Which Creates the Bigger Halo: Cause-Related Marketing Or Cause Sponsorship?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Which Creates the Bigger Halo: Cause-Related Marketing Or Cause Sponsorship? WHICH CREATES THE BIGGER HALO: CAUSE-RELATED MARKETING OR CAUSE SPONSORSHIP? BY AMY ELIZABETH RESTKO THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Advertising in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2014 Urbana, Illinois Adviser: Associate Professor Michelle R. Nelson ABSTRACT Corporate social responsibility (CSR) used to act as a set of rules for companies to abide by, but it has evolved into a central part of the business strategy (Keys et al., 2009). CSR practices have proven to greatly enhance the company’s reputation and brand. This study tests the benefits of a positive halo effect from CSR campaigns and explores the role of involvement in the relationship. A halo effect can be described as a cognitive bias in which one trait can positively affect the subsequent perceptions of a brand (Madden et al., 2012), and this study defines involvement through personal relevance. While there has been research on the positive brand effects produced by CSR, there is limited research into how consumer involvement in terms of personal relevance to the CSR campaigns can contribute to the extent of the halo effects. The present study examined to what extent two types of CSR (cause-related marketing; CRM and cause sponsorship; CS) versus a control group, create an overall favorable impression of the brand. Specifically, a 3 x 1 experiment was conducted with university students to see if a CS, CRM or control (no CSR) Facebook page for a fictitious café brand influenced liking of the brand, quality and other brand attributes, CSR, willingness to recommend, behavior, and behavioral intent. Based on halo effects and involvement, it was expected that the CSR campaigns would have a larger effect on the dependent variables. It was further hypothesized that CRM would create a bigger halo than CS and the control, and that involvement—in terms of personal relevance—would moderate the positive effects of that halo. In other words, if CRM induces greater message involvement, that involvement should heighten the positive effects of CSR. Results of the experimental study revealed that no halo effects were found on brand attribute ratings; however, both CSR campaign messages (CRM and CS) resulted in higher overall CSR ratings for the brand. Further, ii participants who view a CS campaign are more willing to recommend the Facebook page of the restaurant brand than the CRM group, and participants in the CS group are more likely to visit the restaurant. Both theoretical and practical implications are discussed and further research is recommended. iii For all who pushed me to set big goals and kept me on track to accomplish them. Special thanks to Michelle R. Nelson Brittany Duff John Wirtz And My colleagues in the Department of Advertising at the University of Illinois. iv ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This master’s thesis would have been impossible without the support of the faculty and my colleagues in the Department of Advertising at the University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign. Special thanks especially to my adviser, Dr. Michelle R. Nelson, who believed in me and encouraged me to pursue what I believe in—even during crunch time! I would like to thank my committee members, Dr. Brittany Duff and Dr. John Wirtz, who offered support and guidance and encouraged me to stay focused. Special thanks to Dr. Patrick Vargas for all of his wisdom and teachings in interpreting data. Thank you to Dr. Kevin Wise for pushing me to pursue a thesis and having confidence that I could finish it. I would like to also thank my advertising colleagues, the “adgrads” who will shape the future of advertising; especially Christoph Goeken, Nate Hartman, Quinn (Xuan) Qin, and Veronica Vorobyova who inspired me to develop my ideas and spent countless hours discussing theories and studies while choosing a topic. A special thank you to my wonderful friends for supporting me and encouraging me to enjoy the thesis process. Finally, a huge thank you to my family—Mom, Dad, Alli, and Mike, and my dog Sebastian who provided me with necessary stress relief—who supported me with love throughout the length of an extended academic career. v TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION…………………………………………………………………1 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW……………………………………………………….....6 2.1 Corporate Social Responsibility………………………………………………………………6 2.2 CSR Goes Digital…………………………………………………………………………….10 2.3 Involvement………………………………………………………………………………….12 2.4 Halo Effects………………………………………………………………………………….14 CHAPTER 3: HYPOTHESES…………………………………………………………………...19 3.1 Liking of the Brand ………………………………………………………………………….19 3.2 Behavior and Behavioral Intent...……………………………………………………………19 3.3 Brand Attribute Ratings ……………………………………………………………………..20 3.4 CSR Ratings …………………………………………………………………………………20 3.5 Willingness to Recommend …………………………………………………………………20 3.6 Halo Effects …………………………………………………………………………………21 3.7 Involvement …………………………………………………………………………………21 CHAPTER 4: METHOD………………………………………………………………………...22 4.1 Overview……………………………………………………………………………………..22 4.2 Participants…………………………………………………………………………………...22 4.3 Stimuli Development and Manipulation……………………………………………………..23 4.4 Main Experiment…………………………………………………………………………….25 4.5 Measurement…………………………………………………………………………………26 CHAPTER 5: RESULTS………………………………………………………………………...31 5.1 Manipulation Checks………………………………………………………………………...31 vi 5.2 Testing Hypotheses…………………………………………………………………………..31 5.3 Additional Measures – Thought-listing……………………………………………………...37 CHAPTER 6: DISCUSSION……………………………………………………………………38 6.1 Theoretical Implications……………………………………………………………………..39 6.2 Practical Implications………………………………………………………………………..40 6.3 Limitations and Future Research…………………………………………………………….40 CHAPTER 7: REFERENCES…………………………………………………………………...44 TABLES AND FIGURES……………………………………………………………………….51 APPENDIX A: STIMULI 1: FACEBOOK PAGE FOR AROMA CAFE……………………...66 APPENDIX B: STIMULI 2: FACEBOOK PAGE FOR THE BREAD COMPANY…………..67 APPENDIX C: STIMULI 3: FACEBOOK PAGE FOR PAT’S BISTRO, CRM…………..…..68 APPENDIX D: STIMULI 4: FACEBOOK PAGE FOR PAT’S BISTRO, CS……………..…..69 APPENDIX E: STIMULI 5: FACEBOOK PAGE FOR PAT’S BISTRO, CONTROL………..70 APPENDIX F: STIMULI 6: PAT’S BISTRO FLYER AS BEHAVIOR MEASURE………….71 APPENDIX G: MAIN EXPERIMENT QUESTIONNAIRE……………………………………72 APPENDIX H: DEBRIEFING STATEMENT………………………………………………….87 vii CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Corporate social responsibility, or “actions that appear to further some social good, beyond the interests of the firm and that which is required by law” (McWilliams and Siegel, 2001, p. 117), used to act as a set of guidelines forced upon companies in order to stay out of trouble in the media and the law (Keys et al., 2009). Nowadays though, consumers are deliberately seeking out more socially responsible companies and are willing to pay a premium to support them. According to the 2013 Cone Research Report, 91% of global consumers are likely to switch brands to one associated with a good cause, given comparable price and quality, and 50% would be willing to reward a company by paying more for their goods and services. Additionally, 93% of consumers in the U.S. say that they have a more positive image, 90% are more likely to trust, and 90% are more loyal to companies that support a good cause (Cone, 2013). With consumer interest in brands supporting a good cause, brands are starting to take note and focus their marketing efforts on corporate social responsibility (hereafter CSR). McWilliams and Siegel (2001) define CSR as “actions that appear to further some social good, beyond the interests of the firm and that which is required by law” (p. 117). There are several forms of CSR that range from for-profit companies incorporating CSR initiatives into their mission statements, such as TOMS and their one-for-one mission (www.TOMS.com) or Microsoft’s commitment to corporate citizenship (http://www.microsoft.com/about/corporatecitizenship/en-us/) to short-term partnerships with charitable causes. Unilever recently began a new campaign called “Project Sunlight,” which says that “it is possible to build a world where everyone lives well and lives sustainably” by doing small actions everyday to make a difference (Neff, 2014). This campaign partners with Feeding 1 America to build consumer awareness of Unilever and their sustainability efforts, and Unilever provides donations to Feeding America (Neff, 2014). Unilever took “Project Sunlight” to television and paid media, but in this digital age, brands also recognize the need to employ their social responsibility efforts online. For example, Google began a campaign in November 2014 to help fight Ebola, where the company donates $2 for every $1 donated, on their homepage (google.com) and on their Facebook page (facebook.com/Google). Similar to Google, companies are utilizing their owned media to engage consumers with their CSR efforts. Embracing owned media can be much more cost-efficient than paid media and allows for increased speed, consumer engagement, and in some cases even reach on a larger scale (Chui et al., 2012). CSR also can enhance the corporate brand. Studies have shown that CSR can lead to an improvement in consumer beliefs, attitudes and intentions (Becker-Olsen et al., 2006), a positive effect on reputation (Arora and Henderson, 2007; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001), more favorable brand and product evaluations (Brown and Dacin, 1997; Ellen et al., 2000; Sen and Bhattacharya, 2001) and increased loyalty (Maignan et al., 1999). Studies have also demonstrated
Recommended publications
  • Perceptions of Teacher Expectations Among First and Second
    Digital Commons @ George Fox University Doctor of Education (EdD) Theses and Dissertations 12-1-2016 Student Voice: Perceptions of Teacher Expectations Among First and Second Generation Vietnamese and Mexican Students Sara Gandarilla George Fox University, [email protected] This research is a product of the Doctor of Education (EdD) program at George Fox University. Find out more about the program. Recommended Citation Gandarilla, Sara, "Student Voice: Perceptions of Teacher Expectations Among First and Second Generation Vietnamese and Mexican Students" (2016). Doctor of Education (EdD). 90. http://digitalcommons.georgefox.edu/edd/90 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Digital Commons @ George Fox University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctor of Education (EdD) by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ George Fox University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STUDENT VOICE: PERCEPTIONS OF TEACHER EXPECTATIONS AMONG FIRST AND SECOND GENERATION VIETNAMESE AND MEXICAN STUDENTS By SARA GANDARILLA FACULTY RESEARCH COMMITTEE: Chair: Terry Huffman, Ph.D. Members: Ginny Birky, Ph.D. and Tatiana Cevallos, Ed.D. Presented to the College of Education, George Fox University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Education December 7, 2016 ii ABSTRACT This qualitative research study explored the perceptions first and second generation Vietnamese and Mexican high school students hold on teacher expectations based on their racial identity. Specifically, this study explores the critical concepts of stereotype threat, halo effect, and self-fulfilling prophecy. The primary purpose of this investigation was to enhance the understanding of how the perception students have impacts success or lack of success for two different student groups.
    [Show full text]
  • Positive Stereotypes, Negative Outcomes: Reminders of the Positive Components of Complementary Gender Stereotypes Impair Perform
    1 British Journal of Social Psychology (2018) © 2018 The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com Positive stereotypes, negative outcomes: Reminders of the positive components of complementary gender stereotypes impair performance in counter-stereotypical tasks Rotem Kahalon1* , Nurit Shnabel1 and Julia C. Becker2 1Tel-Aviv University, Israel 2Osnabruck University, Germany Gender stereotypes are complementary: Women are perceived to be communal but not agentic, whereas men are perceived to be agentic but not communal. The present research tested whether exposure to reminders of the positive components of these gender stereotypes can lead to stereotype threat and subsequent performance deficits on the complementary dimension. Study 1 (N = 116 female participants) revealed that compared to a control/no-stereotype condition, exposure to reminders of the stereotype about women’s communality (but not to reminders of the stereotype about women’s beauty) impaired women’s math performance. In Study 2 (N = 86 male participants), reminders of the stereotype about men’s agency (vs. a control/no- stereotype condition) impaired men’s performance in a test of socio-emotional abilities. Consistent with previous research on stereotype threat, in both studies the effect was evident among participants with high domain identification. These findings extend our understanding of the potentially adverse implications of seemingly positive gender stereotypes. While the message that negative stereotypes are anti-egalitarian and socially unacceptable is reinforced in contemporary Western society, positive stereotypes are prevalent and considered socially acceptable (Czopp, Kay, & Cheryan, 2015). The present research tested whether, despite their subjectively positive tone, positive gender stereotypes might lead to negative outcomes. In particular, we examined whether highlighting the positive stereotypes about women’s communality and men’s agency can lead to stereotype threat effects.
    [Show full text]
  • Does Physical Attractiveness and Sex Impact Decisions in a Threat Detection Task
    DOES PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS AND SEX IMPACT DECISIONS IN A THREAT DETECTION TASK A research project submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE IN APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY At the UNIVERSITY OF CANTERBURY By JESSIE KENDALL UNIVERSITY OF CANTERBURY 2014 i ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would firstly like to thank Deak Helton for being an excellent and supportive supervisor. I am very grateful of your guidance and your efficient and thorough feedback that you have provided throughout the year. Also, to Kumar Yogeeswaran, my secondary supervisor, for your exceptional guidance and assistance. It has been a privilege to work with you both. A very big thank you also to Jon Wiltshire, who helped design and create the computer simulation game necessary for this research. I would also like to thank Christopher Burt for encouraging me to return to University to complete the second year of the APSY course after a break. 1 CONTENTS CONTENTS .............................................................................................................................................. 1 LIST OF TABLES .................................................................................................................................... 3 ABSTRACT .............................................................................................................................................. 4 SECTION I – INTRODUCTION .......................................................................................................... 5 1.1. STEREOTYPES
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • The Halo Effect in Sports
    International Journal of Business and Social Science Volume 9 • Number 2 • February 2018 The Halo Effect in Sports Prof. Dr. Gerd NUFER Natalie ALESI Reutlingen University ESB Business School Germany Abstract A halo effect can lead to significantly biased and distorted judgments in numerous situations and settings in daily life. However, its impact has barely been researched in the sporting environment, although it might help a great deal in understanding how sport fans think and behave. This paper provides an international literature review on the halo effect in different research fields. Built upon this state of the art, an empirical study based on two Ger- man soccer clubs, VfB Stuttgart and FC Bayern Munich, analyzes the presence of halo effects explained by social identity theory. The study shows that supporters rate aspects of the respective team, for example the president’s competency, more favorable than common sport spectators, and this effect even increases with a higher level of team identification. Keywords: halo effect, heuristic, cognitive bias, sports, sport management, sport marketing, soccer, football 1Introduction People make numerous judgments and decisions every day. These might be only small ones like deciding what to wear or which dish to choose, but also more relevant ones like what to study or which business strategy to follow. People usually take it for granted that their judgments and decisions are based on objective and logical thinking. The truth is, however, that human thinking is quite often unconsciously influenced by cognitive biases. Cognitive biases can be seen as errors in thinking that lead to distorted decisions and judgments (Kahneman, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Machine Understanding and Avoidance of Misunderstanding in Agent-Directed Simulation and in Emotional Intelligence
    Machine Understanding and Avoidance of Misunderstanding in Agent-directed Simulation and in Emotional Intelligence Tuncer Ören1, Mohammad Kazemifard2 and Levent Yilmaz3 1School of Electrical Eng. & Computer Science,University of Ottawa, 800 King Edward Ave.,Ottawa,ON,Canada 2Department of Computer Engineering, Razi University, Kermanshah, Iran 3Department of Computer Science and Software Engineering, Auburn University, Auburn, AL, U.S.A. Keywords: Agents with Emotional Intelligence, Emotional Intelligence Simulation, Machine Understanding, Avoidance of Misunderstanding. Abstract: Simulation is being applied in many very important projects and often it is a vitally important infrastructure for them. Several types of computational intelligence techniques have been part of the abilities of simula- tion. An important aspect of intelligence is the ability to understand. Agent-directed simulation (ADS) is a comprehensive paradigm to cover all aspects of synergy of software agents and simulation and our approach is to develop agents with understanding abilities. After a brief review of ADS, our paradigms of machine understanding is presented. The article clearly indicates types of misunderstandings that might occur. Our research plans are to avoid some of the misunderstandings which could occur and especially to have self- attesting abilities in our applications to document which types of misunderstandings are avoided. 1 INTRODUCTION After this introduction, we start with a concise review of our view of agent directed simulation Simulation is being applied in many very important which provides a comprehensive framework to projects and often it is a vitally important infrastruc- consider all aspects of the synergy of software ture for them. Several types of computational intelli- agents and simulation.
    [Show full text]
  • Omission Neglect and the Bias Blind Spot: Effects of the Self-Other Asymmetry in Susceptibility to Bias and Responsiveness to Debiasing
    Omission Neglect and the Bias Blind Spot: Effects of the Self-Other Asymmetry in Susceptibility to Bias and Responsiveness to Debiasing A dissertation submitted to the Division of Research and Advanced Studies of the University of Cincinnati In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY (PH.D.) IN THE DEPARTMENT OF MARKETING of the College of Business 2011 By Xiaoqi Han M.A., Marquette University Committee Chair: Frank R. Kardes, Ph.D. i ABSTRACT Omission Neglect and the Bias Blind Spot: Effects of the Self-Other Asymmetry in Susceptibility to Bias and Responsiveness to Debiasing By Xiaoqi Han Chair: Dr. Frank R. Kardes Research on the bias blind spot shows that people are less capable of identifying biases in their own judgment than in others. People generally believe that they are less susceptible to biases than their peers and average people. This dissertation investigates the self-other bias asymmetry with respect to omission neglect. Omission neglect refers to insensitivity to missing or unknown information. Results from bias blind spot research imply a potential relationship between the self-other asymmetry in bias belief and omission neglect. Research on bias correction suggests that people holding a higher degree of asymmetry in bias beliefs may be less likely to correct biases even when omissions are made noticeable. Ironically, consumers who believe that they are less vulnerable to omission neglect may be more susceptible to omission neglect. Consumers may be also less likely to correct their judgment even when omitted information is made noticeable. The goal of the dissertation is to develop debiasing techniques to debias omission neglect in order to improve consumer judgment and decision making.
    [Show full text]
  • Heuristics and Cognitive Biases in Decision Making Process
    Heuristics and Cognitive Biases in Decision Making Process Anna Marta Winniczuk Kozminski University [email protected] EXTENDED ABSTRACT The goal of this paper is to recognize the most commonly present heuristics and cognitive biases occurring during managerial decision making process. The study will use simulation game as a space to track how game participants are perceiving their biases and heuristics during the game. The purpose of the study is to identify the most commonly used cognitive biases and heuristics in decision making process and compare them to performance of the team and their decisions in the simulation game, which will give valuable insights that can improve game-based business learning. Decision making is one of the crucial processes occurring every day in every organization. The process of making decisions in business situations is influenced by a variety of different circumstances, thus the subject is widely discussed in both academic and professional publications. The subject of heuristics and cognitive biases was widely introduced into decision making science by David Kahneman and Tversky (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973) and furtherly researched by other authors(Baron & Hershey, 1988); Griffin, D., Gonzalez, R., & Varey, C., 2001). In 2011, David Kahneman, Dan Lovallo and Oliver Sibony in their article “The Big Idea: Before You Make That Big Decision…” published in Harvard Business Review listed a checklist for managers to avoid cognitive biases in their decision making process. The most common cognitive biases in managerial decisions are: confirmation bias, availability bias, anchoring, halo effect , overconfidence, disaster neglect and loss aversion (Kahneman & Sibony, 2011). Mentioned above biases are widely common in managerial decision making process and worth examining in an experimental learning environment of a simulation game.
    [Show full text]
  • Debiasing Halo Effect: Auditor Reputation and the Role of Auditing Learning on Financial Statements User Trust
    International Research Journal of Management, IT & Social Sciences Available online at https://sloap.org/journals/index.php/irjmis/ Vol. 7 No. 6, November 2020, pages: 33-41 ISSN: 2395-7492 https://doi.org/10.21744/irjmis.v7n6.1006 Debiasing Halo Effect: Auditor Reputation and the Role of Auditing Learning on Financial Statements User Trust Radian Sri Rama a Sutrisno T b Erwin Saraswati c Aulia Fuad Rahman d Article history: Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine the effect of auditor reputation (big Submitted: 09 July 2020 four and non-big four) and auditing learning (with audit learning and without Revised: 18 August 2020 audit learning) on the trust of users of financial statements. Testing the Accepted: 27 September 2020 influence of the auditor's reputation and learning will show a halo effect on users of financial statements. The research method used was an experiment. Participants were 102 students. Data analysis was performed with the two-way Anova test. The results showed a halo effect so that the auditor's reputation and Keywords: audit learning affected the users of financial statements. auditing learning; auditor reputation; financial statements; halo effect; International research journal of management, IT and social sciences © 2020. trust; This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Corresponding author: Radian Sri Rama, Accounting Department, Brawijaya University, Indonesia. Email address: [email protected] a Accounting Department, Brawijaya University, Indonesia b Accounting Department, Brawijaya University, Indonesia c Accounting Department, Brawijaya University, Indonesia d Accounting Department, Brawijaya University, Indonesia 33 34 ISSN: 2395-7492 1 Introduction User trust in financial statements is obtained based on confidence in quality audits (Rodgers et al., 2019).
    [Show full text]
  • Too Much Information Not Enough Meaning Need to Act Fast What
    T H E C O G N I T I V E B I A S C O D E X – Kognitive Verzerrungen We store memories differently based We notice things already primed in on how they were experienced Too Much What Should We memory or repeated often We reduce events and lists Tip of the tongue phenomenon Bizarre, funny, visually striking, or Information Remember? to their key elements Levels–of–processing effect anthropomorphic things stick out more than non-bizarre/unfunny things Absent–mindedness Next–in–line effect Part–set cueingSerial–position effect effect Google effect Testing effect Memory inhibition We discard specifics Recency effect Suffix effect Leveling and sharpening Primacy effect DurationModality neglect effect MisinformationList–length effect effect to form generalities Serial recall effect We notice when something has changed Availability heuristic Attentional bias Illusory truth effect Mere–exposure effect Context effect Cue–dependent forgetting Mood–congruent memory bias Frequency illusion Baader–Meinhof Phenomenon Peak–end rule Empathy gap Fading affect bias Omission bias Base rate fallacy Bizarreness effect Negativity bias Humor effect Von Restorff effect We edit and reinforce ImplicitStereotypical stereotypes bias Picture superiority effect Self–relevance effect Negativity bias some memories after the fact Implicit associationPrejudice Anchoring Conservatism Contrast effect Spacing effect Distinction bias Focusing effect Suggestibility Framing effect We are drawn to details False memory Money illusion Misattribution of memory Weber–Fechner law SourceCryptomnesia
    [Show full text]
  • Influence of Cognitive Biases in Distorting Decision Making and Leading to Critical Unfavorable Incidents
    Safety 2015, 1, 44-58; doi:10.3390/safety1010044 OPEN ACCESS safety ISSN 2313-576X www.mdpi.com/journal/safety Article Influence of Cognitive Biases in Distorting Decision Making and Leading to Critical Unfavorable Incidents Atsuo Murata 1,*, Tomoko Nakamura 1 and Waldemar Karwowski 2 1 Department of Intelligent Mechanical Systems, Graduate School of Natural Science and Technology, Okayama University, Okayama, 700-8530, Japan; E-Mail: [email protected] 2 Department of Industrial Engineering & Management Systems, University of Central Florida, Orlando, 32816-2993, USA; E-Mail: [email protected] * Author to whom correspondence should be addressed; E-Mail: [email protected]; Tel.: +81-86-251-8055; Fax: +81-86-251-8055. Academic Editor: Raphael Grzebieta Received: 4 August 2015 / Accepted: 3 November 2015 / Published: 11 November 2015 Abstract: On the basis of the analyses of past cases, we demonstrate how cognitive biases are ubiquitous in the process of incidents, crashes, collisions or disasters, as well as how they distort decision making and lead to undesirable outcomes. Five case studies were considered: a fire outbreak during cooking using an induction heating (IH) cooker, the KLM Flight 4805 crash, the Challenger space shuttle disaster, the collision between the Japanese Aegis-equipped destroyer “Atago” and a fishing boat and the Three Mile Island nuclear power plant meltdown. We demonstrate that heuristic-based biases, such as confirmation bias, groupthink and social loafing, overconfidence-based biases, such as the illusion of plan and control, and optimistic bias; framing biases majorly contributed to distorted decision making and eventually became the main cause of the incident, crash, collision or disaster.
    [Show full text]