, Chicago. Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 43: 263–270. Trumbo ST. 1990a. Reproductive benefits of infanticide in a Weber EM, Olsson IAS. 2008. Maternal behaviour in Mus biparental burying beetle Nicrophorus orbicollis. Behav musculus sp.: An ethological review. Appl Anim Behav Sci Ecol Sociobiol 27: 269–273. 114: 1–22. Trumbo ST. 1990b. Interference competition among bury- Weingrill T. 2000. Infanticide and the value of male-female ing beetles (Silphidae, Nicrophorus). Ecol Entomol 15: relationships in mountain chacma baboons (Papio cyn- 347–355. ocephalus ursinus). Behaviour 137: 337–359.

28 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict

Wielgus RB, Bunnell FL. 2000. Possible negative effects of Wrangham RW, Smuts BB. 1980. Sex differences in the be- adult male mortality on female grizzly bear reproduction. havioural ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Biol Conserv 93: 145–154. Park. J Reprod Fertil Suppl 28: 13–31. Wielgus RB, Morrison DE, Cooley HS, Maletzke B. 2013. Wright PC. 1995. Demography and life history of free-rang- Effects of male trophy hunting on female carnivore pop- ing Propithecus diadema edwardsi in Ranomafana Na- ulation growth and persistence. Biol Conserv 167: 69–75. tional Park, Madagascar. Int J Primatol 16: 835–854. Williams GC. 1966. Adaptation and natural selection. Prince- Yamada K, Nakamichi M. 2006. A fatal attack on an un- ton University Press, Princeton, NJ. weaned infant by a non-resident male in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata)atKat- Williams JM, Pusey AE, Carlis JV,Farm BP,Goodall J. 2002. suyama. Primates 47: 165–169. Female competition and male territorial behaviour influ- ence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Anim Behav Yamagiwa J, Kahekwa J, Basabose AK. 2009. Infanticide and social flexibility in the genus Gorilla. Primates 50: 293– 63: 347–360. 303. Williams JM, Oehlert GW, Carlis JV, Pusey AE. 2004. Why Yamaguchi N, Dugdale H, MacDonald DW. 2006. Female do male chimpanzees defend a group range? Anim Behav receptivity, embryonic diapause, and superfetation in the 68: 523–532. European badger (Meles meles): Implications for the re- Williams JM, Lonsdorf EV, Wilson ML, Schumacher-Stan- productive tactics of males and females. Q Rev Biol 81: key J, Goodall J, Pusey AE. 2008. Causes of death in the 33–48. Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanza- Yamamura N, Hasegawa T,Ito Y.1990. Why mothers do not nia. Am J Primatol 70: 766–777. resist infanticide: A cost-benefit genetic model. Evolution Wolff JO, Cicirello DM. 1991. Comparative paternal and 44: 1346–1357. infanticidal behavior of white-footed mice (Peromyscus Ylo¨nen H, Koskela E, Mappes T. 1997. Infanticide in the leucopus noveoboracensis) and deermice (P. maniculatus bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus): Occurrence and nubiterrae). Behav Ecol 2: 38–45. the effect of familiarity on female infanticide. Ann Zoo Wolff JO, MacDonald DW. 2004. Promiscuous females pro- Fenn 34: 259–266. tect their offspring. Trends Ecol Evol 19: 127–134. Zedrosser A, Dahle B, Støen O-G, Swenson J. 2009. The Wolff JO, Peterson JA. 1998. An offspring-defense hypoth- effects of primiparity on reproductive performance in esis for territoriality in female mammals. Ethol Ecol Evol the brown bear. Oecologia 160: 847–854. 10: 227–239. Zinner D, Deschner T. 2000. Sexual swellings in female Wrangham RW.1980. An ecological model of female-bond- hamadryas baboons after male take-overs: “Deceptive” ed primate groups. Behaviour 75: 262–299. swellings as a possible female counter-strategy against infanticide. Am J Primatol 52: 157–168.

Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 29 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies

Ryne A. Palombit

Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015; doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a017640 originally published online May 18, 2015

Subject Collection The Genetics and Biology of Sexual Conflict

Mechanisms and Evidence of Genital Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Coevolution: The Roles of Natural Selection, Mate Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies Choice, and Sexual Conflict Ryne A. Palombit Patricia L.R. Brennan and Richard O. Prum The Evolution of Sexually Antagonistic Copulatory Wounding and Traumatic Phenotypes Insemination Jennifer C. Perry and Locke Rowe Klaus Reinhardt, Nils Anthes and Rolanda Lange Reproductive : Maternally Inherited Sexual Conflict in Hermaphrodites Symbionts in a Biparental World Lukas Schärer, Tim Janicke and Steven A. Ramm Gregory D.D. Hurst and Crystal L. Frost Sex-Biased Gene Expression and Sexual Conflict Sexual Conflict and Sperm Competition throughout Development Dominic A. Edward, Paula Stockley and David J. Fiona C. Ingleby, Ilona Flis and Edward H. Morrow Hosken Human Homosexuality: A Paradigmatic Arena for Sexually Antagonistic Zygotic Drive: A New Form Sexually Antagonistic Selection? of Genetic Conflict between the Sex Andrea Camperio Ciani, Umberto Battaglia and Chromosomes Giovanni Zanzotto Urban Friberg and William R. Rice Sexual Conflict Arising from Extrapair Matings in Sex Chromosome Drive Birds Quentin Helleu, Pierre R. Gérard and Catherine Alexis S. Chaine, Robert Montgomerie and Bruce Montchamp-Moreau E. Lyon Sexual Conflict and Seminal Fluid Proteins: A Is Sexual Conflict an ''Engine of Speciation''? Dynamic Landscape of Sexual Interactions Sergey Gavrilets Laura K. Sirot, Alex Wong, Tracey Chapman, et al.

For additional articles in this collection, see http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/cgi/collection/

Copyright © 2015 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; all rights reserved Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press

Conflict on the Sex Chromosomes: Cause, Effect, as a Manifestation of Sexual and Complexity Conflict Judith E. Mank, David J. Hosken and Nina Wedell Jutta M. Schneider

For additional articles in this collection, see http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/cgi/collection/

Copyright © 2015 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; all rights reserved