Infanticide As Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Infanticide As Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies Ryne A. Palombit Department of Anthropology, Center for Human Evolutionary Studies, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08901 Correspondence: [email protected] One of the earliest recognized forms of sexual conflict was infanticide by males, which imposes serious costs on female reproductive success. Here I review two bodies of evidence addressing coevolved strategies of males and females. The original sexual selection hy- pothesis arguing that infanticide improves male mating success by accelerating the return of females to fertilizable condition has been generally supported in some taxa—notably, some primates, carnivores, rodents, and cetaceans—but not in other taxa. One result of recent research has been to implicate other selective benefits of infanticide by males in various taxa from insects to birds to mammals, such as acquisition of breeding status or improvement of the female breeding condition. In some cases, however, the adaptive sig- nificance of male infanticide remains obscure. The second body of data I review is arguably the most important result of recent research: clarifying the possible female counterstrategies to infanticide. These potential counterstrategies span diverse biological systems, ranging from sexual behavior (e.g., polyandrous mating), to physiology (e.g., the Bruce effect), to individual behavior (e.g., maternal aggression), to social strategies (e.g., association with coalitionary defenders of either sex). Although much remains to be studied, these current data provide compelling evidence of sexually antagonistic coevolution surrounding the phenomenon of infanticide. t its most elemental level, infanticide is the and Hausfater 1984, p. xv). These definitions Akilling of a newborn individual by a conspe- highlight the heterogeneous and variable nature cific. With the growing appreciation of its bio- of the phenomenon, which can be perpetrated logical significance, however, infanticide came by either sex, by parents or other kin, by indi- to be defined more broadly as any “behavior viduals unrelated to the victim, in a wide variety that makes a direct and significant contribution of social and mating systems, under a range of to the immediate death of an embryo or newly seasonal or aseasonal breeding regimes, and hatched or born member of the performer’s across diverse taxa straddling vertebrates and own species” (Mock 1984, p. 4) or “any form invertebrates. of lethal curtailment of parental investment in One adaptive form of infanticide—the kill- offspring brought about by conspecifics” (Hrdy ing of infants by unrelated males—is arguably Editors: William R. Rice and Sergey Gavrilets Additional Perspectives on The Genetics and Biology of Sexual Conflict available at www.cshperspectives.org Copyright # 2015 Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; all rights reserved; doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a017640 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 1 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press R.A. Palombit the archetype of sexual conflict. In 450 BCE, animals as well as additional data from pri- Herodotus not only documented the behavior mates. among Egyptian cats, but explained it as a male “trick” to obtain sexual access to females other- Sexual Selection Hypothesis wise preoccupied with maternal duties (Delibes et al. 2012). Among the myriad ideas inaugurat- Although male infanticide clearly has no uni- ing sociobiology in the 1970s, the hypothesis tary explanation, the biological relevance of in- that infanticide is a male strategy that improves trasexual selection in males makes the sexual reproductive success at the expense of female selection hypothesis a good starting point for fitness (Hrdy 1974) constituted one of the first discussion. As originally formulated for langurs demonstrations of the “battle of the sexes” the- (Semnopithecus entellus), Hrdy’s (1974, 1977) ory developed by Williams (1966) and Trivers argument is that the killing of an infant pre- (1972). Partly because of the controversy sur- maturely ends lactational amenorrhea (the ces- rounding the appearance of this hypothesis sation of ovulatory cycles during nursing) in (Rees 2009), however, subsequent research fo- its mother, thereby significantly advancing the cused more on male strategy than on the other time when she is available for subsequent fer- party in this sexual dialectic, the female. Thus, tilization by the perpetrator. The potential ben- field and laboratory research has helped to es- efit of this strategy to male fitness will depend tablish its many forms and conditional occur- on many variables, but salient among them is rence, describe its distribution across taxa, and the degree to which sexual access to fecund fe- clarify its adaptive significance, but it is only males is limited by two factors: male–male relatively recently that female counterstrategies competition and the reproductive life history have become the subjects of rigorous study, even of females. though their potential importance was grasped The latter of these factors provides a useful early on (Hrdy 1979). context for analyzing the problem of male in- In this article, I review selected aspects of fanticide. The L/G ratio, which compares the this body of data and analysis. My focus is durations of lactation and gestation, summariz- on nonparental male infanticide targeting de- es the potential value (to males) and vulnera- pendent young—in mammals, nursing individ- bility (to females) of infanticide in mammals. uals—as opposed to older, weaned offspring, When lactation greatly exceeds gestation, post- the killing of which is variably rendered “juve- partum mating and early pregnancy impose nilicide,” “pedicide,” or “filicide” (e.g., Agora- potentially prohibitive costs on females in the moorthy and Mohnot 1988; Palombit 2014, in form of simultaneous support of two sets of press). offspring differing in ages, requirements, and competitive capacities (van Schaik 2000b). The adaptive solution to this female problem is lactational amenorrhea, which, however, gen- THE SEXUAL CONFLICT OF INFANTICIDE: erates the reproductive problem for males that MALE STRATEGIES infanticide can solve. Plainly expressed: An in- Several functional hypotheses have been pro- fant is a “perfect contraceptive,” and infanticide posed to explain male infanticide. Foremost simply serves to remove this effect (Altmann among them historically is the sexual selection et al. 1978, p. 1029). hypothesis. I consider this hypothesis first, with The taxonomic distribution of sexually se- special, although not exclusive, taxonomic fo- lected infanticide across mammals is not fully cus on the group of animals that originally gen- resolved, but current reports of infanticide erated the hypothesis—the primates. I then whose patterning is consistent with this hy- move on to several alternative hypotheses for pothesis are predictably concentrated among male infanticide, the discussion of which will taxa with relatively long L/G ratios— namely, incorporate consideration of data from other primates, fissiped carnivores, and odontocete 2 Cite this article as Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol 2015;7:a017640 Downloaded from http://cshperspectives.cshlp.org/ on September 24, 2021 - Published by Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press Infanticide as Sexual Conflict whales (Fig. 1) (van Schaik 2000b). Several ca- Schaik and Janson 2000; Ebensperger and veats warrant mention. First, this result does Blumstein 2007; Palombit 2012), but some not mean that male infanticide is uniformly general patterns can be highlighted. The largest expressed within these taxa; on the contrary, body of data supporting this hypothesis has there is often considerable variation over time come from study of the nonhuman primates. and across populations (see below). Second, it is In the last 25 years, the number of species in also not the case that male infanticide is un- which infanticide had been observed directly reported in other taxa with lower L/Gratios, or inferred from substantive indirect evidence but in those cases the sexually selected mecha- in the wild has risen from a dozen (Hiraiwa- nism has been less implicated than alternative Hasegawa 1988) to 54, including 84 distinct hypotheses (see below). Finally, L/G ratios are populations distributed across nine of the 14 important, but other factors mayexplain the rar- families of strepsirrhines, monkeys, and apes ity of infanticide in some mammals, for exam- (updated from Palombit 2012). ple, ecologically determined sexual segregation, The circumstances surrounding infanticide (KunzandEbensperger1999),caching ofinfants in primates are typically (although not invari- as an antipredator strategy (Schu¨lke 2005), need ably) consistent with the sexual selection hy- for hibernation (Thalmann 2001), or even phy- pothesis: (1) replacement of a male occupying logenetic inertia (Ebensperger 2001). the position of sole or dominant breeder in the It is not possible here to review comprehen- group by a male who is unlikely to have sired the sively the empirical tests of the sexual selec- infants he subsequently attacks; (2) resumption tion hypothesis (see Hausfater and Hrdy 1984; of ovulatory cycling by the mother; and (3) Parmigiani et al. 1994; Connor et al. 2000;
Recommended publications
  • Are Male Orangutans a Threat to Infants? Evidence of Mother–Offspring Counterstrategies to Infanticide in Bornean Orangutans (Pongo Pygmaeus Wurmbii)
    Are Male Orangutans a Threat to Infants? Evidence of Mother–Offspring Counterstrategies to Infanticide in Bornean Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) Amy M. Scott, Cheryl D. Knott & Tri Wahyu Susanto International Journal of Primatology The Official Journal of the International Primatological Society ISSN 0164-0291 Int J Primatol DOI 10.1007/s10764-019-00097-8 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be self-archived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self- archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com”. 1 23 Author's personal copy International Journal of Primatology https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-019-00097-8 Are Male Orangutans a Threat to Infants? Evidence of Mother–Offspring Counterstrategies to Infanticide in Bornean Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) Amy M. Scott, et al. [full author details at the end of the article] Received: 25 March 2019 /Accepted: 2 April 2019/ # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019 Abstract Sexually selected infanticide by males is widespread in primates.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies
    Human Mating Strategies Human Mating Strategies As descendants of a long line of successful maters, modern humans have inherited the mating strategies that led to their forebear’s success. These include long-term mating, short-term mating, and mixed mating strategies. This article presents empirical evi- dence supporting evolution-based hypotheses about the complexities of these mating strategies, which differ substantially for men and women. array of adaptations specifically dedicated to the David M. Buss, Professor, task of mating. Department of Psychology, Nowhere do people have an equal desire to mate University of Texas, Austin with all people. Everywhere, some people are pre- ferred as mates, others shunned. Desires are central to all facets of mating. They determine who we are attracted to, and who is attracted to us. They influ- ence which attraction tactics will be successful (those that fulfill desires) and which attraction tac- tics will fail (those that violate desires). Successful mate retention tactics involve continuing to provide resources that fulfill the desires of a mate. Failure to Perhaps no adaptive domain is more central to re- fulfill these desires causes breakup and divorce. At production than mating. Those in our evolutionary every step of the mating process, from mate selec- past who failed to mate failed to become ancestors. tion to mate expulsion, desires determine the Modern humans are all descendants of a long and ground rules. unbroken line of ancestors who succeeded in the complex and sometimes circuitous tasks involved in Sexual Selection and Parental Investment mating. As their descendants, modern humans have Although Charles Darwin (1859) recognized that inherited the adaptations that led to the success of survival was central to the evolutionary process, their ancestors.
    [Show full text]
  • Altriciality and the Evolution of Toe Orientation in Birds
    Evol Biol DOI 10.1007/s11692-015-9334-7 SYNTHESIS PAPER Altriciality and the Evolution of Toe Orientation in Birds 1 1 1 Joa˜o Francisco Botelho • Daniel Smith-Paredes • Alexander O. Vargas Received: 3 November 2014 / Accepted: 18 June 2015 Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015 Abstract Specialized morphologies of bird feet have trees, to swim under and above the water surface, to hunt and evolved several times independently as different groups have fish, and to walk in the mud and over aquatic vegetation, become zygodactyl, semi-zygodactyl, heterodactyl, pam- among other abilities. Toe orientations in the foot can be prodactyl or syndactyl. Birds have also convergently described in six main types: Anisodactyl feet have digit II evolved similar modes of development, in a spectrum that (dII), digit III (dIII) and digit IV (dIV) pointing forward and goes from precocial to altricial. Using the new context pro- digit I (dI) pointing backward. From the basal anisodactyl vided by recent molecular phylogenies, we compared the condition four feet types have arisen by modifications in the evolution of foot morphology and modes of development orientation of digits. Zygodactyl feet have dI and dIV ori- among extant avian families. Variations in the arrangement ented backward and dII and dIII oriented forward, a condi- of toes with respect to the anisodactyl ancestral condition tion similar to heterodactyl feet, which have dI and dII have occurred only in altricial groups. Those groups repre- oriented backward and dIII and dIV oriented forward. Semi- sent four independent events of super-altriciality and many zygodactyl birds can assume a facultative zygodactyl or independent transformations of toe arrangements (at least almost zygodactyl orientation.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Selection
    Sexual Selection Carol E. Lee University of Wisconsin Copyright ©2020 Do not upload without permission Natural Selection Evolutionary Mechanisms Genetic Drift Migration Non-adaptive Mutations Natural Selection Adaptive OUTLINE (1) Sexual Selection (2) Constraints on Natural Selection Pleiotropy Evolutionary Tradeoffs Genetic Drift and Natural Selection ■ There are species with 3 or more sexes (some ciliates have 32)... Too complex to discuss here ■ Restrict my discussion here to 2 sexes Male Female Male Female Male Female HUH???? What about us??? (New World Monkeys) Theory of Sexual Selection ■ The sex bearing a higher cost to reproduction or has higher parental investment will generally be the chooser (has more to lose from bad choice) ■ Whereas the sex bearing the lesser cost of reproduction or parental investment generally competes more heavily for mates (Bateman 1948) Asymmetric Limits on Fitness • The sex that has higher reproductive cost will be the choosers • If you’re allocating a lot of resources toward offspring and are limited in the number of offspring you can have, you won’t mate with just anyone • The sex that is being selected (under sexual selection) will be competitive • Fight with each other, fight for resources, fight for access to or control of the other sex Theory of Sexual Selection ■ In sexual species, it is usually males who invest less in each offspring à it is typical for males to compete for access to females ■ In 90% of mammal species, females provide substantial parental care, while males provide little or
    [Show full text]
  • Variance in the Male Reproductive Success of Western Gorillas: Acquiring Females Is Just the Beginning
    Behav Ecol Sociobiol (2010) 64:515–528 DOI 10.1007/s00265-009-0867-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Variance in the male reproductive success of western gorillas: acquiring females is just the beginning Thomas Breuer & Andrew M. Robbins & Claudia Olejniczak & Richard J. Parnell & Emma J. Stokes & Martha M. Robbins Received: 26 April 2009 /Revised: 17 September 2009 /Accepted: 18 September 2009 /Published online: 14 October 2009 # Springer-Verlag 2009 Abstract Variance in male reproductive success is that enhance offspring survival. Thus, this study illustrates expected to be high in sexually dimorphic mammals, even how a detailed analysis of the components of male when it is modulated by the costs and benefits of group reproductive success can shed light on the interrelated living. Here, we investigate the variance in reproductive social and ecological aspects that affect it. success of male western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), a highly dimorphic primate with long-term male–female associa- Keywords Harem size . Intrasexual competition . tions, using 12.5 years of data collected at Mbeli Bai in Mating success . Offspring survival . northern Congo. Access to mates and offspring survival Variance in reproductive success were both major sources of variance in male reproductive success. Males with larger harems had lower offspring mortality with no apparent reduction in female fertility or Introduction observed tenure length, so the size of harems did not seem to be limited by female feeding competition or by the risk High variance in male reproductive success is assumed to of takeovers and infanticide by outsider males. The lower provide a potential for sexual selection that can lead to mortality in larger harems may reflect improved vigilance sexual dimorphism (Darwin 1871; Andersson 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALGARY Determinants of Group Size and Composition for Colobus Vellerosus at Boabeng-Fiema, Ghana
    UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY Determinants of Group Size and Composition for Colobus vellerosus at Boabeng-Fiema, Ghana: Ecological versus Social Factors by Julie A. Teichroeb A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF ANTHROPOLOGY CALGARY, ALBERTA MARCH, 2009 © Julie A. Teichroeb 2009 UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES The undersigned certify that they have read, and recommend to the Faculty of Graduate Studies for acceptance, a thesis titled “Determinants of Group Size and Composition for Colobus vellerosus at Boabeng-Fiema, Ghana: Ecological versus Social Factors” submitted by Julie A. Teichroeb in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. ___________________________________ Supervisor, Dr. Pascale Sicotte, Department of Anthropology ___________________________________ Dr. Linda Fedigan, Department of Anthropology ___________________________________ Dr. Robert Longair, Department of Biological Sciences ___________________________________ Dr. Warren Wilson, Department of Archaeology ___________________________________ External Examiner, Dr. Rasanayagam Rudran, Scientist Emeritus, Smithsonian Institution _____________ Date ii ABSTRACT Intense within-group food competition is not thought to influence social organization for folivorous primates. Thus, they are expected by the socioecological model to live in large groups, exploiting benefits such as predation avoidance. However, many folivores form small groups, well below the threshold to avoid within-group scramble competition for food (the “folivore paradox”, Janson & Goldsmith, 1995). Social factors, rather than ecological factors, are thought to determine social organization for folivores, though this has rarely been systematically tested. In this study, the relative contribution of ecological versus social factors on the group size and composition of ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus ) at the Boabeng-Fiema Monkey Sanctuary, Ghana was explored.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Human Mating: Trade-Offs and Strategic Pluralism
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2000) 23, 573–644 Printed in the United States of America The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism Steven W. Gangestad Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 [email protected] Jeffry A. Simpson Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843 [email protected]. Abstract: During human evolutionary history, there were “trade-offs” between expending time and energy on child-rearing and mating, so both men and women evolved conditional mating strategies guided by cues signaling the circumstances. Many short-term matings might be successful for some men; others might try to find and keep a single mate, investing their effort in rearing her offspring. Recent evidence suggests that men with features signaling genetic benefits to offspring should be preferred by women as short-term mates, but there are trade-offs between a mate’s genetic fitness and his willingness to help in child-rearing. It is these circumstances and the cues that signal them that underlie the variation in short- and long-term mating strategies between and within the sexes. Keywords: conditional strategies; evolutionary psychology; fluctuating asymmetry; mating; reproductive strategies; sexual selection Research on interpersonal relationships, especially roman- attributes (e.g., physical attractiveness) tend to assume tic ones, has increased markedly in the last three decades greater importance in mating relationships than in other (see Berscheid & Reis 1998) across a variety of fields, in- types of relationships (Buss 1989; Gangestad & Buss 1993 cluding social psychology, anthropology, ethology, sociol- [see also Kenrick & Keefe: “Age Preferences in Mates Re- ogy, developmental psychology, and personology (Ber- flect Sex Differences in Human Reproductive Strategies” scheid 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • What, If Anything, Is a Darwinian Anthropology?
    JONATHAN MARKS What, if anything, is a Darwinian anthropology? Not too many years ago, I was scanning the job advertisements in anthropology and stumbled upon one for a faculty post in a fairly distinguished department in California. The ad specified that they were looking for someone who ‘studied culture from an evolutionary perspective’. I was struck by that, because it seemed to me that the alternative would be a creationist perspective, and I had never heard of anyone in this century who did that. Obviously my initial reading was incorrect. That department specifically wanted someone with a particular methodological and ideo- logical orientation; ‘evolutionary perspective’ was there as a code for something else. It has fascinated me for a number of years that Darwin stands as a very powerful symbol in biology. On the one hand, he represents the progressive aspect of science in its perpetual struggle against the perceived oppressive forces of Christianity (Larson 1997); and on the other, he represents as well the prevailing stodgy and stultified scientific orthodoxy against which any new bold and original theory must cast itself (Gould 1980). Proponents of the neutral theory (King and Jukes 1969) or of punctuated equilibria (Eldredge 1985) represented themselves as Darwinists to the outside worlds, and as anti-Darwinists to the inside world. Thus, Darwinism can be both the new and improved ideology you should bring home today, and is also the superseded Brand X ideology. That is indeed a powerful metaphor, to represent something as well as its opposite. Curiously, nobody ever told me in my scientific training that scientific progress was somehow predicated on the development of powerful metaphors.
    [Show full text]
  • Integrating Behaviour Into Wildlife Conservation: the Multiple Ways That Behaviour Can Reduce Ne Laura L
    Biological Conservation 95 (2000) 303±315 www.elsevier.com/locate/biocon Integrating behaviour into wildlife conservation: the multiple ways that behaviour can reduce Ne Laura L. Anthony a, Daniel T. Blumstein a,b,c,* aDepartment of Biological Sciences, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia bDepartment of Psychology, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia cThe Cooperative Research Centre for the Conservation and Management of Marsupials, Macquarie University, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia Received 10 June 1999; received in revised form 19 August 1999; accepted 16 October 1999 Abstract There has been a recent interest in integrating an understanding of behaviour into conservation biology. Unfortunately, there has been no paradigm for such a process. Without a clear framework for integration, conservation biologists may have diculties recognising how behavioural knowledge can help solve real-world conservation problems. Eective population size (Ne) is a key demographic parameter used to understand population viability. A variety of behaviours and behavioural traits impact Ne, yet their importance for conservation is under-appreciated. We suggest that identifying behavioural traits that aect Ne provides a paradigm for integrating behavioural biology into conservation biology. Behaviour can aect Ne through at least three dierent mechanisms: reducing N Ð the population size; reducing r Ð the population growth rate, and/or by increasing reproductive skew. We discuss how nine common behavioural traits can reduce Ne, and suggest how an understanding of these traits may inform management of both free-living and captive animals. # 2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Behaviour and conservation; Eective population size; Population viability 1. Introduction knowledge of animal behaviour to conservation pro- blems, there is no clear framework to help conservation Conservation biology is a crisis discipline aimed at biologists identify the speci®c cases when they should be saving biodiversity (Soule , 1986).
    [Show full text]
  • Predictors of Juvenile Survival in Birds
    Ornithological Monographs Volume (2013), No. 78, 1–55 © The American Ornithologists’ Union, 2013. Printed in USA. PREDICTORS OF JUVENILE SURVIVAL IN BIRDS TERRI J. MANESS1,2,3 AND DAVID J. ANDERSON2 1School of Biological Sciences, Louisiana Tech University, Ruston, Louisiana 71272, USA; and 2Department of Biology, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, North Carolina 27106, USA ABSTRACT.—The survival probability of birds during the juvenile period, between the end of parental care and adulthood, is highly variable and has a major effect on population dynamics and parental fitness. As such, a large number of studies have attempted to evaluate potential predictors of juvenile survival in birds, especially predictors related to parental care. Lack’s hypothesis linking body reserves accumulated from parental care to the survival of naive juveniles has organized much of this research, but various other predictors have also been investigated and received some support. We reviewed the literature in this area and identified a variety of methodological problems that obscure interpretation of the body of results. Most studies adopted statistical techniques that missed the opportunities to (1) evaluate the relative importance of several predictors, (2) control the confounding effect of correlation among predictor variables, and (3) exploit the information content of collinearity by evaluating indirect (via correlation) as well as direct effects of potential predictors on juvenile survival. Ultimately, we concluded that too few reliable studies exist to allow robust evaluations of any hypothesis regarding juvenile survival in birds. We used path analysis to test potential predictors of juvenile survival of 2,631 offspring from seven annual cohorts of a seabird, the Nazca Booby (Sula granti).
    [Show full text]
  • Colobus Vellerosus): Influence of Age, Rank and Contact with Other Groups on Dispersal Decisions
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by DukeSpace Dispersal in male ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus): influence of age, rank and contact with other groups on dispersal decisions Julie A. Teichroeb1), Eva C. Wikberg & Pascale Sicotte (Department of Anthropology, University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive N.W., Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4) (Accepted: 29 April 2011) Summary Dispersal is male-biased in ursine colobus monkeys (Colobus vellerosus), although female dispersal also occurs (Teichroeb et al., 2009). Here we describe the process of male disper- sal and its connection with between-group encounters (BGEs, N = 444) and male incur- sions (when males left their group and approached within 50 m of another group; N = 128) at the Boabeng-Fiema Monkey Sanctuary in central Ghana. Through BGEs and incursions, particularly those with non-aggressive interactions between individuals in different groups (BGEs, N = 17; incursions, N = 4), males could probably assess other groups for disper- sal opportunities. There was a trend for males to perform incursions more frequently before emigrating voluntarily than involuntarily. Incursions were often performed towards the group that the male eventually transferred to. Incursions by alpha males were temporally shorter and more aggressive than those by non-alpha males. We suggest that non-alpha males used incursions to assess other groups for breeding or dispersal opportunities, whereas alpha males performed incursions mainly to convey information about their quality to neighbouring males and females. Male emigrations/disappearances (natal N = 20, secondary N = 43, unknown N = 9) and immigrations (N = 62) were recorded for seven groups during ten years (2000– 2010).
    [Show full text]
  • Reproductive Ecology & Sexual Selection
    Reproductive Ecology & Sexual Selection REPRODUCTIVE ECOLOGY REPRODUCTION & SEXUAL SELECTION • Asexual • Sexual – Attraction, Courtship, and Mating – Fertilization – Production of Young The Evolutionary Enigma of Benefits of Asex Sexual Reproduction • Sexual reproduction produces fewer reproductive offspring than asexual reproduction, a so-called reproductive handicap 1. Eliminate problem to locate, court, & retain suitable mate. Asexual reproduction Sexual reproduction Generation 1 2. Doubles population growth rate. Female Female 3. Avoid “cost of meiosis”: Generation 2 – genetic representation in later generations isn't reduced by half each time Male 4. Preserve gene pool adapted to local Generation 3 conditions. Generation 4 Figure 23.16 The Energetic Costs of Sexual Reproduction Benefits of Sex • Allocation of Resources 1. Reinforcement of social structure 2. Variability in face of changing environment. – why buy four lottery tickets w/ the same number on them? Relative benefits: Support from organisms both asexual in constant & sexual in changing environments – aphids have wingless female clones & winged male & female dispersers – ciliates conjugate if environment is deteriorating Heyer 1 Reproductive Ecology & Sexual Selection Simultaneous Hermaphrodites TWO SEXES • Advantageous if limited mobility and sperm dispersal and/or low population density • Guarantee that any member of your species encountered is the • Conjugation “right” sex • Self fertilization still provides some genetic variation – Ciliate protozoans with + & - mating
    [Show full text]