A Multistudy Examination of Links Between Receiving

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Multistudy Examination of Links Between Receiving SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 1 Shame on me for needing you: A multistudy examination of links between receiving interpersonal emotion regulation and experiencing shame Benjamin A. Swerdlow, Devon Sandel, Sheri L. Johnson University of California, Berkeley Author Note Benjamin A. Swerdlow, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. Devon Sandel, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. Sheri L. Johnson, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley. Support for the lead author’s time was provided by NIMH T32 MH020006-16A1 (PIs: Sheri Johnson & James Gross). We would like to express our gratitude to Drs. Iris Mauss and Dacher Keltner for their valuable feedback on earlier drafts of this manuscript. Portions of these data were presented at the 2018 annual meeting of the Society for Affective Science. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Benjamin A. Swerdlow, Department of Psychology, 2121 Berkeley Way West #1650, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720. Email can be sent to: [email protected]. This is a preprint of a manuscript that is currently undergoing peer review. Do not cite without the authors’ permission. SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 2 Abstract Recent theory and research have drawn attention to interpersonal dimensions of emotion regulation. Yet, few empirical investigations of the outcomes of interpersonal emotion regulation have been conducted. We propose that one negative affective outcome of received interpersonal emotion regulation of conceptual and practical interest is shame. In the present series of studies, participants from six, disparate samples were asked to report on experiences of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation using autobiographical recall and ecological sampling paradigms (total analyzed n = 1868; total analyzed k = 2515 instances of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation). We sought to quantify the frequency and distinctiveness of shame as an outcome of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation. We used an exploratory-confirmatory approach to identify robust and generalizable correlates of shame. We considered individual (e.g., trait external shame-proneness), situational (e.g., desire for regulation), relational (e.g., perceived closeness with the provider) and interaction-specific (e.g., perceptions of provider hostility) variables. Our results indicate that it is not uncommon for people to experience receiving interpersonal emotion regulation as shame-inducing, and these perceptions are distinct from their evaluations of the overall effectiveness of the interaction. The most robust correlates of shame across studies and samples were interaction-specific ratings of responsiveness and hostility, which were negatively and positively correlated with shame, respectively. We discuss the conceptual, methodological, and practical implications of these findings for studying interpersonal emotion regulation and shame. Keywords: interpersonal emotion regulation; shame; shame-proneness SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 3 Shame on me for needing you: A multistudy examination of the association between receiving interpersonal emotion regulation and experiences of shame Imagine the following three scenarios. (1) You ask a close friend for support before a nerve-wracking exam and are bolstered by their expressions of confidence in your abilities. (2) You consider venting your frustrations to a coworker after a challenging day at work, but you worry that expressing your negative emotions could reflect poorly on you. (3) You approach a loved one for comfort after a loss but are confounded by their dismissiveness. These three scenarios reflect the diversity of experiences of seeking interpersonal emotion regulation, emphasizing not only the potential of interpersonal emotion regulation to convey emotional benefits, but also ways in which it may go awry, yielding negative personal or social outcomes for the provider, the receiver, or both. Whereas emotion regulation research traditionally has focused on intrapersonal processes (Campos, Walle, Dahl, & Main, 2011), there is now considerable interest in interpersonal processes involved in emotion regulation (e.g., Dixon-Gordon, Bernecker, & Christensen, 2015; Netzer, Van Kleef, and Tamir, 2015; Niven, 2017; Zaki & Williams, 2013), consistent with compelling evidence that social and emotional functioning are fundamentally reciprocal (Beckes & Coan, 2012). One result is the emerging field of interpersonal emotion regulation research, which focuses on the “slice of interpersonal interactions deliberately devoted to influencing one's own (intrinsic) or others’ (extrinsic) emotions” (Dixon-Gordon, Bernecker, & Christensen, 2015, p. 37). In the present study of interpersonal emotion regulation, we focused on experiences of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation of negative emotions. SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 4 Research indicates that interpersonal emotion regulation is used regularly in everyday life and can effectively downregulate momentary negative affect (e.g., Heiy and Cheavens, 2014; Levy-Gigi and Shamay-Tsoory, 2017). Interpersonal emotion regulation tendencies also have been linked cross-sectionally to wellbeing and adjustment to stress (e.g., Cheung, Gardner, & Anderson, 2015; Horn & Maercker, 2016) and cross-sectionally and longitudinally to social connectedness (e.g., Debrot, Schoebi, Perrez, & Horn, 2013; Rose, Carlson, & Waller, 2007; Williams, Morelli, Ong, & Zaki, 2018). Potential positive outcomes of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation notwithstanding, interpersonal emotion regulation also entails a host of social and communication challenges compared to intrapersonal emotion regulation efforts. For example, effective interpersonal emotion regulation may depend on communicating one’s emotion regulation goals to another person and the other apprehending those emotion goals and responding accordingly. Katherine Dixon-Gordon and colleagues (2015) discuss such challenges in terms of a transactional process of encoding and decoding. Problems arising during either encoding or decoding could yield negative outcomes. Prior research has suggested that some manifestations of interpersonal emotion regulation do produce undesirable outcomes. For example, tendencies to engage in co-rumination and co- brooding, which involve dwelling on negative emotions and problems with another person, have been found to be associated with and to predict symptoms and episodes of depression in adolescents (e.g., Rose, Carlson, & Waller, 2007; Spendelow, Simonds, & Avery, 2017; Stone, Hankin, Gibb, & Abela, 2011) and poorer adjustment to a recent major life stressor in adult romantic couples (Horn & Maercker, 2016). Self-reported tendencies to engage in reassurance- seeking in hypothetical interpersonal emotion scenarios predicted greater self-injury over the SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 5 course of a two-week daily diary study (Dixon-Gordon, Haliczer, Conkey, & Whalen, 2018). Among mother-daughter dyads, the combination of high maternal problem-solving and low maternal validation during conflict conversations significantly moderated the relationship between daughters’ negative affect and daughters’ borderline symptoms, such that daughters’ negative affect was only associated with borderline symptoms when maternal problem-solving was high and maternal validation was low (Dixon-Gordon et al., 2017). These findings suggest that some patterns of interpersonal emotion regulation may be associated with greater distress. One potential specific negative outcome of receiving interpersonal emotion regulation is that of shame. Shame has been conceptualized as a self-conscious, aversive emotion associated with negative attributions about the whole self (e.g., I am a terrible person; Izard, 1971; Tangney, 1991; Tracy & Robins, 2007). Shame is thought to be triggered by failures to attain standards or ideals, particularly when we simultaneously perceive ourselves as negatively evaluated by others and our role in the social hierarchy as threatened (Gilbert, 2003; Gruenewald, Kemeny, Aziz, & Fahey, 2004; Tracy & Robins, 2007). For example, shame has been theorized to be elicited by situations in which social rank and power are threatened, potentially motivating submission displays and social withdrawal as a means of appeasement (Gilbert, 2003; Gruenewald, Kemeny, Aziz, & Fahey, 2004; Martens, Tracy, & Shariff, 2012; but see also de Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, 2010 for an account of how and when shame can motivate approach behavior). There are compelling conceptual reasons to think that interpersonal emotion regulation could induce shame on the part of the receiver. Seeking interpersonal emotion regulation effectively places the seeker in the role of supplicant, conversely placing the provider in a position of authority to either grant or deny the seeker’s request and thereby producing power SHAME AND INTERPERSONAL EMOTION REGULATION Page 6 asymmetry (Anderson & Berdahl, 2002). At the same time, even the most genuine proffer of interpersonal emotion regulation may convey an implicit message of deficiency or inadequacy. Consistent with this notion, visible acts of social support can undermine receivers’ self-esteem and self-efficacy precisely because such acts can convey and evoke negative evaluations regarding receivers’ ability to cope and foster concerns about indebtedness (Bolger, Zuckermann,
Recommended publications
  • Bad Is Stronger Than Good
    Review of General Psychology Copyright 2001 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2001. Vol. 5. No. 4. 323-370 1089-2680/O1/S5.O0 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323 Bad Is Stronger Than Good Roy F. Baumeister and Ellen Bratslavsky Catrin Finkenauer Case Western Reserve University Free University of Amsterdam Kathleen D. Vohs Case Western Reserve University The greater power of bad events over good ones is found in everyday events, major life events (e.g., trauma), close relationship outcomes, social network patterns, interper- sonal interactions, and learning processes. Bad emotions, bad parents, and bad feedback have more impact than good ones, and bad information is processed more thoroughly than good. The self is more motivated to avoid bad self-definitions than to pursue good ones. Bad impressions and bad stereotypes are quicker to form and more resistant to disconfirmation than good ones. Various explanations such as diagnosticity and sa- lience help explain some findings, but the greater power of bad events is still found when such variables are controlled. Hardly any exceptions (indicating greater power of good) can be found. Taken together, these findings suggest that bad is stronger than good, as a general principle across a broad range of psychological phenomena. Centuries of literary efforts and religious pothesis that bad is stronger than good (see also thought have depicted human life in terms of a Rozin & Royzman, in press). That is, events struggle between good and bad forces. At the that are negatively valenced (e.g., losing metaphysical level, evil gods or devils are the money, being abandoned by friends, and receiv- opponents of the divine forces of creation and ing criticism) will have a greater impact on the harmony.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information Company 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 9427762 Examining the relationship of negative affectivity and subjective well-being to goal-setting processes and task performance Necowitz, Lawrence B., Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 32 (2016) 25–37
    Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 32 (2016) 25–37 Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology www.elsevier.es/rpto Counterproductive work behavior among frontline government employees: Role of personality, emotional intelligence, affectivity, emotional labor, and emotional exhaustion a b,∗ c Ponniah Raman , Murali Sambasivan , Naresh Kumar a Putra Business School, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Malaysia b Taylor’s Business School, Taylor’s University Lakeside Campus, Subang Jaya, Malaysia c Global Entrepreneurship Research and Innovation Center, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Technology Park Malaysia, Bukit Jalil, Malaysia a b s t r a c t a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: The main objective of this research is to study the effect of personality, emotional intelligence (EI), affectiv- Received 7 January 2015 ity, emotional labor and emotional exhaustion on counterproductive work behavior (CWB) of frontline Accepted 24 November 2015 employees in the government sector. A questionnaire was designed and distributed to 625 frontline Available online 28 January 2016 employees working at service counters in 25 ministries in Malaysia. We received responses from 519 employees (response rate = 83%). The data was analyzed using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). The Keywords: main findings are: (1) personality factors of employees drive their EI, affectivity, emotional labor, emo- Counterproductive work behavior tional exhaustion, and CWB and (2) EI and affectivity impact emotional labor, emotional exhaustion and Personality traits CWB. Through the integrated model, we have studied the indirect roles of emotional labor and emotional Affectivity exhaustion. This is one of the few studies that have effectively integrated the five constructs into a single Emotional labor Emotional exhaustion framework to study their effects on CWB.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect As Predictors of Facial
    Anuario de Psicología ISSN: 0066-5126 [email protected] Universitat de Barcelona España Rus-Calafell, M.; Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J.; Frerich, N. Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect as predictors of Facial Emotion Recognition Capability using static and dynamic images Anuario de Psicología, vol. 43, núm. 1, abril, 2013, pp. 7-21 Universitat de Barcelona Barcelona, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=97027472001 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Anuario de Psicología/The UB Journal of Psychology 2013, vol. 43, nº 1, 7-21 © 2013, Facultat de Psicologia Universitat de Barcelona Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect as predictors of Facial Emotion Recognition Capability using static and dynamic images M. Rus-Calafell J. Gutiérrez-Maldonado N. Frerich Universitat de Barcelona The main purpose of the present study is to investigate the capacity of schizotypy and alexithymia traits, in combination with affectivity to predict facial emotion recognition capability in a sample of nonclinical adults. Consecutive healthy participants (N= 98) were investigated using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20), the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences-Reduced Version (O-LIFE-R), and the Positive and NA Schedule (PANAS). A set of validated photographs (static images) and virtual faces (dynamic images) for presenting the basic emotions was used to assess emotion recognition. Pearson correlations were applied to investigate the relationship between the study variables; the amount of variance in emotion recognition capability predicted by OLIFE-R, TAS-20 and PANAS was calculated by using the linear regression model.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Characteristics and Physiological Reactivity to Acute Stress in Mothers of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
    Received: 28 September 2018 Revised: 19 February 2019 Accepted: 19 April 2019 DOI: 10.1002/smi.2870 RESEARCH ARTICLE Psychological characteristics and physiological reactivity to acute stress in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder Elena Pattini1 | Luca Carnevali2 | Alfonso Troisi3 | Guido Matrella4 | Dolores Rollo5 | Mauro Fornari6 | Andrea Sgoifo2 1 Centro per la Cura, la Diagnosi e lo Studio dei Disturbi della Comunicazione e della Abstract Socializzazione, Ausl Parma, Parma, Italy Stress related to parenting a child with autism spectrum disorder can differently 2 Stress Physiology Lab, Department of affect caregiver's physiological reactivity to acute stress. Here, parental stress levels, Chemistry, Life Sciences and Environmental Sustainability, University of Parma, Parma, Italy psychological characteristics, and coping strategies were assessed alongside measures 3 Department of Systems Medicine, University of heart rate, heart rate variability, and cortisol during a psychosocial stress test in of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy mothers of children with ASD (M‐ASD, n = 15) and mothers of typically developing 4 Department of Engineering and Architecture, University of Parma, Parma, Italy children (n = 15). M‐ASD reported significantly higher levels of parental stress, anxi- 5 Department of Medicine and Surgery, ety, negative affectivity, social inhibition, and a larger preference for avoidance strat- University of Parma, Parma, Italy egies. M‐ASD showed larger heart rate and cortisol responses to the psychosocial 6 Collegio
    [Show full text]
  • Correlation Between Social Anxiety and Negative Affect
    The International Journal of Indian Psychology ISSN 2348-5396 (Online) | ISSN: 2349-3429 (Print) Volume 8, Issue 3, July- Sep, 2020 DIP: 18.01.037/20200803, DOI: 10.25215/0803.037 http://www.ijip.in Research Paper Correlation between social anxiety and negative affect Anushtha Mishra1* ABSTRACT Social Anxiety Disorder is the third largest mental health care problem in the world today. Social anxiety (SA) has been studied widely in terms of epidemiology, aetiology, pathology, comorbidity, etc. There have been few recent studies that have tried to assess the relationship between social anxiety and negative affect (NA) including diminished positive affect. However, very few studies have been conducted to specifically study the correlation SA and NA share by examining specific negative affective states. The aim of this research is to study the relationship between SA and NA in detail. The sample size is 101 (male = 26, Female = 75). Ages ranges were 18 to 25 yrs. (young adults). Purposive sampling through online platform was used. A google form was created consisting informed consent, demographic details, Leibowitz Social Anxiety Scale – SR (LSAS-SR) and Positive Affect Negative Affect Schedule – SF (PANAS-SF), which was sent to the participants. Mean scores of SA and NA were 59 and 26.9 respectively. Pearson’s correlation between SA and NA was .467 (p<0.01). A moderately strong correlation of SA was found with affective states of nervousness (.508, p<0.01) and afraid (.419, p<0.01) followed by guilty (.349, p<0.01), distressed (.325, p<0.01) and ashamed (.303, p<0.01).
    [Show full text]
  • Impact of Negative Affectivity and Trait Forgiveness on Aortic Blood Pressure and Coronary Circulation
    Psychophysiology, •• (2014), ••–••. Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Printed in the USA. Copyright © 2014 Society for Psychophysiological Research DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12325 BRIEF REPORT Impact of negative affectivity and trait forgiveness on aortic blood pressure and coronary circulation MARCOS A. SANCHEZ-GONZALEZ,a,b ROSS W. MAY,a ANDREW P. KOUTNIK,c and FRANK D. FINCHAMa aFamily Institute, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA bOffice of Clinical Research, Larkin Community Hospital, South Miami, Florida, USA cDepartment of Biomedical Sciences, College of Medicine, Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA Abstract Prior research suggests that negative affectivity (NA) may have a direct adverse effect on coronary circulation, whereas forgiveness may provide cardioprotection. This study examined whether NA and forgiveness were independently related to aortic hemodynamics and the subendocardial viability index (SVI), a marker of coronary perfusion. A sample of 131 adults (M = 21.11 years, SD = 2.52) were evaluated for NA (depression, anxiety, and anger symptoms) and forgiveness (Tendency to Forgive Scale; TTF). Aortic hemodynamic parameters via applanation tonometry were assessed at rest and during sympathostimulation (cold pressor test; CPT). Hierarchical multiple regression analyses of resting values showed that NA was related to higher aortic blood pressure (ABP) and lower SVI. After controlling for demographics and for NA, TTF scores were significantly associated with decreased ABP, but increased SVI. CPT changes from baseline
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Emotions and the Emotional Disorders: a Quantitative
    International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology ISSN: 1697-2600 [email protected] Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual España Watson, David; Clark, Lee Anna; Stasik, Sara M. Emotions and the emotional disorders: A quantitative hierarchical perspective International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, vol. 11, núm. 3, 2011, pp. 429-442 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual Granada, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=33719289001 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative © International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology ISSN 1697-2600 print ISSN 2174-0852 online 2011, Vol. 11, Nº 3, pp. 429-442 Emotions and the emotional disorders: A quantitative hierarchical perspective David Watson1, Lee Anna Clark, and Sara M. Stasik (University of Notre Dame, USA) ABSTRACT. Previous evidence has established that general negative affect represents a non-specific factor common to both anxiety and depression, whereas low positive affect is more specifically related to the latter. Little is known, however, about how specific, lower order affects relate to these constructs. We investigated how six emotional disorders—major depression, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), panic disorder, social phobia, and obsessive compulsive disorder — are linked to both general and specific types of affect in two samples (Ns = 331 and 253), using the Expanded Form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS- X). Replicating previous results, the General Negative Affect scale was nonspecifically related to the emotional disorders, whereas General Positive Affect had a specific (inverse) association with major depression.
    [Show full text]
  • Future Research Direction on Decoding Animal Well-Being: the Case of the Horse
    GENERAL COMMENTARY published: 10 March 2014 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00173 Future research direction on decoding animal well-being: the case of the horse Nutankumar S. Thingujam* Department of Psychology, Sikkim University, Gangtok, India *Correspondence: [email protected] Edited by: Mattie Tops, VU University Amsterdam, Netherlands Reviewed by: Clémence Lesimple, Laboratoire EthoS—UMR 6552, France Keywords: animal wellbeing, observation, gender, age, personality, affectivity A commentary on of the decoder. But, such research attempt was associated with the higher accu- requires several observers to enable the racy of recognition of several emotional How accurate are we at assessing others’ use of inferential statistical analysis. For expressions. In particular, overall emotion well-being? The example of welfare assess- example, 30 male and 30 female trained recognition from the human face was asso- ment in horses observers could serve as the observers of ciated positively with conscientiousness, by Lesimple, C., and Hausberger, M. (2014). the horse’s SB/ARB with affectivity and openness to experience, extraversion, but Front. Psychol. 5:21. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg. personality measures as controlled vari- negatively with neuroticism (Matsumoto 2014.00021 ables. Alternatively, 30 young (say, 20–30 et al., 2000). Similarly, other researchers years) and 30 (say, 60 and above years) old also reported that human emotion decod- The study by Lesimple and Hausberger trained observers could form the sample ing of sadness, happiness, and contempt (2014) compared in 26 cases the decod- for a study in which affectivity and person- from the facial expression was predicted ingofwellbeingofthehorsebyacare- ality could be used as controlled variables. by openness to experience; sadness decod- taker through a questionnaire on the one ing by agreeableness; anger recognition OBSERVERS’ GENDER AND DECODING hand and an experienced outside observer by extraversion (Mill et al., 2009).
    [Show full text]
  • Rumination, Emotion, and Forgiveness: Three Longitudinal Studies
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 92, No. 3, 490–505 0022-3514/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.490 Rumination, Emotion, and Forgiveness: Three Longitudinal Studies Michael E. McCullough, Giacomo Bono, and Lindsey M. Root University of Miami In 3 studies, the authors investigated whether within-persons increases in rumination about an interper- sonal transgression were associated with within-persons reductions in forgiveness. Results supported this hypothesis. The association of transient increases in rumination with transient reductions in forgiveness appeared to be mediated by anger, but not fear, toward the transgressor. The association of rumination and forgiveness was not confounded by daily fluctuations in positive affect and negative affect, and it was not moderated by trait levels of positive affectivity, negative affectivity, or perceived hurtfulness of the transgression. Cross-lagged associations of rumination and forgiveness in Study 3 more consistently supported the proposition that increased rumination precedes reductions in forgiveness than the propo- sition that increased forgiveness precedes reductions in rumination. Keywords: forgiveness, rumination, emotion, personality, longitudinal For some of us whose unforgiving emotions lurk beneath a barely man, and Sherwood (2003) defined rumination more generally as civil surface, allowing ourselves to recall our hurts and offenses can a coping strategy characterized by a “passive and repetitive focus plunge us straight into rumination and thus bitter unforgiveness. To on the negative and damaging features of a stressful transaction” forgive, we must recall the hurt and offense, but we must do it (p. 242). Aggression researchers in particular have discussed ru- differently than we usually do.
    [Show full text]
  • Moral Injury and Moral Repair in War Veterans: a Preliminary Model and Intervention Strategy
    Clinical Psychology Review 29 (2009) 695–706 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Clinical Psychology Review Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: A preliminary model and intervention strategy a,b, a a c c Brett T. Litz ⁎, Nathan Stein , Eileen Delaney , Leslie Lebowitz , William P. Nash , a d Caroline Silva , Shira Maguen a National Center for PTSD, VA Boston Healthcare System, United States b Boston University, United States c Newton, Massachusetts, United States d San Francisco VA Medical Center, University of California at San Francisco, United States article info abstract Keywords: Throughout history, warriors have been confronted with moral and ethical challenges and modern unconventional Moral injury and guerilla wars amplify these challenges. Potentially morally injurious events, such as perpetrating, failing to Iraq War prevent, or bearing witness to acts that transgress deeply held moral beliefs and expectations may be deleterious in Afghanistan the long-term, emotionally, psychologically, behaviorally, spiritually, and socially (what we label as moral injury). OIF Although there has been some research on the consequences of unnecessary acts of violence in war zones, the OEF lasting impact of morally injurious experience in war remains chiefly unaddressed. To stimulate a critical examination of moral injury, we review the available literature, define terms, and offer a working conceptual framework and a set of intervention strategies designed to repair moral injury. Published by Elsevier Ltd. Contents 1. Introduction .............................................................. 696 2. What might be potentially morally injurious in war? ........................................... 696 3. Research on military atrocities and killing ................................................ 697 4. What aspects of existing theory might explain moral injury? ....................................... 698 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Character Strengths and Psychological Wellbeing Among Students of Teacher Education Josep Gustems & Caterina Calderon1
    Instructions for authors, subscriptions and further details: http://ijep.hipatiapress.com Character Strengths and Psychological Wellbeing among Students of Teacher Education Josep Gustems & Caterina Calderon1 1) University of Barcelona, Spain Date of publication: October 24th, 2014 Edition period: October 2014 - February 2015 To cite this article: Gustems, J., & Calderon, C. (2014). Character strengths and psychological wellbeing among students of teacher education. International Journal of Educational Psychology, 3(3), 265-286. doi: 10.4471/ijep.2014.14 To link this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.4471/ijep.2014.14 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE The terms and conditions of use are related to the Open Journal System and to Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY). IJEP – International Journal of Educational Psychology, Vol. 3 No. 3 October 2014 pp. 265-286 Character Strengths and Psychological Wellbeing among Students of Teacher Education Josep Gustems and Caterina Calderon University of Barcelona Abstract The relation between character strengths and psychological well-being can have an important effect on students’ academic performance. We examined relationships between character strengths and psychological well-being as assessed by the Values in Action Inventory of Strengths and Brief Symptom Inventory. A sample of 98 teacher education students participated. The participants showed high scores in character strength scales. The five character strengths with the highest scores were kindness, fairness, teamwork, love, and honesty. The participants scored higher in character strengths that focused on other people than in the strengths that focused on the self, and higher on the so-called “strengths of the heart” than on “strengths of the head”.
    [Show full text]