ESPEN Guidelines on Nutrition in Dementia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

ESPEN Guidelines on Nutrition in Dementia YCLNU2644_proof ■ 28 September 2015 ■ 1/23 Clinical Nutrition xxx (2015) 1e23 55 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect 56 57 Clinical Nutrition 58 59 60 journal homepage: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/clnu 61 62 63 e-SPEN guideline 64 65 1 Q7 ESPEN guidelines on nutrition in dementia 66 2 67 3 a, * b c d 68 Q6 Dorothee Volkert , Michael Chourdakis , Gerd Faxen-Irving , Thomas Frühwald , 4 e f g a, h 69 5 Francesco Landi , Merja H. Suominen , Maurits Vandewoude , Rainer Wirth , i 70 6 Stephane M. Schneider 71 7 a Institute for Biomedicine of Aging (IBA), Friedrich-Alexander-Universitat€ (FAU) Erlangen-Nürnberg, Nuremberg, Germany 72 8 b Department of Medicine, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki (AUTH), Greece 73 9 c Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden 74 10 d Department of Geriatric Acute Care, Krankenhaus Hietzing, Vienna, Austria e 75 11 Department of Geriatrics, Neurosciences and Orthopedics, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Rome, Italy f Unit of Primary Health Care, Helsinki University Central Hospital, Helsinki, Finland 76 12 g Department of Geriatrics, Medical School, University of Antwerp, Belgium 77 13 h St. Marien-Hospital Borken, Department for Internal Medicine and Geriatrics, Borken, Germany 78 i 14 Nutritional Support Unit, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire de Nice, Nice, France 79 15 80 16 81 17 article info summary 82 18 83 19 Article history: Background: Older people suffering from dementia are at increased risk of malnutrition due to various 84 20 Received 20 August 2015 nutritional problems, and the question arises which interventions are effective in maintaining adequate Accepted 10 September 2015 85 21 nutritional intake and nutritional status in the course of the disease. It is of further interest whether fi 86 22 supplementation of energy and/or speci c nutrients is able to prevent further cognitive decline or even Keywords: fi fi 87 23 correct cognitive impairment, and in which situations arti cial nutritional support is justi ed. Guideline Objective: It is the purpose of these guidelines to cover these issues with evidence-based 88 24 Dementia recommendations. 89 25 Malnutrition 90 Nutritional support Methods: The guidelines were developed by an international multidisciplinary working group in 26 fi Nutritional interventions accordance with of cially accepted standards. The GRADE system was used for assigning strength of 91 27 evidence. Recommendations were discussed, submitted to Delphi rounds and accepted in an online 92 28 survey among ESPEN members. 93 29 Results: 26 recommendations for nutritional care of older persons with dementia are given. In every 94 30 person with dementia, screening for malnutrition and close monitoring of body weight are recom- 95 31 mended. In all stages of the disease, oral nutrition may be supported by provision of adequate, attractive 96 food in a pleasant environment, by adequate support and elimination of potential causes of malnutrition. 32 97 Supplementation of single nutrients is not recommended unless there is a sign of deficiency. Oral 33 98 34 nutritional supplements are recommended to improve nutritional status but not to correct cognitive impairment or prevent cognitive decline. Artificial nutrition is suggested in patients with mild or 99 35 moderate dementia for a limited period of time to overcome a crisis situation with markedly insufficient 100 36 oral intake, if low nutritional intake is predominantly caused by a potentially reversible condition, but 101 37 not in patients with severe dementia or in the terminal phase of life. 102 38 Conclusion: Nutritional care and support should be an integral part of dementia management. In all 103 39 stages of the disease, the decision for or against nutritional interventions should be made on an indi- 104 40 vidual basis after carefully balancing expected benefit and potential burden, taking the (assumed) patient 105 41 will and general prognosis into account. 106 © 2015 Elsevier Ltd and European Society for Clinical Nutrition and Metabolism. All rights reserved. 42 107 43 108 44 109 45 110 46 111 47 112 48 Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer's disease; APOE, apolipoprotein E-e4 allele; DHA, docosahexaenoic acid; EPA, eicosapentaenoic acid; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; MNA, 113 49 mini nutritional assessment; MNA-SF, mini nutritional assessment short form; MMSE, mini mental state examination; RCT, randomized controlled trial. 114 50 * Corresponding author. Institute for Biomedicine of Aging (IBA), Friedrich-Alexander-Universitat€ (FAU) Erlangen-Nürnberg, Kobergerstraße 60, 90408 Nürnberg, Germany. 115 þ 51 Tel.: 49 911 5302 96150. 116 E-mail address: [email protected] (D. Volkert). 52 117 53 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clnu.2015.09.004 118 54 0261-5614/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd and European Society for Clinical Nutrition and Metabolism. All rights reserved. 119 Please cite this article in press as: Volkert D, et al., ESPEN guidelines on nutrition in dementia, Clinical Nutrition (2015), http://dx.doi.org/ 10.1016/j.clnu.2015.09.004 YCLNU2644_proof ■ 28 September 2015 ■ 2/23 2 D. Volkert et al. / Clinical Nutrition xxx (2015) 1e23 1 1. Development of guidelines on nutrition in dementia Table 2 66 2 Strength of recommendation. 67 3 Q1 The European Society for Clinical Nutrition and Metabolism Strength of recommendation 68 4 69 (ESPEN) launched a process of developing guidelines on nutrition Strong We recommend/do not recommend 5 care for patients with dementia. The group included physicians, Weak We suggest/do not suggest 70 6 nutritionists and dietitians with a background in geriatrics, nutri- 71 7 tion and/or ethics, all experienced in treatment and nutritional 72 8 therapy of persons with dementia, as well as the guidelines coor- more common, but it is not normal healthy aging and it is not 73 9 dinator (SMS); all are authors of this guideline document. benign. It is a malignant and devastating condition leading invari- 74 10 The experts followed the GRADE method, which was based on ably to dependence and finally death [4]. As a clinical syndrome, it 75 11 determinations of grade of evidence and strength of recommenda- is characterized by global cognitive impairment with a decline in 76 12 tion; the methodology is described elsewhere [1]. A two-day live memory and at least in one other cognitive domain, such as lan- 77 13 meeting was organized in Biedenkopf, Germany, in April of 2014, guage, visuospatial, or executive function. It represents a decline 78 14 and three phone conferences were held. from the previous level of cognitive functioning, and is associated 79 15 Descriptive findings that did not lead to specific recommenda- with impairment in functional abilities and, in many cases, 80 16 tions are grouped in a first part (general considerations), whereas behavioral and psychiatric disturbances [5,6]. Many diseases can 81 17 all questions that led to comparisons of interventions and to rec- cause a dementia syndrome; Alzheimer's disease and cerebrovas- 82 18 ommendations are grouped in a second part (recommendations). cular dementia are the two most common causes, and many cases 83 19 A systematic literature search was conducted in PubMed and the of dementia involve both these disorders. Lewy body disorders 84 20 Cochrane Library. The grade of evidence (GOE) was determined by a (Parkinson's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies) and fronto- 85 21 number of factors, starting with the number and type of research temporal dementia are less common but still make up 8% of peo- 86 22 studies [2]. Grading from High to Very Low was used to rate the ple referred to a memory clinic [5]. Although some potentially 87 23 quality of the underlying evidence and the level of certainty for reversible conditions, such as hypothyroidism or vitamin B12 defi- 88 24 effect (Table 1) [3]. Highest quality evidence resulted from consis- ciency, are often thought to cause dementia, no more than 1.5% of 89 25 tent results or meta-analysis of multiple randomized controlled cases of mild to moderate dementia are fully reversible. Age is the 90 26 trials, with the next highest level defined by at least one well- best-studied and strongest risk factor for dementia, which explains 91 27 designed randomized controlled trial. Moderate and low-level ev- the increasing burden of cognitive disorders in the years to come 92 28 idence came from controlled trials that were not randomized, from [7]. Other risk factors for Alzheimer's disease include genetic risk 93 29 cohort- or case-controlled studies, or from multiple time series factors such as having a first-degree relative with a history of Alz- 94 30 trials. Very low-level evidence was from expert clinical experience heimer's disease, having the apolipoprotein ε4 genotype or 95 31 or from descriptive studies. The grade was then decreased if there suffering from the Down syndrome. Cardiovascular risk factors 96 32 were limitations to study quality, inconsistencies in findings, such as hypertension are associated with an increased risk for both 97 33 imprecise or sparse data, or high likelihood of reporting bias. The Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Also lifestyle factors 98 34 grade was increased if there was high consistency of findings or such as low educational level or head trauma may play an impor- 99 35 strong evidence of association (Table 1). tant role [7]. 100 36 The strength of recommendation was based on a consensus dis- Dementia causes a high burden of suffering for patients, their 101 37 cussion, which included expression and deliberation of expert families, and society [8]. For patients, it leads to increased anxiety, 102 38 opinions, risk-benefit ratio of recommendation, costs, and a review depression and dependency and complicates other comorbid con- 103 39 of supportive evidence, followed by Delphi rounds and votes until ditions.
Recommended publications
  • Psychogenic and Organic Amnesia. a Multidimensional Assessment of Clinical, Neuroradiological, Neuropsychological and Psychopathological Features
    Behavioural Neurology 18 (2007) 53–64 53 IOS Press Psychogenic and organic amnesia. A multidimensional assessment of clinical, neuroradiological, neuropsychological and psychopathological features Laura Serraa,∗, Lucia Faddaa,b, Ivana Buccionea, Carlo Caltagironea,b and Giovanni A. Carlesimoa,b aFondazione IRCCS Santa Lucia, Roma, Italy bClinica Neurologica, Universita` Tor Vergata, Roma, Italy Abstract. Psychogenic amnesia is a complex disorder characterised by a wide variety of symptoms. Consequently, in a number of cases it is difficult distinguish it from organic memory impairment. The present study reports a new case of global psychogenic amnesia compared with two patients with amnesia underlain by organic brain damage. Our aim was to identify features useful for distinguishing between psychogenic and organic forms of memory impairment. The findings show the usefulness of a multidimensional evaluation of clinical, neuroradiological, neuropsychological and psychopathological aspects, to provide convergent findings useful for differentiating the two forms of memory disorder. Keywords: Amnesia, psychogenic origin, organic origin 1. Introduction ness of the self – and a period of wandering. According to Kopelman [33], there are three main predisposing Psychogenic or dissociative amnesia (DSM-IV- factors for global psychogenic amnesia: i) a history of TR) [1] is a clinical syndrome characterised by a mem- transient, organic amnesia due to epilepsy [52], head ory disorder of nonorganic origin. Following Kopel- injury [4] or alcoholic blackouts [20]; ii) a history of man [31,33], psychogenic amnesia can either be sit- psychiatric disorders such as depressed mood, and iii) uation specific or global. Situation specific amnesia a severe precipitating stress, such as marital or emo- refers to memory loss for a particular incident or part tional discord [23], bereavement [49], financial prob- of an incident and can arise in a variety of circum- lems [23] or war [21,48].
    [Show full text]
  • What Is It Like to Be Confabulating?
    What is it like to be Confabulating? Sahba Besharati, Aikaterini Fotopoulou and Michael D. Kopelman Kings College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London UK Different kinds of confabulations may arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders. This chapter first offers conceptual distinctions between spontaneous and momentary (“provoked”) confabulations, as well as between these types of confabulation and other kinds of false memories. The chapter then reviews current explanatory theories, emphasizing that both neurocognitive and motivational factors account for the content of confabulations. We place particular emphasis on a general model of confabulation that considers cognitive dysfunctions in memory and executive functioning in parallel with social and emotional factors. It is argued that all these dimensions need to be taken into account for a phenomenologically rich description of confabulation. The role of the motivated content of confabulation and the subjective experience of the patient are particularly relevant in effective management and rehabilitation strategies. Finally, we discuss a case example in order to illustrate how seemingly meaningless false memories are actually meaningful if placed in the context of the patient’s own perspective and autobiographical memory. Key words: Confabulation; False memory; Motivation; Self; Rehabilitation. 1 Memory is often subject to errors of omission and commission such that recollection includes instances of forgetting, or distorting past experience. The study of pathological forms of exaggerated memory distortion has provided useful insights into the mechanisms of normal reconstructive remembering (Johnson, 1991; Kopelman, 1999; Schacter, Norman & Kotstall, 1998). An extreme form of pathological memory distortion is confabulation. Different variants of confabulation are found to arise in neurological and psychiatric disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • Metamemory and Memory Discrepancies in Directed Forgetting of Emotional Information
    Research Reports Metamemory and Memory Discrepancies in Directed Forgetting of Emotional Information Dicle Çapan a, Simay Ikier b [a] Department of Psychology, Koç University, Istanbul, Turkey. [b] Department of Psychology, Bahçeşehir University, Istanbul, Turkey. Europe's Journal of Psychology, 2021, Vol. 17(1), 44–52, https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.2567 Received: 2019-12-17 • Accepted: 2020-04-23 • Published (VoR): 2021-02-26 Handling Editor: Rhian Worth, University of South Wales, Pontypridd, United Kingdom Corresponding Author: Dicle Çapan, Koç University, Rumelifeneri Mahallesi, Sarıyer Rumeli Feneri Yolu, 34450 Sarıyer Istanbul, Turkey. E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Directed Forgetting (DF) studies show that it is possible to exert cognitive control to intentionally forget information. The aim of the present study was to investigate how aware individuals are of the control they have over what they remember and forget when the information is emotional. Participants were presented with positive, negative and neutral photographs, and each photograph was followed by either a Remember or a Forget instruction. Then, for each photograph, participants provided Judgments of Learning (JOLs) by indicating their likelihood of recognizing that item on a subsequent test. In the recognition phase, participants were asked to indicate all old items, irrespective of instruction. Remember items had higher JOLs than Forget items for all item types, indicating that participants believe they can intentionally forget even emotional information—which is not the case based on the actual recognition results. DF effect, which was calculated by subtracting recognition for Forget items from Remember ones was only significant for neutral items. Emotional information disrupted cognitive control, eliminating the DF effect.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory
    IFM LAB TUTORIAL SERIES # 6, COPYRIGHT c IFM LAB Cognitive Functions of the Brain: Perception, Attention and Memory Jiawei Zhang [email protected] Founder and Director Information Fusion and Mining Laboratory (First Version: May 2019; Revision: May 2019.) Abstract This is a follow-up tutorial article of [17] and [16], in this paper, we will introduce several important cognitive functions of the brain. Brain cognitive functions are the mental processes that allow us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information that we've received from external stimuli. This process allows us to understand and to relate to the world more effectively. Cognitive functions are brain-based skills we need to carry out any task from the simplest to the most complex. They are related with the mechanisms of how we learn, remember, problem-solve, and pay attention, etc. To be more specific, in this paper, we will talk about the perception, attention and memory functions of the human brain. Several other brain cognitive functions, e.g., arousal, decision making, natural language, motor coordination, planning, problem solving and thinking, will be added to this paper in the later versions, respectively. Many of the materials used in this paper are from wikipedia and several other neuroscience introductory articles, which will be properly cited in this paper. This is the last of the three tutorial articles about the brain. The readers are suggested to read this paper after the previous two tutorial articles on brain structure and functions [17] as well as the brain basic neural units [16]. Keywords: The Brain; Cognitive Function; Consciousness; Attention; Learning; Memory Contents 1 Introduction 2 2 Perception 3 2.1 Detailed Process of Perception .
    [Show full text]
  • The Three Amnesias
    The Three Amnesias Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Evelyn F. and William L. McKnight Brain Institute University of Florida PO Box 100165 HSC Gainesville, FL 32610-0165 USA Bauer, R.M. (in press). The Three Amnesias. In J. Morgan and J.E. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of Clinical Neuropsychology. Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis/Psychology Press. The Three Amnesias - 2 During the past five decades, our understanding of memory and its disorders has increased dramatically. In 1950, very little was known about the localization of brain lesions causing amnesia. Despite a few clues in earlier literature, it came as a complete surprise in the early 1950’s that bilateral medial temporal resection caused amnesia. The importance of the thalamus in memory was hardly suspected until the 1970’s and the basal forebrain was an area virtually unknown to clinicians before the 1980’s. An animal model of the amnesic syndrome was not developed until the 1970’s. The famous case of Henry M. (H.M.), published by Scoville and Milner (1957), marked the beginning of what has been called the “golden age of memory”. Since that time, experimental analyses of amnesic patients, coupled with meticulous clinical description, pathological analysis, and, more recently, structural and functional imaging, has led to a clearer understanding of the nature and characteristics of the human amnesic syndrome. The amnesic syndrome does not affect all kinds of memory, and, conversely, memory disordered patients without full-blown amnesia (e.g., patients with frontal lesions) may have impairment in those cognitive processes that normally support remembering.
    [Show full text]
  • Disordered Recognition Memory: Recollective Confabulation
    cortex xxx (2013) 1e12 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Special issue: Research report Disordered recognition memory: Recollective confabulation Chris J.A. Moulin* Laboratoire d’Etude de l’Apprentissage et du De´veloppement, CNRS UMR 5022, Universite´ de Bourgogne, Dijon, France article info abstract Article history: Recollective confabulation (RC) is encountered as a conviction that a present moment is a Received 31 January 2012 repetition of one experienced previously, combined with the retrieval of confabulated Reviewed 12 April 2012 specifics to support that assertion. It is often described as persistent de´ja` vu by family Revised 24 September 2012 members and caregivers. On formal testing, patients with RC tend to produce a very high Accepted 24 January 2013 level of false positive errors. In this paper, a new case series of 11 people with dementia or Published online xxx mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and with de´ja` vu-like experiences is presented. In two experiments the nature of the recognition memory deficit is explored. The results from Keywords: these two experiments suggest e contrary to our hypothesis in earlier published case re- Dementia ports e that recollection mechanisms are relatively spared in this group, and that patients De´ja` vu experience familiarity for non-presented items. The RC patients tended to be overconfident Reduplicative paramnesia in their assessment of recognition memory, and produce inaccurate assessments of their Familiarity performance. These findings are discussed with reference to delusions more generally, and Metacognition point to a combined memory and metacognitive deficit, possibly arising from damage to temporal and right frontal regions.
    [Show full text]
  • Sleep Is for Forgetting
    464 • The Journal of Neuroscience, January 18, 2017 • 37(3):464–473 Dual Perspectives Dual Perspectives Companion Paper: Sleep to Remember, by Susan J. Sara Sleep Is for Forgetting Gina R. Poe Department of Integrative Biology and Physiology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90095-7246 It is possible that one of the essential functions of sleep is to take out the garbage, as it were, erasing and “forgetting” information built up throughout the day that would clutter the synaptic network that defines us. It may also be that this cleanup function of sleep is a general principle of neuroscience, applicable to every creature with a nervous system. Key words: depotentiation; development; mental health; noradrenaline; REM sleep; spindles; theta; TR sleep Introduction was for forgetting useless tidbits of information learned through- In this article, I discuss and illustrate the importance of forgetting out the day that, if not eliminated, would soon saturate the mem- for development, for memory integration and updating, and for ory synaptic network with junk. Therefore, the type of forgetting resetting sensory-motor synapses after intense use. I then intro- we will discuss here is also not the global synaptic weight down- duce the mechanisms whereby sleep states and traits could serve scaling of the synaptic homeostasis hypothesis (SHY) (Tononi this unique forgetting function separately for memory circuits and Cirelli, 2003), although evidence from excellent experiments within reach of the locus coeruleus (LC) and those formed and used to support the idea of sleep for synaptic homeostasis will be governed outside its noradrenergic net. Specifically, I talk about used to make points here.
    [Show full text]
  • Proper Names and Repression. a Freudian Finding
    ACTA SCIENTIFIC Neurology (ISSN: 2582-1121) Volume 3 Issue 9 September 2020 Review Article Proper Names and Repression. A Freudian Finding Araceli Colín* Psychoanalyst. PhD in Anthropology from Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México Received: July 14, 2020 and Research Professor at the Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Published: August 28, 2020 Querétaro, Mexico © All rights are reserved by Araceli Colín. *Corresponding Author: Araceli Colín, Psychoanalyst. PhD in Anthropology from Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México and Research Professor at the Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Querétaro, Mexico. Abstract I intend to locate Freud's contributions in relation to personal names and repression. His most spreaded idea of the unconscious is it is necessary to highlight the way in which the personal name is tied to a lack. Without lack the reality-test could not be installed a consequence of repression. Repression operates in three stages described in his metapsychology. In order to articulate his findings, and the separation between conscious and unconscious could not be established. For Freud, the core of the unconscious is made up of agents of the representation (Vorstellungrepräsentanz). They are of the order of nonsense. In my opinion those representatives Fixierung - are letters. Letters of personal names are one of the first fixation materials ( ). Fixation constitutes the first time of repres unconscious: the inherited unconscious. Freud deployed this idea in his last work. sion: It is a necessary but not sufficient condition for the psychic apparatus to be founded. I also analyze another Freudian idea of the Keywords: Proper Name; Repression; Primal Repression; Fixation; Letters Introduction primal Fixierung), 2) Repression proper (Verdrängung) that is manifested, in the case of names, as a In Freud’s times, there were no theoretical developments repression or fixation ( forgetting, and 3) return of the repressed [21].
    [Show full text]
  • Forgetting Is a Feature, Not A
    PSSXXX10.1177/0956797619859531Popov et al.Preceding Cues in Item-Method Directed Forgetting 859531research-article2019 ASSOCIATION FOR Research Article PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Psychological Science 2019, Vol. 30(9) 1303 –1317 Forgetting Is a Feature, Not a Bug: © The Author(s) 2019 Article reuse guidelines: sagepub.com/journals-permissions Intentionally Forgetting Some Things DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619859531 10.1177/0956797619859531 Helps Us Remember Others by Freeing www.psychologicalscience.org/PS Up Working Memory Resources TC Vencislav Popov1 , Ivan Marevic2, Jan Rummel2 , and Lynne M. Reder1 1Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University and 2Department of Psychology, Heidelberg University Abstract In the present study, we used an item-method directed-forgetting paradigm to test whether instructions to forget or remember one item affect memory for subsequently studied items. In two experiments (Ns = 138 and 33, respectively), recall was higher when a word pair was preceded during study by a to-be-forgotten word pair. This effect was cumulative: Performance increased when more preceding study items were to be forgotten. The effect decreased when memory was conditioned on instructions for items appearing farther back in the study list. Experiment 2 used a dual-task paradigm that suppressed, during encoding, verbal rehearsal or attentional refreshing. Neither task removed the effect, ruling out that rehearsal or attentional borrowing is responsible for the advantage conferred from previous to-be-forgotten items. We propose that memory formation depletes a limited resource that recovers over time and that to-be-forgotten items consume fewer resources, leaving more resources available for storing subsequent items. A computational model implementing the theory provided excellent fits to the data.
    [Show full text]
  • Monitoring One's Own Forgetting in Younger and Older Adults
    Psychology and Aging © 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 26, No. 3, 631–635 0882-7974/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022852 BRIEF REPORT Monitoring One’s Own Forgetting in Younger and Older Adults Vered Halamish Shannon McGillivray and Alan D. Castel University of Haifa University of California, Los Angeles The present study examined whether there are age-related differences in the ability to accurately monitor forgetting. Young and older adults studied a mixed list of categorized words, and later recalled items when cued with each category. They then estimated the number of additional items that they did not recall—a form of monitoring one’s forgetting. Older adults exhibited impaired memory performance compared with young adults, but also accurately estimated they forgot more information than young adults. Both age groups were fairly accurate in predicting forgetting in terms of resolution, indicating that aging does not impair the ability to monitor forgetting. Keywords: aging, forgetting, memory, metacognition, metamemory It is widely documented that older adults’ episodic memory is and again results are inconsistent and often differ depending on the impaired (e.g., Craik & Salthouse, 2008), and that memory failures actual type of measure used to assess monitoring. Some studies are one of the most common complaints of older adults (Hertzog have examined this form of monitoring using postdictive judg- & Hultsch, 2000; Levy-Cushman & Abeles, 1998). One’s aware- ments that generally involve asking participants, after a recall test, ness of how memory changes and the ability to monitor changes or how many items they think they remembered, given the total declines in memory performance involves metacognition, and the number of items presented during encoding (e.g., Bunnell et al., ability to accurately monitor memory (and forgetting) has impor- 1999; Hertzog, Saylor, Fleece, & Dixon, 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Load and Time-Based Forgetting
    COGNITIVE LOAD AND TIME-BASED FORGETTING _______________________________________ A Thesis presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School at the University of Missouri-Columbia _______________________________________________________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts _____________________________________________________ by TIMOTHY J. RICKER Dr. Nelson Cowan, Thesis Supervisor JULY 2009 The undersigned, appointed by the dean of the Graduate School, have examined the thesis entitled COGNITIVE LOAD AND TIME-BASED FORGETTING presented by Timothy Ricker, a candidate for the degree of master of arts, and hereby certify that, in their opinion, it is worthy of acceptance. Professor Nelson Cowan Professor Jeffery Rouder Professor John Kerns Professor Judith Goodman ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank Dr. Nelson Cowan for his invaluable guidance and assistance through all parts of this research. I would also like to thank Dr. Candice Morey for her help designing the programming and selecting the stimuli used in the experiments detailed here, as well as, Dr. Judith Goodman, Dr. John Kerns, and Dr. Jeff Rouder for their helpful comments and support. ii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ................................................................................................ ii LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................... iv ABSTRACT .........................................................................................................................v
    [Show full text]
  • Sleep, Memory and Emotion
    G. A. Kerkhof and H. P. A. Van Dongen (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 185 ISSN: 0079-6123 Copyright Ó 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. CHAPTER 4 Sleep, memory and emotion Matthew P. Walker* Sleep and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA Abstract: As critical as waking brain function is to cognition, an extensive literature now indicates that sleep supports equally important, different, yet complementary operations. This review will consider recent and emerging findings implicating sleep, and specific sleep-stage physiologies, in the modulation, regulation and even preparation of cognitive and emotional brain processes. First, evidence for the role of sleep in memory processing will be discussed, principally focusing on declarative memory. Second, at a neural level, several mechanistic models of sleep-dependent plasticity underlying these effects will be reviewed, with a synthesis of these features offered that may explain the ordered structure of sleep, and the orderly evolution of memory stages. Third, accumulating evidence for the role of sleep in associative memory processing will be discussed, suggesting that the long-term goal of sleep may not be the strengthening of individually memory items, but, instead, their abstracted assimilation into a schema of generalized knowledge. Forth, the newly emerging benefit of sleep in regulating emotional brain reactivity will be considered. Finally, and building on this latter topic, a
    [Show full text]