Mixed Languages: YARON MATRAS University of Manchester a Functional±Communicative Approach

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mixed Languages: YARON MATRAS University of Manchester a Functional±Communicative Approach Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3 (2), 2000, 79±99 # 2000 Cambridge University Press 79 Mixed languages: YARON MATRAS University of Manchester a functional±communicative approach It has been suggested that the structural composition of mixed languages and the linguistic processes through which they emerge are to some extent predictable, and that they therefore constitute a language ``type'' (e.g. Bakker and Mous, 1994b; Bakker and Muysken, 1995). This view is challenged here. Instead, it is argued that the compartmentalisation of structures observed in mixed languages (i.e. the fact that certain structural categories are derived from one ``parent'' language, others from another) is the result of the cumulative effect of different contact mechanisms. These mechanisms are de®ned in terms of the cognitive and communicative motivations that lead speakers to model certain functions of language on an alternative linguistic system; each mechanism will typically affect particular functional categories. Four relevant processes are identi®ed: lexical re-orientation, selective replication, convergence, and categorial fusion. Different combinations of processes will render different outcomes, hence the diversity of mixed languages as regards their structure, function, and development. terest for a theory of grammar is therefore in de®ning Introduction and explaining qualitative and quantitative differ- Recent collections by Bakker and Mous (1994a) and ences between the effects of contact in mixed lan- by Thomason (1997a) highlight growing interest in guages, as opposed to more conventional linguistic what have been termed ``mixed languages'' or ``bilin- systems. gual mixtures''. Examples of mixed languages that It appears however that few structural properties have received considerable attention in recent litera- are shared by a majority of the languages classi®ed as ture are Michif, the Cree±French mixture spoken by mixed in the literature. The split between lexicon and the MeÂtis, descendants of buffalo hunters in the grammar, each derived from a different source lan- Canadian Plains and North Dakota; Ma'a, a Bantu guage, is often highlighted as prototypical or even as language in Tanzania that includes material from intrinsically constitutive of mixed languages (see e.g. Cushitic languages; Copper Island Aleut, an Aleut± Bakker, 1994 and elsewhere), but such splits are Russian admixture spoken by a mixed population off rarely if ever consistent. The dif®culty in ®nding clear the coast of Kamchatka; Media Lengua, a Quechua± structural criteria for mixed languages suggests that Spanish mixture of Ecuador; Angloromani and other the contact phenomena that give rise to them are not so-called Para-Romani languages, which are uniform but varied. The question I wish to pursue Romani-derived vocabularies used by ethnic Gypsies here is whether there is any functional signi®cance to within non-standard varieties of various European the structural compartmentalisation in a given mixed languages; and others. system, one that can be accounted for in terms of the Most languages are to some extent at least motivations to create or maintain such a mixture. My ``mixed'', in the sense that they have components that suggestion is that mixed languages differ from con- can be traced back to more than one source language ventional cases of contact in the density of different as a result of a situation of contact in the language's contact phenomena and their cumulative effect on the earlier history. So when languages are referred to in overall structure of the system. the literature explicitly as ``mixed'' (as for example in The present paper is structured as follows: ®rst I Bakker and Mous, 1994b; or Bakker and Muysken, survey current de®nitions of mixed languages and 1995), it is presumably in order to highlight that they models explaining their emergence; next I identify go beyond the commonly attested patterns of four function-based mechanisms of contact. I then mixture. Mixed languages are thus understood impli- proceed to examine the discourse relevance of mixed citly at least to breach conventional constraints on utterances in non-self-contained mixed systems, I contact-induced language change. The point of in- present the Functional Turnover hypothesis that ex- Address for correspondence Department of Linguistics, University of Manchester, Manchester M13 9PL, UK E-mail: [email protected] http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 21 Oct 2011 IP address: 130.88.56.124 80 Yaron Matras plains selective copying of grammatical features from Media Lengua). In fact, as pointed out by Smith an ancestral language, and ®nally I discuss the cumu- (1995; cf. also Thomason, 1995), the cultural motiva- lative effect of different contact mechanisms in so- tion behind the emergence of mixed varieties can just called plain or self-contained mixed systems. In con- as well be retention, re-gaining, or re-de®nition of clusion I argue that mixed languages are anything ethnic identity, defying pressure to change or assim- but a coherent or uniform language type, but that ilate. This is the case with a series of languages they offer opportunities to identify a complexity of treated as mixed in the literature, such as Para- processes of contact-induced change, and so can Romani (Boretzky and Igla, 1994), Shelta (also called de®nitely help revise our overall notion of structural Gammon, the secret language of Irish Travellers, cf. ``borrowing''. Grant, 1994), or Ma'a (Mous, 1994; cf. also Sasse, 1992). As regards the overall social context of emer- gence, an expression of cultural de®ance might there- Typologies of mixed languages fore seem a more accurate indicator. Smith (1995) has suggested a distinction between De®nitions ``plain'' mixed languages, which serve as everyday Genetic af®liation and historical reconstruction play community languages, and ``symbiotic'' varieties, a crucial role in attempts to de®ne mixed languages. which are specialised varieties of a non-mixed lan- Thomason and Kaufman (1988) consider a language guage used in the same community, typically secret as mixed if it does not offer opportunities for histor- languages. Like pidgins, secret languages are not ical reconstruction of its parent language (see also native languages and have a restricted functional Bakker and Mous, 1994b; Bakker and Muysken, scope. Like creoles, so-called ``plain'' mixed lan- 1995; Thomason, 1995, 1997b, e). Bakker (1994, 27) guages are full-¯edged native community languages puts this in more positive terms, de®ning mixed that re¯ect a newly emerged ethnic identity (cf. languages as idioms that evoke identi®cation on the Thomason, 1997e). One must, however, take into part of speakers of two separate source languages. account that the great majority of mixed languages Contrasting with pidgins and creoles, mixed lan- attested so far are spoken in communities alongside guages are assumed to be products of full bilingu- at least one of their parent or source languages, and alism (cf. Thomason, 1997b, e). This is not entirely we only rarely ®nd them in isolation. This may add unproblematic, however, if one takes into account to their controversial status as mixed registers (cf. the role of language attrition and language loss in e.g. Mous, 1994 on Ma'a) rather than full-¯edged producing mixed systems (see e.g. Sasse, 1992, on languages. As far as the ``languageness'' (Thomason, Krekonika, a Greek±Albanian mixture; and Ma'a; 1997b) is concerned, then, varieties discussed in the Boretzky and Igla, 1994, on Para-Romani). Para- literature as ``mixed'' are obviously spread along a doxically, what distinguishes mixed languages from rather wide continuum. pidgins and creoles is the fact that they show con- tinuity of signi®cant portions of the grammar from Models of emergence the grammaticiser parent language (or from both parent languages), yet it is precisely the interrupted Explanatory models of mixed languages can be transmission or non-continuity of a signi®cant grouped along three main points of focus: (1) Lan- portion of structural material that identi®es mixed guage maintenance and language shift, (2) unique languages as opposed to cases of ``normal'' language and pre-determined processes (``intertwining''), and development. (3) conventionalisation of language mixing patterns. Mixed languages have been argued to be the The ®rst approach highlights the substitution of major outcome of mixed marriages giving rise to new components of a given language ± for example entire ethno-cultural identities (Bakker, 1994, 1996, 1997; morphological paradigms or typological features ± for case studies see also Hancock, 1970, 1992; through material (alternative structures) from a dif- Golovko, 1994; de Gruiter, 1994; Thomason, 1997c). ferent source language. According to Thomason and Indeed some approaches view the emergence of a Kaufman (1988) mixed languages arise in situations new ethnic group as the chief setting for the develop- where language shift is partly resisted, and so parts of ment of a mixed language (see especially Bakker, the original language are retained while signi®cant 1997, as well as Thomason and Kaufman, 1988, and portions are replaced by the pressure-exerting lan- Thomason, 1995, 1997b, e). There are, however, guage. Sasse (1992) assumes re-lexi®cation of the cases of acculturation leading to the emergence of target language after a shift has taken place, with mixed languages in situations where no population younger speakers having only partial access to the mixture is attested (cf. Muysken's (1997), account of vocabulary of their ancestral language. Such cases http://journals.cambridge.org Downloaded: 21 Oct 2011 IP address: 130.88.56.124 Mixed languages 81 are considered by Sasse not as re-vitalisation (as mixed languages, and Bakker (1997, 213) similarly proposed for Para-Romani by Boretzky and Igla, admits that intertwined languages are created ``more 1994; see also Boretzky, 1998), but as processes of or less consciously''. Mous (1994) assumes deliberate language renewal through which new languages creation of Ma'a as a register of Mbugu by speakers emerge.
Recommended publications
  • Quichua-Spanish Language Contact in Salcedo, Ecuador: Revisiting Media Lengua Syncretic Language Practices
    QUICHUA-SPANISH LANGUAGE CONTACT IN SALCEDO, ECUADOR: REVISITING MEDIA LENGUA SYNCRETIC LANGUAGE PRACTICES BY MARCO SHAPPECK DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2011 Urbana, Illinois Doctoral Committee: Professor Hans Henrich Hock, Director of Research Professor Rajeshwari Vijay Pandharipande, Chair Associate Professor Anna María Escobar Professor José Ignacio Hualde Abstract The purpose of the current thesis is to develop a better understanding of the interaction between Spanish and Quichua in the Salcedo region and provide more information for the processes that might have given rise to Media Lengua, a ‘mixed’ language comprised of a Quichua grammar and Spanish lexicon. Muysken attributes the formation of Media Lengua to relexification, ruling out any influence from other bilingual phenomena. I argue that the only characteristic that distinguishes Media Lengua from other language contact varieties in central Ecuador is the quantity of the overall Spanish borrowings and not the type of processes that might have been employed by Quichua speakers during the genesis of Media Lengua. The results from the Salcedo data that I have collected show how processes such as adlexification, code-mixing, and structural convergence produce Media Lengua-type sentences, evidence that supports an alternative analysis to Muysken’s relexification hypothesis. Overall, this dissertation is developed around four main objectives: (1) to describe the variation of Spanish loanwords within a bilingual community in Salcedo; (2) to analyze some of the prominent and recent structural changes in Quichua and Spanish; (3) to determine whether Spanish loanword use can be explained by the relationship consultants have with particular social categories; and (4) to analyze the consultants’ language ideologies toward syncretic uses of Spanish and Quichua.
    [Show full text]
  • Modelling Mixed Languages: Some Remarks on the Case of Old Helsinki Slang1
    MODELLING MIXED LANGUAGES: SOME REMARKS ON THE CASE OF OLD HELSINKI SLANG1 Merlijn de Smit Stockholm University Abstract Old Helsinki Slang (OHS) a linguistic variety spoken in the working-class quarters of Helsinki from approx. 1900 to 1945, is marked by the usage of a virtually wholly Swedish vocabulary in a Finnish morphosyntactic framework. It has recently been subject of two interestingly contrasting treatments by Petri Kallio and Vesa Jarva. Kallio argues that the morphosyntactic base of OHS gives cause to analyzing it as unambiguously Finnic, and therefore Uralic, from a genetic perspective, whereas Jarva, drawing attention to the possible origins of OHS in frequent code-switching, believes it deserves consideration as a mixed language alongside such cases as Ma’a and Media Lengua. The contrasting approaches of the two authors involve contrasting presuppositions which deserve to be spelled out: should the genetic origin of a language be based on the pedigree of its structure (with mixed structures pointing to a mixed genetic origin) or on the sociolinguistic history of its speakers? Taking the latter course, I argue that the most valid model for the emergence of genetically mixed languages is the code-switching one proposed by Peter Auer. Measuring OHS against Auer’s model, however, it is a marginal case for a mixed language, particularly as Auer’s and similar models imply some composition in structural domains, which seems wholly absent in the case of OHS. Thus OHS is not a genetically mixed language, even if it may have developed as one, had early OHS taken a different course as it eventually did.
    [Show full text]
  • Negotiating Ludic Normativity in Facebook Meme Pages
    in ilburg apers ulture tudies 247 T P C S Negotiating Ludic Normativity in Facebook Meme Pages by Ondřej Procházka Tilburg University [email protected] December 2020 This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nd/4.0/ Negotiating ludic normativity in Facebook meme pages Negotiating ludic normativity in Facebook meme pages PROEFSCHRIFT ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan Tilburg University, op gezag van de rector magnificus, prof. dr. W.B.H.J. van de Donk, in het openbaar te verdedigen ten overstaan van een door het college voor promoties aangewezen commissie in de Portrettenzaal van de Universiteit op maandag 7 december 2020 om 16.00 uur door Ondřej Procházka geboren te Kyjov, Tsjechië Promotores: prof. J.M.E. Blommaert prof. A.M. Backus Copromotor: dr. P.K. Varis Overige leden van de promotiecommissie: prof. A. Georgakopoulou prof. A. Jaworski prof. A.P.C. Swanenberg dr. R. Moore dr. T. Van Hout ISBN 978-94-6416-307-0 Cover design by Veronika Voglová Layout and editing by Karin Berkhout, Department of Culture Studies, Tilburg University Printed by Ridderprint BV, the Netherlands © Ondřej Procházka, 2020 The back cover contains a graphic reinterpretation of the material from the ‘Faceblock’ article posted by user ‘Taha Banoglu’ on the Polandball wiki and is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution- Share Alike License. All rights reserved. No other parts of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any other means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without permission of the author.
    [Show full text]
  • “Goat-Sheep-Mixed-Sign” in Lhasa – Deaf Tibetans' Language Ideologies
    Theresia Hofer “Goat-Sheep-Mixed-Sign” in Lhasa – Deaf Tibetans’ language ideologies and unimodal codeswitching in Tibetan and Chinese sign languages, Tibet Autonomous Region, China 1 Introduction Among Tibetan signers in Lhasa, there is a growing tendency to mix Tibetan Sign Language (TSL) and Chinese Sign Language (CSL). I have been learning TSL from deaf TSL teachers and other deaf, signing Tibetan friends since 2007, but in more recent conversations with them I have been more and more exposed to CSL. In such contexts, signing includes not only loan signs, loan blends or loan trans- lations from CSL that have been used in TSL since its emergence, such as signs for new technical inventions or scientific terms. It also includes codeswitching to CSL lexical items related to core social acts, kinship terms or daily necessities, for which TSL has its own signs, such as for concepts including “to marry”, “mother”, “father”, “teacher”, “house”, “at home”, “real”, “fake”, “wait”, “why”, “thank you” and so on.1 Some Tibetan signers refer to the resulting mixed sign language as “neither- goat-nor-sheep sign” (in Tibetan ra-ma-luk lak-da). This phrase is partly derived from the standard Lhasa Tibetan expression of something or somebody being “neither-goat-nor-sheep” (in Tibetan ra-ma-luk), an expression widely used in the context of codeswitching between Lhasa Tibetan and Putunghua (i.e. stan- dard Chinese) and the resulting “neither-goat-nor-sheep language” (in Tibetan 1 Although the acronym TSL is also used for Taiwan Sign Language and Thai Sign Language, I use it here, because it is used in the English designations of many TSL-related publications written in the Tibetan language, co-authored by deaf Tibetans.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Projections for Canada, 2011 to 2036
    Catalogue no. 89-657-X2017001 ISBN 978-0-660-06842-8 Ethnicity, Language and Immigration Thematic Series Language Projections for Canada, 2011 to 2036 by René Houle and Jean-Pierre Corbeil Release date: January 25, 2017 How to obtain more information For information about this product or the wide range of services and data available from Statistics Canada, visit our website, www.statcan.gc.ca. You can also contact us by email at [email protected] telephone, from Monday to Friday, 8:30 a.m. to 4:30 p.m., at the following numbers: • Statistical Information Service 1-800-263-1136 • National telecommunications device for the hearing impaired 1-800-363-7629 • Fax line 1-514-283-9350 Depository Services Program • Inquiries line 1-800-635-7943 • Fax line 1-800-565-7757 Standards of service to the public Standard table symbols Statistics Canada is committed to serving its clients in a prompt, The following symbols are used in Statistics Canada reliable and courteous manner. To this end, Statistics Canada has publications: developed standards of service that its employees observe. To . not available for any reference period obtain a copy of these service standards, please contact Statistics .. not available for a specific reference period Canada toll-free at 1-800-263-1136. The service standards are ... not applicable also published on www.statcan.gc.ca under “Contact us” > 0 true zero or a value rounded to zero “Standards of service to the public.” 0s value rounded to 0 (zero) where there is a meaningful distinction between true zero and the value that was rounded p preliminary Note of appreciation r revised Canada owes the success of its statistical system to a x suppressed to meet the confidentiality requirements long-standing partnership between Statistics Canada, the of the Statistics Act citizens of Canada, its businesses, governments and other E use with caution institutions.
    [Show full text]
  • Nostratic Grammar: Synthetic Or Analytic?
    Nostratic grammar: synthetic or analytic? A. Dolgopolsky Haifa University § 1. The criteria determining the original analytic status of a gram- matical morpheme can be defined as follows. [1] Mobility: in the daughter languages the same morpheme some- times precedes autosemantic words and sometimes follows them (e. g. the pN *mA that forms participles and other deverbal nominals in daugh- ter-languages is found both before and after the verbal root). [2] In some daughter languages the morpheme is still analytic or has phonological evidence for its former analytic status [e. g. the N morpheme *bA (that forms animal names and other nouns of quality bearers) sur- vives in proto-Uralic *-pa; since the N intervocalic *-b- yields pFU *-w-, while the word-initial *b- yields U *p-, the Uralic suffix *pa must go back to a separate word]. [3] A grammatical morpheme of daughter-languages is etymologically identical with a separate word (e. g. the 1st person ending *-mi of the IE verb is identical with the N pronoun *mi that is preserved as a pronoun in daughter-languages). Sometimes other typological features (position in the word) may be taken into account. For instance, if in Semitic and Cushitic (Beja) verbs the feminine marker (*-ī) is used in the 2nd person only and is separated (as a suffix) from the person marker (prefix *t-), it is natural to suppose that it goes back to an address word (Hebrew tiħyī 'you' [f. sg.] will live' < N *t{u} 'thou' + *Хay{u} 'live' + *ʔ{a}yV 'mother'), which is typologically compara- ble with the gender distinction of a yes-word in spoken English: [yesз:] (< yes, sir) vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Dutch - Indonesian Language Mixing in Jakarta
    Dutch - Indonesian language mixing in Jakarta Herman Giesbers 1. Introduction Within the field of language contact and language change we see an increasing interest in the phenomenon of language mixing or language intertwining (Bakker 1992). See e.g. Bakker & Mous (1994) for a recent overview of several mixed languages. Bakker (1992:187) proposes a definition of a mixed language as '(•••) a language which shows positive genetic similarities, in significant numbers, with two different languages.' Such mixed languages can be related to two different language families. However, they differ from Pidgins and Creoles in that they combine the grammar of an existing language A with the lexicon of a language B. Thus, the central process in language intertwining is not replacement, but combination. Which language is language A or B is totally determined by social factors. Language A providing the grammatical system for the mixed language, is always the better known language, which is also the language of the closest outsiders. In general, this language A is the language of the mothers in mixed families while language B providing the lexicon, is the language of the fathers. Bakker predicts from this model of intertwining that in a mixed language the grammar (syntax, morphology, phonology) is from language A, the lexicon from language B, and "free grammatical morphemes" or "function words" from both or from either of the languages. Another way to look at the process of language mixing is suggested by Myers-Scotton (1992). She claims that her Matrix Language Frame (MLF) model of code-switching can also account for other manifestations of language contact including the formation of 'new' languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Field-Testing Code-Switching Constraints: a Report on a Strategic Languages Project
    languages Article Field-Testing Code-Switching Constraints: A Report on a Strategic Languages Project John M. Lipski Department of Spanish, Italian and Portuguese, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16801, USA; [email protected]; Tel.: +1-814-865-6583 Received: 28 November 2017; Accepted: 28 January 2019; Published: 1 February 2019 Abstract: The present article provides an overview of ongoing field-based research that deploys a variety of interactive experimental procedures in three strategically chosen bilingual contact environments, whose language dyads facilitate a partial separation of morphosyntactic factors in order to test the extent to which proposed grammatical constraints on intra-sentential code-switching are independent of language-specific factors. For purposes of illustration, the possibility of language switches between subject pronouns and verbs is compared for the three bilingual groups. The first scenario includes Ecuadoran Quichua and Media Lengua (entirely Quichua syntax and system morphology, all lexical roots replaced by Spanish items; both are null-subject languages). The second juxtaposes Spanish and the Afro-Colombian creole language Palenquero; the languages share highly cognate lexicons but differ substantially in grammatical structures (including null subjects in Spanish, only overt subjects in Palenquero). Spanish and Portuguese in north-eastern Argentina along the Brazilian border form the third focus: lexically and grammatically highly cognate languages that are nonetheless kept distinct by speakers (both null-subject languages, albeit with different usage patterns). Results from the three communities reveal a residual resistance against PRONOUN + VERB switches irrespective of the subject-verb configuration, thereby motivating the application of similar techniques to other proposed grammatical constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • Glish Is No Glitch: Spanish-English Contact Phenomena in Advertising Copy Katherine Hagan April 20, 2009
    The ‘Glish is no Glitch: Spanish-English Contact Phenomena in Advertising Copy Katherine Hagan April 20, 2009 Potato, potahto, tomato, tomahto: some customers call their favorite cereal “Corn flakes,” while others reach for los cornflais (Ilan Stavans 2005: 102). Faced with language choices, advertisers might be calling the whole thing off. The fast food chain Chick-Fil-A combines English and Spanish in their caricatured cow-friendly calendar: “Chikin es mooey gud.” Fashion magazine Ocean Drive Español promises “lo más fashion para la playa (the most fashion for the beach),” and hosts headlines on the “hoteles con star power (hotels with star power)” (italics theirs). The cows are painting it. The fashionistas are styling it. “Spanglish,” according to the title of Ilan Stavans’ 2003 book on the topic, is “the new American language.” The present study examines competing definitions of “Spanglish.” After reviewing various English-Spanish contact phenomena, the presence of code-switching, calques, Spanglish neologisms, and bilingual translation in print advertising are examined. Qualitative analysis serves to assess the motivations, rules, and ramifications of meshing Spanish and English in the media. Such analysis reveals that the function of advertising language is often more symbolic than referential. Close readings of the ad also serve as an additional account of linguistic correlates of “Spanglish” in the hybrid communication between advertiser and consumer. Preface Upon every return to my native Atlanta, I serve a couple shifts as a waitress in a local soul food restaurant. The menu is traditionally Southern: barbecue beef brisket with black-eyed peas and turnip greens. The best fried-green tomatoes on this side of the Mississippi—claim the most consistent customers—and all the cornbread and yeast rolls you can eat.
    [Show full text]
  • Contact Languages Around the World and Their Levels of Endangerment
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by ScholarSpace at University of Hawai'i at Manoa Vol. 12 (2018), pp. 53–79 http://nflrc.hawaii.edu/ldc http://hdl.handle.net/10125/24764 Revised Version Received: 23 Jan 2017 Contact languages around the world and their levels of endangerment Nala H. Lee National University of Singapore This paper provides an up-to-date report on the vitality or endangerment status of contact languages around the world, including pidgins, creoles, and mixed lan- guages. By utilizing information featured in the Endangered Languages Project and the Atlas of Pidgin and Creole Languages online portals, 96 contact languages are assessed on the Language Endangerment Index, a method of assessment that is based on four factors including intergenerational transmission, absolute num- ber of speakers, speaker number trends, and domains of use. Results show that the contact languages are most at risk with respect to intergenerational transmis- sion and domains of use. This is explained by the social and historical nature of contact languages. Overall results further raise the concern that the proportion of pidgins, creoles and mixed languages at some level of risk is extremely high. Rea- sons are provided for why linguists should be concerned about the endangerment of these languages. 1. Introduction1 While the language endangerment problem has been generally well- highlighted in linguistics, the endangerment of contact languages has not received the same level of attention. Krauss (1992) postulates that at least half and possibly as much as 90% of the world’s languages will no longer be spoken by the end of the present century, while a more recent empirical study estimates a slightly less catas- trophic rate of loss, at one language every three months (Campbell et al.
    [Show full text]
  • MIXED CODES, BILINGUALISM, and LANGUAGE MAINTENANCE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requi
    BILINGUAL NAVAJO: MIXED CODES, BILINGUALISM, AND LANGUAGE MAINTENANCE DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Charlotte C. Schaengold, M.A. ***** The Ohio State University 2004 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Professor Brian Joseph, Advisor Professor Donald Winford ________________________ Professor Keith Johnson Advisor Linguistics Graduate Program ABSTRACT Many American Indian Languages today are spoken by fewer than one hundred people, yet Navajo is still spoken by over 100,000 people and has maintained regional as well as formal and informal dialects. However, the language is changing. While the Navajo population is gradually shifting from Navajo toward English, the “tip” in the shift has not yet occurred, and enormous efforts are being made in Navajoland to slow the language’s decline. One symptom in this process of shift is the fact that many young people on the Reservation now speak a non-standard variety of Navajo called “Bilingual Navajo.” This non-standard variety of Navajo is the linguistic result of the contact between speakers of English and speakers of Navajo. Similar to Michif, as described by Bakker and Papen (1988, 1994, 1997) and Media Lengua, as described by Muysken (1994, 1997, 2000), Bilingual Navajo has the structure of an American Indian language with parts of its lexicon from a European language. “Bilingual mixed languages” are defined by Winford (2003) as languages created in a bilingual speech community with the grammar of one language and the lexicon of another. My intention is to place Bilingual Navajo into the historical and theoretical framework of the bilingual mixed language, and to explain how ii this language can be used in the Navajo speech community to help maintain the Navajo language.
    [Show full text]
  • Spanish-Based Creoles in the Caribbean
    Spanish-based creoles in the Caribbean John M. Lipski The Pennsylvania State University Introduction The Caribbean Basin is home to many creole languages, lexically related to French, English, and—now only vestigially—Dutch. Surrounded by Spanish-speaking nations, and with Portuguese-speaking Brazil not far to the south, the Caribbean contains only a single creole language derived from a (highly debated) combination of Spanish and Portuguese, namely Papiamentu, spoken on the Netherlands Antilles islands of Curaçao and Aruba. If the geographical confines of the designation `Caribbean’ are pushed a bit, the creole language Palenquero, spoken in the Afro-Colombian village Palenque de San Basilio, near the port of Cartagena de Indias, also qualifies as a Spanish-related creole, again with a hotly contested Portuguese component. There are also a number of small Afro-Hispanic enclaves scattered throughout the Caribbean where ritual language, songs, and oral traditions suggest at least some partial restructuring of Spanish in small areas. Finally, there exists a controversial but compelling research paradigm which asserts that Spanish as spoken by African slaves and their immediate descendents may have creolized in the 19th century Spanish Caribbean—particularly in Cuba—and that this putative creole language may have subsequently merged with local varieties of Spanish, leaving a faint but detectable imprint on general Caribbean Spanish. A key component of the inquiry into Spanish-related contact varieties is the recurring claim that all such languages derive from earlier Portuguese-based pidgins and creoles, formed somewhere in West Africa1 and carried to the Americas by slaves transshipped from African holding stations, and by ships’ crews and slave traders.
    [Show full text]