Historical Thinking As a Tool for Theoretical Psychology on Objectivity Thomas Teo
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Chapter 11: Behaviorism
Chapter 11: Behaviorism Megan Davis, Nicole Pelley and Stephanie Quinlan Behaviorism (1892-1956) ● Psychology has been the study of the mind since the Greeks ○ The definition of the mind has been debated extensively ○ 20th century: Shift from what the mind was to what it did ■ Mind causes behavior ● New field of research ○ Psychology was redefined with help from animal psychology ○ People started believing humans evolved from animal forms ○ Had to rethink Descartes’ definition of the mind New Directions in Animal Psychology New Directions in Animal Psychology Animal psychology as Romanes begun it, used 2 methods: 1. Anecdotal Method → Collect data 2. Method of Inference → Interpret data Close examination in late 19th, early 20th century. Anecdote → Experiment From Anecdote to Experiment ● Experiment replaced anecdotes and informal, naturalistic experiments ● Aim of animal psychology - produce natural science and anecdote not the path to science ● Two important research programs: ○ Thorndike ○ Pavlov From Anecdote to Experiment Edward Lee Thorndike (1874-1949): ● Initially wanted to study children ● Not many readily available, took up animals ● Studied with William James ● Developed “connectionism” ○ Methodological and theoretical approach to animal learning ○ Formulation of an S-R psychology he called “connectionism” ○ Anecdotal method overestimated animal intelligence From Anecdote to Experiment Thorndike’s Puzzle Boxes ● Trap cat inside box ● Each box opened by cat in different way ● Rewarded with salmon for escaping ○ Ex. of instrumental -
An Analysis of Existential Psychology Arthur Erwin Wolfgarth University of Nebraska at Omaha
University of Nebraska at Omaha DigitalCommons@UNO Student Work 6-1961 An analysis of existential psychology Arthur Erwin Wolfgarth University of Nebraska at Omaha Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/studentwork Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Wolfgarth, Arthur Erwin, "An analysis of existential psychology" (1961). Student Work. 125. https://digitalcommons.unomaha.edu/studentwork/125 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by DigitalCommons@UNO. It has been accepted for inclusion in Student Work by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@UNO. For more information, please contact [email protected]. AN ANALYSIS OF EXISTENTIAL. PSYCHOLOGY by Arthur Erwin Wolfgarth A Thesis Presented to the Graduate Faoulty of the Department of Psychology University of Omaha In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts June 1961 UMI Number: EP72773 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dtsssrtslioni Publishing UMI EP72773 Published by ProQuest LLC (2015). Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 PREFACE The purpose of this thesis is to trace the thread of existential psychological thought from its first system atic statements in Denmark through its expansion in Europe to its influence in contemporary thought in the United States, This study begins with S^ren Kierkegaard who cast existential expression into molds that have not broken to the present day. -
Agnotology in the Dialectics of the History and Philosophy of Psychology
23610.1177/0959354313504838Theory & PsychologyTeo TAP 53 04838 Comment Theory & Psychology 23(6) 840–851 Agnotology in the dialectics © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: of the history and philosophy sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0959354313504838 of psychology tap.sagepub.com Thomas Teo York University Abstract It is suggested that Robinson’s (2013) arguments are based on a less than clear articulation of the relationship between the history and philosophy of psychology. After tackling the relationship between these two subdisciplines and conceptualizing them in a dialectic relationship from a programmatic point of view, consequences for writing and reading works in the history and philosophy of psychology are examined. Lessons learned from the reflections suggest that there are inherent conflicts between the two subdisciplines, that the history of psychology needs to reflect on its implicit assumptions as well as on relevance, and that the substance of philosophical psychology can be improved when an historical perspective is taken. Consequences for a critical history and philosophy of psychology are discussed. Keywords history, philosophy, pilosophy of science, theory Agnotology, the study of ignorance-making (Proctor & Schiebinger, 2008), could be a starting point for a response to Robinson’s (2013) concerns about ignorance. However, Robinson’s arguments are reasonable and I actually do not disagree with many of his remarks. Yet, I suggest that an analysis of the relationship between the history and phi- losophy of psychology clarifies some misunderstandings or misgivings. In approaching my own argument, I could choose an historical approach that reconstructs the trajectory of the history of psychology and the philosophy of psychology as differing thought styles and thought collectives (Fleck, 1935/1979); or I could tackle the problem philosophi- cally/systematically (theoretically) neglecting history. -
Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books 0010-7549 February 2000, Vol
Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books 0010-7549 February 2000, Vol. 45, No. 1, Pg. 60 - 62 History Is Beautiful! Review of: Psychology: Theoretical-Historical Perspectives. (1998) Robert W. Rieber, Kurt Salzinger Washington, DC: American Psychological Association ISBN: 1-55798-524-3, 509 pp. $39.95 (member); $49.95 (nonmember) Thomas Teo About the author(s): ROBERT W. RIEBER, Department of Psychology, John Jay College of Criminal Justice, and the Graduate Center, City University of New York. KURT SALZINGER, Department of Psychology, Hofstra University. THOMAS TEO, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M3J 1P3. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected]. It is not surprising that in the areas of the history and theory of psychology one finds as many different positions, interests, and perspectives as there exist in other fields of psychology. However, most historians of psychology are united by attributing significance to the past as a source for understanding the form and content of contemporary psychology. And most theoreticians, or rather metatheoreticians, are united by the conviction that metatheory and theory play guiding roles in research. But what is the relationship between theoretical and historical psychology? This question is answered differently by insiders. Some believe that one can study metatheory without including history, whereas others argue that metatheory without historical knowledge is shortsighted. History, on the other hand, does not necessarily require metatheory, but if historians of psychology wish to study more than chronological data, then they must certainly learn from and about metatheory. In a Kantian mode one might argue that metatheory without history is blind, and history without metatheory is vain. -
Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology
Points of View in the Modern History of Psychology Edited by Claude E. Buxton Department of Psychology Yale University New Haven, Connecticut 1985 ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers) Orlando San Diego New York London Toronto Montreal Sydney Tokyo Passages from the following are reprinted by permission of the publishers: Newell, Α., Duncker on Thinking, in S. Koch & D. Leary (Eds.), A Century of Psychology as Science. Copyright 1985 by McGraw-Hill. Neisser, U., Cognitive Psychology. © 1967 by Prentice-Hall. COPYRIGHT © 1985 BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Orlando, Florida 32887 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24-28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX LIBRARY OF CONGRESS CATALOGING IN PUBLICATION DATA Main entry under title: Points of view in the modern history of psychology. Includes indexes. 1. Psychology— History. I. Buxton, Claude E. BF81.P57 1985 150\9 85-4010 ISBN 0-12-148510-2 (alk. paper) PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 85 86 87 88 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Mitchell G. Ash (295), Department of History, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242 William Bevan (259), John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation, Chicago, Illinois 60603 Arthur L. Blumenthal (19, 51), Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts at Boston, Boston, Massachusetts 02125 Claude E. -
Psychology Old and New
University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons IRCS Technical Reports Series Institute for Research in Cognitive Science 1-1-2001 Psychology Old and New Gary Hatfield University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports Part of the Psychology Commons Hatfield, Gary, "Psychology Old and New" (2001). IRCS Technical Reports Series. 23. https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports/23 University of Pennsylvania Institute for Research in Cognitive Science Technical Report No. IRCS-01-07. This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/ircs_reports/23 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Psychology Old and New Abstract Psychology as the study of mind was an established subject matter throughout the nineteenth century in Britain, Germany, France, and the United States, taught in colleges and universities and made the subject of books and treatises. During the period 1870-1914 this existing discipline of psychology was being transformed into a new, experimental science, especially in Germany and the United States. The increase in experimentation changed the body of psychological writing, although there remained considerable continuity in theoretical content and non-experimental methodology between the old and new psychologies. This paper follows the emergence of the new psychology out of the old in the national traditions of Britain (primarily England), Germany, and the United States, with some reference to French, Belgian, Austrian, and Italian thinkers. The final section considers some methodological and philosophical issues in these literatures. Disciplines Psychology Comments University of Pennsylvania Institute for Research in Cognitive Science Technical Report No. IRCS-01-07. -
The Center for Advanced Study in Theoretical Psychology
Recovery and Renewal: The Center for Advanced Study in Theoretical Psychology By: Cor Baerveldt, Evan Shillabeer, and Vickie Richard Culture in Action Lab, University of Alberta Summary The Center for Advanced Study in Theoretical Psychology was a world-renowned institute at the University of Alberta in the period 1965-1990, dedicated to conceptual analysis and integration in psychology and the training of new generations of ‘generalist’ psychologists. In the period of its active operation the Center brought together over 120 distinguished visiting scholars from a range of disciplinary backgrounds, including several Nobel laureates. Many of those visiting scholars remained in residence in order to engage in a series of so- called Seminars-in-the-Round that involved intense mutual exchange of ideas and no-holds barred verbal involvement. Recently recovered thought-to-be-lost audio and video recordings from this period offer a unique and multifaceted picture of a discipline that prides itself for its empirical approach, but seems to lose itself in narrow specialization and a lack of articulation of what constitutes the psychological. More than a thousand hours of reel-to-reel audio recordings, as well as a selection of U-matic video recordings have been recovered up till now. Our Culture in Action lab at the University of Alberta has started the process of digitizing those materials and will create a repository in order to make them available for the international academic community. Together with colleagues Dr. Leo Mos (former director of the Center) and Mike Dawson, we are actively seeking collaboration with scholars interested in the history and theory of the behavioural, social and human sciences, in the conceptual, epistemological and ontological basis of psychology and in the discursive processes that constitute science-in- action. -
What Is Epistemological Violence in the Empirical Social Sciences? Thomas Teo* York University
Social and Personality Psychology Compass 4/5 (2010): 295–303, 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00265.x What is Epistemological Violence in the Empirical Social Sciences? Thomas Teo* York University Abstract This article discusses the meaning of epistemological violence in the empirical social sciences. It is argued that the concept is closer to personal than to structural violence in that it has a subject, an object, and an action, even if the violence is indirect and nonphysical: the subject of violence is the researcher, the object is the Other, and the action is the interpretation of data that is presented as knowledge. Using a hypothetical example, the problem of interpretation in empirical research on the Other is discussed. Epistemological violence refers to the interpretation of social-scientific data on the Other and is produced when empirical data are interpreted as showing the inferiority of or problematizes the Other, even when data allow for equally viable alternative interpretations. Interpretations of inferiority or problematizations are understood as actions that have a negative impact on the Other. Because the interpretations of data emerge from an academic context and thus are presented as knowledge, they are defined as epistemologically violent actions. Problems, consequences, and practices surrounding this concept are discussed. Introduction Forty-one years ago, Galtung (1969) encouraged the conceptualization of ‘theoretically significant dimensions of violence that can lead thinking, research, and potentially, action, toward the most important problems’ (p. 168). Following this maxim, he developed the now famous distinction between personal and structural violence, arguing convincingly that structures such as social injustice can be understood as violence. -
Cognition and Emotion: from Order to Disorder [Book Review]
Downloaded by [New York University] at 13:13 12 August 2016 COGNITION AND EMOTION This fully updated edition of the highly praised Cognition and Emotion provides a comprehensive overview of contemporary research and traditional philosophical perspectives on both normal emotional experience and emotional disorders. Uniquely, this book integrates two areas which are often treated separately: the main theories of normal emotions and theories of emotional disorders (e.g. depression, post-traumatic stress disorder and phobias). It draws these strands together with a theoretical framework which can be applied to both normal and disordered emotions. The authors also provide a comprehensive review of the literature on cognition and emotion, including coverage of the historical background and philosophy of emotion, and research on the five basic emotions of fear, anger, sadness, disgust and happiness. Cognition and Emotion provides a novel approach with a range of implications for clinical practice. It will be essential reading for undergraduate and postgraduate courses on emotion. Mick Power is a Professor of Clinical Psychology at the National University of Downloaded by [New York University] at 13:13 12 August 2016 Singapore. In the past he has worked at universities and hospitals in London, Edinburgh, Norway, Italy, China and New Zealand. He has worked for the Medical Research Council and for many years has been a Research Advisor with the World Health Organization. Tim Dalgleish is a Senior Research Scientist and practising clinical psychologist at the Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit in Cambridge. His main research interests include psychological reactions to trauma and cognition– emotion relations in the emotional disorders. -
Theory & Psychology
Theory & Psychology http://tap.sagepub.com Emotion, Meaning, and Appraisal Theory Michael McEachrane Theory Psychology 2009; 19; 33 DOI: 10.1177/0959354308101418 The online version of this article can be found at: http://tap.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/19/1/33 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com Additional services and information for Theory & Psychology can be found at: Email Alerts: http://tap.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://tap.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav Citations http://tap.sagepub.com/cgi/content/refs/19/1/33 Downloaded from http://tap.sagepub.com at UNIV MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST on March 3, 2009 Emotion, Meaning, and Appraisal Theory Michael McEachrane UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS, AMHERST ABSTRACT. According to psychological emotion theories referred to as appraisal theory, emotions are caused by appraisals (evaluative judgments). Borrowing a term from Jan Smedslund, it is the contention of this article that psychological appraisal theory is “pseudoempirical” (i.e., misleadingly or incorrectly empiri- cal). In the article I outline what makes some scientific psychology “pseudoem- pirical,” distinguish my view on this from Jan Smedslund’s, and then go on to show why paying heed to the ordinary meanings of emotion terms is relevant to psychology, and how appraisal theory is methodologically off the mark by employing experiments, questionnaires, and the like, to investigate what follows from the ordinary meanings of words. The overarching argument of the article is that the scientific research program of appraisal theory is fundamentally mis- guided and that a more philosophical approach is needed to address the kinds of questions it seeks to answer. -
(1988). Under What Conditions Can Theoretical Psychology Survive And
hychotogiel Rsiw cqright reE8 by the Amqim ***,ffi9_'d;:SA{ffdJi"j 1988. Vol. 95, No. 4, 559-565 Under What Conditions Can Theoretical Psychology Survive and Prosper? Integrating the Rational and Empirical Epistemologies DonaldG. MacKay University of California, Los Angeles This article reexamines some important issues raised by Greenwald, Pratkanis, Leippe, and Baum- gardner (1986) conceming the nature oftheory and its role in research progress, practical applica- tions of psychological knowledge, strategies for develqing and evaluating theories, and relations between empirical and theoretical psychology. I argue that Greenwald et al.'s result+entered meth- ods will not solve problems such as confirmation bias and irreplicability and will aggravate other existing problems: lack of viable theory, fragmentation of the field, mechanical fact gathering, lim- ited applicability of psychological knowledge, and noncumulative develryment of facts, with need- less duplication ofresults and reinvention ofempirical constructs. I conclude that all ofthese prob- lems are best solved by establishing a balance between the "rational" and "empirical" epistemologies in psychology. We are currently witnessing an upsurge ofinterest in theoreti- approach ofthe rational epistemology is to develop theories that cal psychology (see e.g., Hyland, l98l), and detailed and funda- explain available facts, facilitate practical applications, and pre- mental theories are under construction, especially in cognitive dict new facts for future test. This "surface definition" ofap- psychology, currently one ofthe most advanced areas ofpsycho- proaches is deceptive, howeve4 because ofhidden differences in logical theorizing (Mandle6 1985; Royce, 1978). Royce (1984) how the two epistemologies interpret seemingly straightforward has complained, however, that in general, "psychological theo- conc€pts such as "practical application," "fact," and "theory." ries do not mesh well with the relevant data. -
51 Annual Meeting Macewan University (Edmonton, AB
Cheiron 2019 1 51st Annual Meeting MacEwan University (Edmonton, AB, CA) June 20‐23, 2019 Conference Program and Abstracts Cheiron 2019 2 Acknowledgments We would like to thank the following people for their much appreciated support and assistance in planning our annual meeting this year: Cheiron Executive Officers: David R. Robinson, Executive Officer David C. Devonis, Treasurer Michael Dawson, Communications Cheiron Review Committee Members: Rémy Amouroux; Nancy Digdon; Kim M. Hajek; Alan Tjeltveit; Phyllis Wentworth; Leila Zenderland. Cheiron Book Prize Committee Members: Daniela Barberis, Kate Harper, Fred Weizmann, and Leila Zenderland (Chair). Two most recent past program chairs: Robert Kugelmann & Jacy Young. Proposal Reviewers: Each conference proposal was vetted by at least two anonymous reviewers, who provided valuable comments and recommendations. Session Chairs: Katalin Dzinas; Christopher D. Green; Kim M. Hajek; Robert Kugelmann; Katharine Milar; David R. Robinson; Henderikus Stam; Alan Tjeltveit; Nadine Weidman; Leila Zenderland. Local Helpers: We are grateful to many people at MacEwan who supported this conference: Melike Schalomon & Rob Wiznura from FAS Dean’s Office; Susan Cooper, Kristine Monteiro & Jen Brunsch from Conference and Event Planning; Terri Suntjens, Krista Hanscomb & Ashley Albert‐Hunter from kihêw waciston Indigenous Centre; Cynthia Zutter, Lyndsey Drozak & Bryce Wicks from Research Office; Aimee Skye & Psychology Department; Diana Porter from Residence; Marija Vukusic from M‐Store; Jackie Paley from Communications; Provost Craig Monk; President Deb Saucier and Ray Baril on behalf of General Faculty Council; and student volunteers: Nour Al Adhban, Stephanie Belland, Leanne Buttery, Kennedy Garrett, Nick Hemmings, Mustafa Janoowalla, Hadla Omar, Geoff Rachor, Abby Riehl & Jessie Swanek. We very much appreciate the financial support we received from a SSHRC Exchange Grant and from Conference and Event Planning, who gave us an affiliate discount.