The Functions of Sleep

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Functions of Sleep Vo l u me 2, Issue 3, 155–171. DOI:10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.3.155 Received date 3 May 2015, Accepted date 12 August 2015, Published date 24 August 2015 http://www.aimspress.com/ Review The Functions of Sleep Samson Z Assefa 1, *, Montserrat Diaz-Abad 1, Emerson M Wickwire 1, and Steven M Scharf 1 1 Sleep Disorders Center, Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care, Department of Medicine, University of Maryland, Baltimore, MD 21201 * Correspondence: E-mail: [email protected]; Tel: 410-706-4771; Fax: 410-706-0345 Abstract: Sleep is a ubiquitous component of animal life including birds and mammals. The exact function of sleep has been one of the mysteries of biology. A considerable number of theories have been put forward to explain the reason(s) for the necessity of sleep. To date, while a great deal is known about what happens when animals sleep, there is no definitive comprehensive explanation as to the reason that sleep is an inevitable part of animal functioning. It is well known that sleep is a homeostatically regulated body process, and that prolonged sleep deprivation is fatal in animals. In this paper, we present some of the theories as to the functions of sleep and provide a review of some hypotheses as to the overall physiologic function of sleep. To better understand the purpose for sleeping, we review the effects of sleep deprivation on physical, neurocognitive and psychic function. A better understanding of the purpose for sleeping will be a great advance in our understanding of the nature of the animal kingdom, including our own. Keywords: sleep; sleep function; sleep deprivation; sleep theories; sleep restoration Abbreviations: EEG–electroencephalogram; REM–Rapid eye movement; NREM–non rapid eye movement; BCE–Before the common Era; ICSD3–International classifications of sleep disorders third edition; SWS–slow wave sleep; Ag–Antigen; IgG–Immunoglobulin G; RNA–Ribonucleic Acid; BRFSS–Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System; CRP–C reactive protein; PTSD–posttraumatic stress disorder; PVT–psychomotor vigilance task; TIB–time in bed; SWA– Slow Wave Activity; h– hour(s) 1. Introduction All mammals and birds sleep, that is they assume a state of reduced perceptual engagement from the environment with elevated arousal threshold. Sleep is a neurologically dynamic behavior with physical quiescence, elevated arousal threshold and a state of rapid reversibility. Historically, sleep was sometimes considered to be a state between wakefulness and death. In fact, it could be 156 argued that comparison of sleep to a deathlike state, i.e. an absence of function, may have contributed to the delay in the modern scientific study of and understanding of sleep. Sleep patterns vary among species in often surprising ways in order to meet the particular life patterns of each species. Although “we all do it,” the actual overall function of sleep is as yet incompletely understood. The great sleep researcher and pioneer Allen Rechtschaffen noted that “if sleep does not serve an absolute vital function, then it is the biggest mistake the evolutionary process ever made.” [1]. There are many biologic functions associated with sleep, including those that lead to restoration of body, brain and neurocognition. In a later publication, Rechtschaffen reviewed a number of the then existing scientific ideas as to the importance of sleep [2]. He notes that sleep is ubiquitous in higher animals. On the surface, sleep appears to be maladaptive—sleeping humans, for example, desist from procreation, self-protection, and even “making a living.” There appears to be a homeostatic regulation of sleep and sleep loss is followed by rebound sleep. It has been shown that when totally deprived of sleep, rodents die. Further, “sleep” is not a single “state of being.” Sleep in mammals is actually organized in cycles composed of two fundamentally different “states of being:” namely Rapid Eye Movement (REM) and non-REM sleep (NREM). The latter is further subdivided into “deep” (slow wave) and “light” stages (Stages 1 and 2). These different sleep stages all have differing biologic manifestations, neurologically and physically. Specific types of brain activity distinguish them. On electroencephalogram (EEG) study, NREM sleep reveals local and global slow cortical oscillations of wave activity (stage 3) and sleep spindles (stage 2). In contrast, during REM sleep the EEG patterns of the brain bear a resemblance to those in waking with associated eye movement in phasic REM stage, and without eye movement in tonic REM sleep [3]. Circadian and homeostatic processes regulate sleep [4]. This is often referred to as the “two- process model” for determination of the state of alertness. Circadian regulation is demonstrated by the maintenance of a 24-hour rhythm cycle in sleep propensity that is independent of prior sleep. The natural cycle of the brain is slightly longer than 24 hours, but it is “resynchronized” each day by the advent of blue light. Thus, normal circadian rhythmicity assures that humans are awake in the daylight (mostly morning) and ready for sleep in the night. Secretion of the hormone melatonin at night by the pineal gland in the brain is one important mechanism assuring the function of the “biologic clock.” Homeostatic regulation is reflected by rise in sleep propensity during waking and its dissipation during sleep. The interaction between circadian driven sleepiness-alertness cycles, and homeostatic (sleep deprivation or excess of wakefulness) is what determines the state of alertness- sleepiness. As will be reviewed in this article, sleep deprivation has many and varied adverse effects on body, mind and psyche. However, one cannot help but wonder as to why the creator (through evolution) saw fit to design animals with the necessity of a period of down time. After all, man’s mechanical contrivances do not usually need to be taken out of commission for daily servicing. For example, for an automobile, a tune-up every 3000 miles or so does quite well. For the purposes of this review, we have taken the view that we can learn about the “function” of sleep by learning about the pathophysiological consequences of sleep deprivation. We have briefly reviewed some of the historical notions about the function of sleep, and have concentrated most of this review on what is known in the scientific literature about the effects of sleep deprivation in humans on physical, mental and psychic restoration. AIMS Neuroscience Volume 2, Issue 3, 155-171. 157 2. Sleep Throughout History Sleep was in fact well recognized by the ancients, and even revered as a state in which human consciousness was open to inputs (supernatural) to which it is not normally receptive. In Genesis 28, verses 11 ff, God describes his plans for the Hebrew Patriarch Jacob while the Patriarch was asleep: “And he lighted upon the place, and tarried there all night, because the sun was set; and he took one of the stones of the place, and put it under his head, and lay down in that place to sleep. And he dreamed, and behold a ladder set up on the earth, and the top of it reached to heaven; and behold the angels of God ascending and descending on it. And, behold, the LORD stood beside him, and said: 'I am the LORD, the God of Abraham thy father, and the God of Isaac. The land whereon thou liest, to thee will I give it, and to thy seed.” (Genesis 28, verses 11–13) Dream interpretation was a major activity among ancient holy men, as well as modern psychotherapists. The ancient Egyptians constructed temples for the goddess Isis where congregants and priests would gather to perform dream interpretations. Note the importance of dream interpretation for the “career” of Joseph in ancient Egypt. Joseph’s knack for interpreting Pharaoh’s recurring dreams enabled Joseph’s rise to power from slave to vice regent of the ancient Egyptian kingdom. The ancient Greeks recognized that sleep was not only a portal for divine, or sometimes demonic, interface with man, but of great physiologic importance to well-being. In approximately 350 BCE, Aristotle, in a surprising “modern” evaluation of sleep reported that: “It is inevitable that every creature which wakes must also be capable of sleeping, since it is impossible that it should continue actualizing its powers perpetually. So, also, it is impossible for any animals to continue always sleeping.” [5] Throughout most of man’s history human sleep was considered a “state” in which man was open to communication to deities and demons, or in which man’s “soul” could wander unfettered by the bounds of the physical body. Dreams were variously considered to be messages from seemly or unseemly supernatural entities, or, later as the study of the human mind developed, a reflection of “inner” unexpressed human desires or psychic traits. The study of sleep as a physiologic phenomenon was addressed in the early 20th century by the French scientist Henri Pieron, in his pioneering treatise “Le problem physiologique du sommeil.” [6]. In the 1920’s Nathaniel Kleitman, the father of American sleep research, performed a series of pioneering studies on the regulation of the sleep/wakefulness cycle. His crucial work included descriptions of sleep characteristics in different populations and effects of sleep deprivation. In 1953, along with his graduate student Aserinsky, he described the state of REM sleep [7]. Another of Kleitman’s students, Dr. William C. Dement, extended Kleitman’s work, describing the cyclical nature of sleep and established that vivid dreams occurred during REM sleep. In addition, Dement engaged in research on animals that opened the way for elucidation of electrophysiological, pharmacological, and biochemical correlates of different sleep stages under various circumstances [8]. Since the pioneering work of these and many other workers, the field of sleep medicine has expanded and become a recognized medical discipline with a specified training process, research AIMS Neuroscience Volume 2, Issue 3, 155-171.
Recommended publications
  • The Meaning of Dreams Dreams May Reflect a Fundamental Aspect of Mammalian Memory Processing
    The Meaning of Dreams Dreams may reflect a fundamental aspect of mammalian memory processing. Crucial information acquired during the waking state may be reprocessed during sleep by Jonathan Winson The Author hroughout history human beings have sought to understand the meaning of dreams. The ancient Egyptians believed dreams JONATHAN WINSON possessed oracular power—in the Bible, for example, Joseph’s started his career as an elucidation of Pharaoh’s dream averted seven years of famine. SCALA/ART RESOURCE aeronautical engineer, grad- TOther cultures have interpreted dreams as inspirational, curative or alterna- uating with an engineering degree from the California tive reality. During the past century, scientists have offered conflicting psy- Institute of Technology in chological and neuroscientific explanations for dreams. In 1900, with the 1946. He completed his publication of The Interpretation of Dreams, Sigmund Freud proposed that Ph.D. in mathematics at dreams were the “royal road” to the unconscious; that they revealed in dis- Columbia University and guised form the deepest elements of an individual’s inner life. then turned to business for More recently, in contrast, dreams have been characterized as meaningless, 15 years. Because of his keen interest in neurosci- the result of random nerve cell activity. Dreaming has also been viewed as the ence, however, Winson means by which the brain rids itself of unnecessary information—a process started to do research at the of “reverse learning,” or unlearning. Rockefeller University on Based on recent findings in my own and other neuroscientific laboratories, memory processing during I propose that dreams are indeed meaningful. Studies of the hippocampus (a waking and sleeping states.
    [Show full text]
  • ESRS 40Th Anniversary Book
    European Sleep Research Society 1972 – 2012 40th Anniversary of the ESRS Editor: Claudio L. Bassetti Co-Editors: Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz European Sleep Research Society 1972 – 2012 40th Anniversary of the ESRS Editor: Claudio L. Bassetti Co-Editors: Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz Imprint Editor Publisher and Layout Claudio L. Bassetti Wecom Gesellschaft für Kommunikation mbH & Co. KG Co-Editors Hildesheim / Germany Brigitte Knobl, Hartmut Schulz www.wecom.org © European Sleep Research Society (ESRS), Regensburg, Bern, 2012 For amendments there can be given no limit or warranty by editor and publisher. Table of Contents Presidential Foreword . 5 Future Perspectives The Future of Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine in Europe: A Need for Academic Multidisciplinary Sleep Centres C. L. Bassetti, D.-J. Dijk, Z. Dogas, P. Levy, L. L. Nobili, P. Peigneux, T. Pollmächer, D. Riemann and D. J. Skene . 7 Historical Review of the ESRS General History of the ESRS H. Schulz, P. Salzarulo . 9 The Presidents of the ESRS (1972 – 2012) T. Pollmächer . 13 ESRS Congresses M. Billiard . 15 History of the Journal of Sleep Research (JSR) J. Horne, P. Lavie, D.-J. Dijk . 17 Pictures of the Past and Present of Sleep Research and Sleep Medicine in Europe J. Horne, H. Schulz . 19 Past – Present – Future Sleep and Neuroscience R. Amici, A. Borbély, P. L. Parmeggiani, P. Peigneux . 23 Sleep and Neurology C. L. Bassetti, L. Ferini-Strambi, J. Santamaria . 27 Psychiatric Sleep Research T. Pollmächer . 31 Sleep and Psychology D. Riemann, C. Espie . 33 Sleep and Sleep Disordered Breathing P. Levy, J. Hedner . 35 Sleep and Chronobiology A.
    [Show full text]
  • Donald B. Lindsley Papers Biomed.0423
    http://oac.cdlib.org/findaid/ark:/13030/kt1p3036g6 No online items Finding Aid for the Donald B. Lindsley Papers Biomed.0423 Finding aid prepared by Jason Richard Miller, 2010. The processing of this collection was generously supported by Arcadia funds. UCLA Library Special Collections Online finding aid last updated 2020 November 18. Room A1713, Charles E. Young Research Library Box 951575 Los Angeles, CA 90095-1575 [email protected] URL: https://www.library.ucla.edu/special-collections Finding Aid for the Donald B. Lindsley Biomed.0423 1 Papers Biomed.0423 Contributing Institution: UCLA Library Special Collections Title: Donald B. Lindsley papers Identifier/Call Number: Biomed.0423 Physical Description: 58.5 Linear Feet(97 boxes, 4 half document boxes, 4 shoe boxes, 2 flat oversize boxes, 1 magazine box, 3 LP boxes) Date (inclusive): 1866-2001 Abstract: Donald B. Lindsley was an early pioneer of the electroencephalogram (EEG) and an internationally recognized psychologist and brain scientist. Originally from Ohio, Lindsley worked throughout the United States and spent the last half of his career at UCLA where he was instrumental in founding UCLA's Brain Research Institute. Nearly half of this collection is constituted by Lindsley's correspondence spanning over 70 years. The remainder of the collection consists of reprints, typescripts of papers and talks, research notes, research and technical data, audiovisual material, and autobiographical ephemera that date from the late nineteenth century to the beginning of the twenty-first century. Stored off-site. All requests to access special collections material must be made in advance using the request button located on this page.
    [Show full text]
  • Giovanni Berlucchi
    BK-SFN-NEUROSCIENCE-131211-03_Berlucchi.indd 96 16/04/14 5:21 PM Giovanni Berlucchi BORN: Pavia, Italy May 25, 1935 EDUCATION: Liceo Classico Statale Ugo Foscolo, Pavia, Maturità (1953) Medical School, University of Pavia, MD (1959) California Institute of Technology, Postdoctoral Fellowship (1964–1965) APPOINTMENTS: University of Pennsylvania (1968) University of Siena (1974) University of Pisa (1976) University of Verona (1983) HONORS AND AWARDS: Academia Europaea (1990) Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei (1992) Honorary PhD in Psychology, University of Pavia (2007) After working initially on the neurophysiology of the sleep-wake cycle, Giovanni Berlucchi did pioneering electrophysiological investigations on the corpus callosum and its functional contribution to the interhemispheric transfer of visual information and to the representation of the visual field in the cerebral cortex and the superior colliculus. He was among the first to use reaction times for analyzing hemispheric specializations and interactions in intact and split brain humans. His latest research interests include visual spatial attention and the representation of the body in the brain. BK-SFN-NEUROSCIENCE-131211-03_Berlucchi.indd 97 16/04/14 5:21 PM Giovanni Berlucchi Family and Early Years A man’s deepest roots are where he has spent the enchanted days of his childhood, usually where he was born. My deepest roots lie in the ancient Lombard city of Pavia, where I was born 78 years ago, on May 25, 1935, and in that part of the province of Pavia that lies to the south of the Po River and is called the Oltrepò Pavese. The hilly part of the Oltrepò is covered with beautiful vineyards that according to archaeological and historical evidence have been used to produce good wines for millennia.
    [Show full text]
  • Focus on the REM Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD)
    Sleep phenotyping of the animal models of neurodegenerative diseases: Focus on the REM sleep behavior disorder (RBD) Pierre-Hervé Luppi, Team "Physiopathology of the neuronal network of the sleep-waking cycle” Centre of Neuroscience of Lyon UMR 5292 CNRS/U1028 INSERM , Lyon, France Michel Jouvet: the father of paradoxical (REM) sleep and RBD! 16 November 1925- 3 October 2017 CNRS Research Director (full time researcher) 2011- Head of the Team “Physiopathology of the neuronal network of the sleep-waking cycle" of the Center of Neuroscience of Lyon, UMR 5292 CNRS/U1028 INSERM, Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1, France. 2002-2010: Director of the CNRS laboratory UMR5167, University Claude Bernard Lyon I. 1999 – 2001 Director of Research (DR2), CNRS, INSERM U52, University Claude Bernard Lyon I. 1989 – 1999 CR1 CNRS, INSERM U52, University Claude Bernard Lyon I. 1984 – 1989 PhD in Neurosciences, University Claude Bernard Lyon I. Michel Jouvet, Advisor. 1990-911983 – 1984Visiting DEA (Master) scientist Neuroscience, (6 months in Universitytotal) at the Claude Department Bernard of Lyon I. MentalMichel Jouvet,Health Sciences,Advisor. Hahnemann University, Philadelphia, USA (Pr. ASTON-JONES), Study of the afferents to the locus coeruleus. 1992 Invited scientist (6 months), at the Brain Research Institute, UCLA, Los Angeles, USA. (Dr. MICHAEL CHASE), Study of the glycinergic neurons responsible for muscle atonia of paradoxical sleep. What is paradoxical (REM) sleep (PS)? Waking SWS or nonREM sleep EEG EEG EMG EMG EOG EOG Paradoxical (REM) sleep EEG EMG EOG total sleep 12h50 (SWS 10h50 min - PS2h00 min) Hypnogram of 24h PS SWS W 07h00 19h00 07h00 A paradoxical (REM) sleep (PS)-like state with muscle atonia is still present in ªpontine catsº: muscle atonia of PS is generated by a brainstem circuit Jouvet, 1962, Arch.
    [Show full text]
  • Je Voudrais Parler Français: Brain Activity During Sleep Supports Second Language Learning
    Je voudrais parler français: Brain activity during sleep supports second language learning Kristen Thompson Thesis submitted to the University of Ottawa in partial Fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Art in Psychology Department of Psychology Faculty of Social Sciences University of Ottawa © Kristen Thompson, Ottawa, Canada, 2020 ii Abstract Language learning depends on a variety of cognitive abilities, including long-term memory. Sleep is important for the enhancement of memory for newly acquired information and skills. Much of what we know about the relationship between sleep and memory has come from the investigation of two distinct long-term memory systems: declarative (memory for e.g., facts, figures and events) and procedural (memory for e.g., strategy, rules and motor skills). Several sleep-specific electrophysiological markers of memory processing have been identified. More specifically, sleep spindles (bursts of neural oscillatory activity which characterize non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep) may be a marker of consolidation for declarative memory (e.g., semantics, facts, figures, events), while rapid eye movements may serve as a marker for cognitive aspects (e.g., grammatical rule-learning) of procedural memory. In adults, second language acquisition (SLA) is thought to depend at first on declarative memory for grammar and linguistic rules (i.e., “early SLA”), and then shifts to procedural memory as the learner gains experience (i.e., “late SLA”). Given the unique roles of spindles and rapid eye movements in declarative and procedural memory consolidation, it was hypothesized that sleep spindles would correlate with language improvement during early SLA, whereas rapid eye movements would correlate with language improvement during late SLA.
    [Show full text]
  • Homeostatic Plasticity in the Nervous System
    Neural Plasticity Homeostatic Plasticity in the Nervous System Guest Editors: Arianna Maffei, Dirk Bucher, and Alfredo Fontanini Homeostatic Plasticity in the Nervous System Neural Plasticity Homeostatic Plasticity in the Nervous System Guest Editors: Arianna Maffei, Dirk Bucher, and Alfredo Fontanini Copyright © 2012 Hindawi Publishing Corporation. All rights reserved. This is a special issue published in “Neural Plasticity.” All articles are open access articles distributed under the Creative Commons Attri- bution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Editorial Board Robert Adamec, Canada George W. Huntley, USA Kerry J. Ressler, USA Shimon Amir, Canada Yuji Ikegaya, Japan Susan J. Sara, France Michel Baudry, USA Leszek Kaczmarek, Poland Timothy Schallert, USA Michael S. Beattie, USA Jeansok J. Kim, USA Menahem Segal, Israel Clive Raymond Bramham, Norway Eric Klann, USA Panagiotis Smirniotis, USA Anna Katharina Braun, Germany Małgorzata Kossut, Poland Ivan Soltesz, USA Sumantra Chattarji, India Frederic Libersat, Israel Michael G. Stewart, UK Robert Chen, Canada Stuart C. Mangel, UK Naweed I. Syed, Canada David Diamond, USA Aage R. Møller, USA Donald A. Wilson, USA M. B. Dutia, UK Diane K. O’Dowd, USA J. R. Wolpaw, USA Richard Dyck, Canada Sarah L. Pallas, USA Chun-Fang Wu, USA Zygmunt Galdzicki, USA A. Pascual-Leone, USA J. M. Wyss, USA PrestonE.Garraghty,USA Maurizio Popoli, Italy Lin Xu, China Paul E. Gold, USA Bruno Poucet, France Min Zhuo, Canada Manuel B. Graeber, Australia Lucas Pozzo-Miller, USA Anthony Hannan, Australia Vilayanur S. Ramachandran, USA Contents Homeostatic Plasticity in the Nervous System, Arianna Maffei, Dirk Bucher, and Alfredo Fontanini Volume 2012, Article ID 913472, 2 pages Wherefore Art Thou, Homeo(stasis)? Functional Diversity in Homeostatic Synaptic Plasticity, Bridget N.
    [Show full text]
  • N.B. This Is a Final Draft Only. Наreview of Michel Jouvet, The
    1/30/2017 Michel Jouvet, The Paradox of Sleep: the story of dreaming N.B. This is a final draft only. ­ Review of Michel Jouvet, The Paradox of Sleep: the story of dreaming. Translated by Laurence Garey. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999. xiii + 211 pp. Patricia Cox Miller, Dreams in Late Antiquity: studies in the imagination of a culture. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1998 (first paper edition); 1994 (originally published). xii + 273 pp., forthcoming in Journal of the History of the Neurosciences. Please do send comments: email me. Back to my home page. Michel Jouvet, The Paradox of Sleep: the story of dreaming. Translated by Laurence Garey. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999. xiii + 211 pp. Patricia Cox Miller, Dreams in Late Antiquity: studies in the imagination of a culture. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1998 (first paper edition); 1994 (originally published). xii + 273 pp. "It is not up to the neurobiologist", confesses Michel Jouvet, one of the pioneers of research on paradoxical sleep since the 1950s, "to retrace the history of dreaming". Yet, like J. Allan Hobson, Peretz Lavie, and other great dream scientists of the late 20th century, Jouvet delights in historical evidence and anecdote, from Cro­Magnon depictions of erection in sleep to the great 19th­century dream journals. There is so little theoretical consensus in our dream sciences ­ Jouvet laments heroically the 'very strange stature' of the 'hypno­oneirologist' in neuroscience ­ that it might seem perverse to multiply the difficulty of the subject by trying to deal with the history of dreams and the science of dreams at once.
    [Show full text]
  • Theories of Dreaming and Lucid Dreaming: an Integrative Review
    Theories of dreaming and lucid dreaming I J o D R Theories of dreaming and lucid dreaming: An integrative review towards sleep, dreaming and consciousness Nicolas Zink & Reinhard Pietrowsky Heinrich-Heine-University, Düsseldorf, Department of Clinical Psychology, Düsseldorf, Germany Summary. The present review gives an overview on common theories of dreaming with a specific emphasis on how they are able to explain lucid dreaming. The theories are grouped either to such that describe structural or biological processes of dreams or to such that describe evolutionary and adaptive functions of dreams. This overview shows that none of the theories outlined is fully capable of explaining neither non-lucid dreaming nor lucid dreaming. With respect to the first group, the concept of “protoconsciousness” is the theory that at best explains lucid dreaming. With respect to theories with an evolutionary and adaptive function of dreams, those theories, that stress the problem solving or simula- tion functions of dreams are more suited to explain lucid dreaming. Further, aspects that induce or amplify lucidity and the neural mechanisms that may be involved in lucid dreaming are described. Keywords: Lucid dreaming, lucidity, evolutionary functions, dreaming, consciousness, dream theories, protoconsciousness 1. Introduction and theories proposing evolutionary and adaptive functions for dreaming. In other words: theories that explain how Over the last several decades an effort has been made to dreaming works and theories that explain why the dreaming shed light on the phenomenon of lucid dreaming and the process evolved in our ancestors. We thus put an emphasis factors associated with this ability. The phenomenon of on lucid dream research and its compatibility with current lucid dreams in REM sleep was verified in the late 1970s dream theories.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2007.09560V2 [Q-Bio.NC] 24 Sep 2020
    The Overfitted Brain: Dreams evolved to assist generalization Erik Hoel∗1 1Allen Discovery Center, Tufts University, Medford, MA, USA September 25, 2020 Abstract Understanding of the evolved biological function of sleep has advanced considerably in the past decade. However, no equivalent understanding of dreams has emerged. Contemporary neuroscientific theories generally view dreams as epiphenomena, and the few proposals for their biological function are contradicted by the phenomenology of dreams themselves. Now, the recent advent of deep neural networks (DNNs) has finally provided the novel conceptual framework within which to understand the evolved function of dreams. Notably, all DNNs face the issue of overfitting as they learn, which is when performance on one data set increases but the network’s performance fails to generalize (often measured by the divergence of performance on training vs. testing data sets). This ubiquitous problem in DNNs is often solved by modelers via “noise injections” in the form of noisy or corrupted inputs. The goal of this paper is to argue that the brain faces a similar challenge of overfitting, and that nightly dreams evolved to combat the brain’s overfitting during its daily learning. That is, dreams are a biological mechanism for increasing generalizability via the creation of corrupted sensory inputs from stochastic activity across the hierarchy of neural structures. Sleep loss, specifically dream loss, leads to an overfitted brain that can still memorize and learn but fails to generalize appropriately. Herein this "overfitted brain hypothesis" is explicitly developed and then compared and contrasted with existing contemporary neuroscientific theories of dreams. Existing evidence for the hypothesis is surveyed within both neuroscience and deep learning, and a set of testable predictions are put forward that can be pursued both in vivo and in silico.
    [Show full text]
  • The Function of Dream Sleep Francis Crick* & Graeme Mitchison*
    _NA_ru_~__ vo_L_.~ __ I4_ru_Lv __ I~_J _________________ ~()~~E~TJ\Frf----------------------------~~~~ The function of dream sleep Francis Crick* & Graeme Mitchison* We propose that the function of dream sleep (more properly rapid-eye movement or REM sleep) is to remove certain undesirable modes of interaction in networks of cells in the cerebral cortex. We postulate that this is done in REM sleep by a reverse learning mechanism (see also p.l58), so that the trace in the brain of the unconscious dream is weakened, rather than strengthened, by the dream. MANKIND has always been fascinated by is explained in more detail below. Without to go into large-amplitude instabilities5 • dreams. As might be expected, there have it we believe that the mammalian cortex been many attempts to assign a purpose or could not perform so well. Neuronal networks significance to them. Although we dream We first describe our ideas about the cor­ Now, if one asks what functions such richly for one or two hours every night, we do not tex followed by a brief account of neural interconnected assemblies of cells could remember most of our dreams. Earlier networks. Next we outline what is known serve, one attractive possibility is that they thinkers, such as Freud, did not know this. about REM sleep. (For general accounts, could store associations6- 8 • To see this, Modern theories (not reviewed here in see refs 1,2.) We then describe our suppose an 'event' is represented by the ac­ detail) have usually proposed that sleep postulated mechanism and how it might be tivity of a subset of cells in a cell assembly.
    [Show full text]
  • Dreams May Be Crucial in Mammalian Memory Processing. Important Information Acquired While Awake May Be Reprocessed During Sleep
    Dreams may be crucial in mammalian memory processing. Important information acquired while awake may be reprocessed during sleep TheMeaning ofDreams By Jonathan Winson Throughout history, human beings have sought to understand the meaning of dreams. The ancient Egyptians believed dreams possessed oracular pow- state, a period of several minutes when thoughts consist of frag- er—in the Bible, for example, Joseph’s elucidation of Pharaoh’s mented images or minidramas. The hypnogogic state is followed dream averted seven years of famine. Other cultures have in- by slow-wave sleep, so called because at that time the brain terpreted dreams as inspirational, curative or alternative reality. waves of the neocortex (the convoluted outer mantle of the During the past century, scientists have offered conflicting psy- brain) are low in frequency and large in amplitude. These sig- chological and neuroscientific explanations for dreams. In nals are measured as electroencephalographic (EEG) recordings. 1900, with the publication of The Interpretation of Dreams, Researchers also discovered that a night’s sleep is punctu- Sigmund Freud proposed that dreams were the “royal road” to ated by periods in which the EEG readings are irregular in fre- the unconscious, that they revealed in disguised form the deep- quency and low in amplitude—similar to those observed in est elements of an individual’s inner life. awake individuals. These periods of mental activity are called More recently, in contrast, dreams have been characterized REM sleep. Dreaming takes place solely during these periods. as meaningless, the result of random nerve cell activity. Dream- While in REM sleep, motor neurons are inhibited, preventing ing has also been viewed as the means by which the brain rids the body from moving freely but allowing extremities to remain itself of unnecessary information—a process of “reverse learn- slightly active.
    [Show full text]