Positive and Negative Affectivity and Their Relation to Anxiety and Depressive Disorders

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Positive and Negative Affectivity and Their Relation to Anxiety and Depressive Disorders Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1988 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 1988, Vol. 97, No. 3, 346-333 0021-843X/88/$00.75 Positive and Negative Affectivity and Their Relation to Anxiety and Depressive Disorders David Watson and Lee Anna Clark Southern Methodist University Greg Carey Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado Distinguishing between depression and anxiety has been a matter of concern and controversy for some time. Studies in normal samples have suggested, however, that assessment of two broad mood factors—Negative Aflect (NA) and Positive Affect (PA)—may improve their differentiation. The present study extends these findings to a clinical sample. As part of an ongoing twin study, 90 inpa- tient probands and 60 cotwins were interviewed with the anxiety and depression sections of the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Robins, Helzer, Croughan, & Ratcliff, 1981). Respondents also completed trait NA and PA scales. Consistent with previous research, NA was broadly correlated with symptoms and diagnoses of both anxiety and depression, and acted as a general predictor of psychiatric disorder. In contrast, PA was consistently related (negatively) only to symptoms and diagnoses of depression, indicating that the loss of pleasurable engagement is a distinctive feature of depression. The results suggest that strengthening the PA component in depression measures may enhance their discriminative power. Oinicians have long been concerned by the conceptual and as they are among themselves, and thus often load on a single, empirical overlap between anxiety and depression, and have re- undifferentiated factor, together with measures of hostility and cently devoted considerable attention to their differentiation anger, neuroticism, physical complaints, repression-sensitiza- (e.g., Akiskal, 1985; Breier, Charney, & Heninger. 1985). Al- tion, irrational beliefs, and (on the opposite pole) with ego though the association is strong enough to suggest to some re- strength and social desirability (e.g., Dobson, 1985; Gotlib, searchers that they are variants of a single disorder, most clini- 1984; Mendels et al., 1972; Tanaka-Matsumi & Kameoka, cians and researchers in the area continue to believe that the 1986). Many investigators have concluded that all of these mea- basic distinction is valid (e.g., Akiskal, 1985;Foa&Foa, 1982). sures tap a common, underlying construct of Negative Affectiv- A complete discussion of this literature is beyond the scope of ity, Neuroticism, or General Psychological Distress (Eysenck, our article, but we will present evidence illustrating the magni- 1970; Gotlib, 1984; Watson & Clark, 1984; Zurawski & Smith, tude of the problem. 1987). Numerous studies have demonstrated that self-report anxiety These findings obviously cause interpretive problems for re- and depression scales are highly correlated, typically in the search involving self-report scales and suggest that they offer range of .40 to .70. This finding is, moreover, both robust and little help in differential diagnosis. Indeed, "anxiety" scales general: Such correlations have been reported in college stu- have been found to correlate as highly with clinical ratings of dents (Dobson, 1985; Gotlib, 1984;Nezu,Nezu,&Nezu, 1986; depression as they do with anxiety; and conversely, "depres- Tanaka-Matsumi & Kameoka, 1986), children (Blumberg & sion" scales are as strongly correlated with clinically rated anxi- Izard, 1986; Wolfe et al., 1987), normal adults (Orme, Reis, & ety as depression (e.g., Deluty, Deluty, & Carver, 1986; Zucker- Herz, 1986), and diverse patient samples (Bouman & Luteijn, man, Persky, Eckman, & Hopkins, 1967). It would be a mistake, 1986; Mendels, Weinstein, & Cochrane, 1972; Zurawski & however, to conclude that these data simply reflect the limita- Smith, 1987). The correlations are often high enough to suggest tions of self-report. Clinicians' and teachers' ratings of anxiety that they tap a single construct. In fact, different measures of and depression are also strongly correlated (Deluty et al., 1986; anxiety and depression are as highly correlated with each other Foa et al., 1983; Lipman, Rickels, Covi, Derogatis, & Uhlen- huth, 1969; Wolfe etal., 1987;Zuckermanetal., 1967), anxious and depressive symptoms co-occur in many patients (Dero- This research was supported in part by National Institute of Mental gatis, Klerman, & Lipman, 1972; Roth, Guraey, Garside, & Health Grant MH14677. Kerr, 1972), and comorbidity of the full clinical syndromes oc- We are deeply indebted to Irving I. Gottesman, Codirector of the curs in about half of all patients with anxiety or depressive diag- Washington University Twin Series, for his help in securing the data noses (e.g., Breier, Charney, & Heninger, 1986; Woodruff, Guze, used in this study. & Clayton, 1972; for reviews, see Breier et al., 1985; Clark, in Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to David Watson, Department of Psychology, Southern Methodist University, press; Gersh & Fowles, 1979). Dallas, Texas 75275. Taken together, these data demonstrate a substantial degree 346 POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY 347 of overlap between anxiety and depression, regardless of the pected, the NA and PA scales each defined a factor. The anxiety level considered. However, the data by no means imply that a and depression scales had significant loadings on both factors; distinction between anxiety and depression cannot be made. however, the anxiety scale loaded more strongly on the NA fac- Correlations in the .40 to .70 range leave much variance unac- tor, whereas the depression scale was a much better marker of counted for, and half of all patients with anxiety or depressive low PA. Similarly, Blumberg and Izard (1986) used self-report disorders show relatively pure syndromes. Furthermore, one mood scales to predict scores on measures of depression and can point to distinct subgroups of patients within each class, anxiety. Several of the negative emotion scales (most notably and to subjective and physiological correlates that are unique Sadness and Fear) contributed to the prediction of both mea- to each type of disorder (e.g., the disturbance of rapid eye move- sures, but the positive emotion scales (Joy and Interest) added ment [REM] sleep in depression, but not anxiety; see Akiskal, significantly only to the prediction of depression. The mood 1985; Kupfer et al., 1983). Even proponents of differentiability, data therefore suggest that PA may be an important factor in however, recognize the aforementioned problems, and agree on differentiating anxiety from depression (Tellegen, 1985; Watson the need for ftirther research to identify factors that will im- & Tellegen, 1985). prove their distinction. These studies, however, were conducted with normal subjects In this article we will report on one such factor. Specifically, and did not involve trait measures. Two studies have reported we will relate symptoms and diagnoses of anxiety and depres- supportive results using clinical samples. First, Hall (1977) ob- sion to two general mood-based personality factors, Positive tained diagnostic data and clinicians'ratings of anxiety and de- Affectivity and Negative Affectivity. We will show that Positive pression on a sample of 108 male outpatients. She found ratings Affectivity—but not Negative Affectivity—can be clinically and diagnoses of anxiety to be significantly correlated with NA, useful in distinguishing these two classes of disorder. but not PA, whereas ratings and diagnoses of depression were more highly related to (low) PA than NA. Second, Bouman and Positive and Negative Affect Luteijn (1986) examined three groups of patients: (a) major de- pressives, (b) dysthymics, and (c) nondepressives. Scores on a Extensive evidence demonstrates that two broad mood fac- number of mood and personality scales were factor analyzed, tors—Positive Affect and Negative Affect—are the dominant and two factors were extracted and interpreted as NA and PA. dimensions in self-reported mood (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Consistent with the model outlined earlier, the major depres- 1984; Watson & Tellegen, 1985). Although their names might sives had significantly lower PA scores than the dysphoric pa- suggest that they are opposite poles of the same dimension, Pos- tients, who were, in turn, lower on PA than the nondepressed itive and Negative Affect are in fact highly distinctive dimen- group. The latter data do not permit any comparison between sions that can be meaningfully represented as orthogonal (un- anxiety and depression, however. correlated) factors. Both mood factors can be measured either Our study provides the most comprehensive test of the model as a state (i.e., transient fluctuations in mood) or as a trait (i.e., to date. We examined the relation of trait PA and NA scores to stable individual differences in general affective tone). Our arti- symptoms and diagnoses of depression and anxiety in a clinical cle will focus on the traits, which Tellegen (1982) has termed patient population. On the basis of the data we have reviewed, Negative Affectivity (NA) and Positive Affectivity (PA). we predicted that NA scores would be significantly correlated Negative Affect is a general factor of subjective distress, and with both anxiety and depression, whereas PA scores would be subsumes a broad range of negative mood states, including fear, associated only with the latter. anxiety, hostility, scorn, and disgust. Mood states related to de- pression such
Recommended publications
  • Bad Is Stronger Than Good
    Review of General Psychology Copyright 2001 by the Educational Publishing Foundation 2001. Vol. 5. No. 4. 323-370 1089-2680/O1/S5.O0 DOI: 10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323 Bad Is Stronger Than Good Roy F. Baumeister and Ellen Bratslavsky Catrin Finkenauer Case Western Reserve University Free University of Amsterdam Kathleen D. Vohs Case Western Reserve University The greater power of bad events over good ones is found in everyday events, major life events (e.g., trauma), close relationship outcomes, social network patterns, interper- sonal interactions, and learning processes. Bad emotions, bad parents, and bad feedback have more impact than good ones, and bad information is processed more thoroughly than good. The self is more motivated to avoid bad self-definitions than to pursue good ones. Bad impressions and bad stereotypes are quicker to form and more resistant to disconfirmation than good ones. Various explanations such as diagnosticity and sa- lience help explain some findings, but the greater power of bad events is still found when such variables are controlled. Hardly any exceptions (indicating greater power of good) can be found. Taken together, these findings suggest that bad is stronger than good, as a general principle across a broad range of psychological phenomena. Centuries of literary efforts and religious pothesis that bad is stronger than good (see also thought have depicted human life in terms of a Rozin & Royzman, in press). That is, events struggle between good and bad forces. At the that are negatively valenced (e.g., losing metaphysical level, evil gods or devils are the money, being abandoned by friends, and receiv- opponents of the divine forces of creation and ing criticism) will have a greater impact on the harmony.
    [Show full text]
  • Clinical Science
    APA CLINICAL SCIENCE III Society for the Science of Clinical Psychology Section III of the Division of Clinical Psychology of Division12 Ψ the American Psychological Association Developing clinical psychology as an experimental-behavioral science Newsletter Fall 2007 Issue Board Members and Contributors: President: Table of Contents: Daniel N. Klein, Ph.D. Stony Brook University Feature Article: Past-President: Antonette M. Zeiss, Ph.D. What is the role of clinical internships in training clinical V.A. Central Office, Washington, D.C. scientists? A. Spirito, E. McQuaid, J. Nash, G. Tremont, G. Francis, President-Elect: L. Weinstock, M. Andover .................................................................2 Lee Anna Clark, Ph.D. University of Iowa Announcements Secretary/Treasurer: Elizabeth Hayden, Ph.D. 2008 APS Student Poster Award.......................................................5 University of Western Ontario 2007 Election Results........................................................................6 Membership Committee Chairs: Sherryl Goodman, Ph.D. Emory University Douglas Mennin, Ph.D. Yale University Division 12 Representative: E. David Klonsky, Ph.D. Stony Brook University INSTRUCTIONS FOR AUTHORS Listserv Manager: Michael B. Miller, Ph.D. Clinical Science is published as a service to the members of Section III of the Division of Clinical University of Minnesota Psychology of the American Psychological Association. The purpose is to disseminate current information relevant to the goals of our organization. Feature Articles may be submitted to the editor via e-mail. They should be approximately 16 double- Editor: spaced pages and should include an abstract of 75- to 100- words. William P. Horan, Ph.D. Brief Articles may also be submitted,and should also include a 75- to 100-word abstract. All articles should be submitted as an attachment to an e-mail and formatted according to the University of California, Los Angeles Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 5th edition.
    [Show full text]
  • An Item Response Theory Integration of Normal and Abnormal Personality Scales
    CORRECTED JUNE 24, 2010; SEE LAST PAGE Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment © 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 1, No. 1, 5–21 1949-2715/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018136 An Item Response Theory Integration of Normal and Abnormal Personality Scales Douglas B. Samuel Leonard J. Simms Yale University School of Medicine University at Buffalo, State University of New York Lee Anna Clark W. John Livesley University of Iowa University of British Columbia Thomas A. Widiger University of Kentucky The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV–TR) currently conceptualizes personality disorders (PDs) as categorical syndromes that are distinct from normal personality. However, an alternative dimensional viewpoint is that PDs are maladaptive expressions of general personality traits. The dimensional perspective postulates that personality pathology exists at a more extreme level of the latent trait than does general personality. This hypothesis was examined using item response theory analyses comparing scales from two personality pathology instruments—the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology-Basic Questionnaire (DAPP-BQ; Livesley & Jackson, in press) and the Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Person- ality (SNAP; Clark, 1993; Clark, Simms, Wu, & Casillas, in press)—with scales from an instrument designed to assess normal range personality, the NEO Personality Inventory–Revised (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992). The results indicate that respective scales from these instruments assess shared latent constructs, with the NEO PI-R providing more information at the lower (normal) range and the DAPP-BQ and SNAP providing more information at the higher (abnormal) range. Nevertheless, the results also demonstrated substantial overlap in coverage.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information Company 300 North Zeeb Road. Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Order Number 9427762 Examining the relationship of negative affectivity and subjective well-being to goal-setting processes and task performance Necowitz, Lawrence B., Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Positive Psychology: Happiness
    POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY: HAPPINESS FIB QUIZ POWERPOINT NOTES INTRODUCTION & POSITIVE EMOTIONS • Positive psychology is concerned with the __1__ life, the __2__ life and the __3__ life. • Peterson’s study found that orientations to engagement and meaning were __4__strongly associated with life satisfaction than orientation to pleasure. • There are 2 distinct types of positive emotions concerned with the present: __5__ pleasures and more enduring __6__. • Gratifications differ from pleasures in that they entail states of absorption or __7__ that come from engagement in activities that involve using our signature __8__. POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE AFFECTIVITY • A wide range of data have shown that emotional experience can be best described by __9__ broad dimensions. Some researchers (Larsen, Diener, Averill) have labeled these dimensions (1)“activation or __10__” and (2) “pleasant/__11__ to unpleasant/__12__”. • Some researchers (Watston, Tellergen) suggest 2 dimensions, __13__ and __14__ affectivity (emotional experiences) captures the combinations of these two broad dimensions. Positive and negative affectivity can be assessed with the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). • Positive and negative affectivity is moderately __15__. • Positive affectivity is associated with greater job satisfaction and marital satisfaction • People with high positive affectivity tend to have __16__ life styles and better __17__ strategies. POSITIVE AFFECTIVITY & HAPPINESS • Positive affectivity may be enhanced through: • engagement in regular physical __18__, • maintaining a regular and adequate pattern of __19__, • making and maintaining strong __20__ an socializing frequently with supportive friends • through working towards __21__(3 words). • Positive affectivity is one aspect of __22__ • Diener assessed data of happiness and well-being from over a million people all over the world.
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 32 (2016) 25–37
    Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 32 (2016) 25–37 Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology www.elsevier.es/rpto Counterproductive work behavior among frontline government employees: Role of personality, emotional intelligence, affectivity, emotional labor, and emotional exhaustion a b,∗ c Ponniah Raman , Murali Sambasivan , Naresh Kumar a Putra Business School, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Serdang, Malaysia b Taylor’s Business School, Taylor’s University Lakeside Campus, Subang Jaya, Malaysia c Global Entrepreneurship Research and Innovation Center, Universiti Malaysia Kelantan, Technology Park Malaysia, Bukit Jalil, Malaysia a b s t r a c t a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: The main objective of this research is to study the effect of personality, emotional intelligence (EI), affectiv- Received 7 January 2015 ity, emotional labor and emotional exhaustion on counterproductive work behavior (CWB) of frontline Accepted 24 November 2015 employees in the government sector. A questionnaire was designed and distributed to 625 frontline Available online 28 January 2016 employees working at service counters in 25 ministries in Malaysia. We received responses from 519 employees (response rate = 83%). The data was analyzed using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). The Keywords: main findings are: (1) personality factors of employees drive their EI, affectivity, emotional labor, emo- Counterproductive work behavior tional exhaustion, and CWB and (2) EI and affectivity impact emotional labor, emotional exhaustion and Personality traits CWB. Through the integrated model, we have studied the indirect roles of emotional labor and emotional Affectivity exhaustion. This is one of the few studies that have effectively integrated the five constructs into a single Emotional labor Emotional exhaustion framework to study their effects on CWB.
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect As Predictors of Facial
    Anuario de Psicología ISSN: 0066-5126 [email protected] Universitat de Barcelona España Rus-Calafell, M.; Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J.; Frerich, N. Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect as predictors of Facial Emotion Recognition Capability using static and dynamic images Anuario de Psicología, vol. 43, núm. 1, abril, 2013, pp. 7-21 Universitat de Barcelona Barcelona, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=97027472001 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Anuario de Psicología/The UB Journal of Psychology 2013, vol. 43, nº 1, 7-21 © 2013, Facultat de Psicologia Universitat de Barcelona Schizotypy, Alexithymia and Affect as predictors of Facial Emotion Recognition Capability using static and dynamic images M. Rus-Calafell J. Gutiérrez-Maldonado N. Frerich Universitat de Barcelona The main purpose of the present study is to investigate the capacity of schizotypy and alexithymia traits, in combination with affectivity to predict facial emotion recognition capability in a sample of nonclinical adults. Consecutive healthy participants (N= 98) were investigated using the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20), the Oxford-Liverpool Inventory of Feelings and Experiences-Reduced Version (O-LIFE-R), and the Positive and NA Schedule (PANAS). A set of validated photographs (static images) and virtual faces (dynamic images) for presenting the basic emotions was used to assess emotion recognition. Pearson correlations were applied to investigate the relationship between the study variables; the amount of variance in emotion recognition capability predicted by OLIFE-R, TAS-20 and PANAS was calculated by using the linear regression model.
    [Show full text]
  • Curriculum Vitae
    EUNYOE RO CURRICULUM VITAE CONTACT INFORMATION Department of Psychology University of Notre Dame Notre Dame, IN 46556 Email: [email protected] EDUCATION 2010 Ph.D., Clinical Psychology Department of Psychology, University of Iowa 2009-2010 Predoctoral Internship (APA approved) University of Mississippi Medical Center/VA Medical Center Consortium 1998-2001 Clinical Psychology Residency Department of Psychiatry, Asan Medical Center, Seoul, Korea 1997 M.A., Clinical Psychology Korea University, Seoul, Korea 1994 B.A., Russian Language and Literature Korea University, Seoul, Korea PUBLICATIONS Peer-Reviewed Journals Lawrence, E., Barry, R.A., Brock, R.L., Bunde, M., Langer, A., Ro, E., et al. (in press). The Relationship Quality Interview: Evidence of reliability, convergent and divergent validity, and incremental utility. Psychological Assessment. Ro, E., & Clark, L.A. (2009). Psychosocial functioning in the context of diagnosis: Assessment and theoretical issues. Psychological Assessment, 21, 313-324. Lawrence, E., Yoon, J., Langer, A., & Ro, E. (2009). Is psychological aggression as detrimental as physical aggression? The independent effects of psychological aggression on depression and anxiety symptoms. Violence & Victims, 24, 20-35. Ro, E., & Lawrence, E. (2007). Comparing three measures of psychological aggression: Psychometric properties and differentiation from negative communication. Journal of Family Violence, 22, 575-586. Book Chapter Lawrence, E., Ro, E., Barry, R., & Bunde, M. (2006). Mechanisms of Distress and Dissolution in Physically Aggressive Romantic Relationships. In Handbook of Divorce and Relationship Dissolution. M. A. Fine, & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), pp. 263-286. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. AWARDS AND GRANTS Grants Funded 2008 University of Iowa Executive Council of Graduate and Professional Students (ECGPS) Research Grant, Principal Investigator – Examining relations between functioning and personality pathology, $1,000.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Characteristics and Physiological Reactivity to Acute Stress in Mothers of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
    Received: 28 September 2018 Revised: 19 February 2019 Accepted: 19 April 2019 DOI: 10.1002/smi.2870 RESEARCH ARTICLE Psychological characteristics and physiological reactivity to acute stress in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder Elena Pattini1 | Luca Carnevali2 | Alfonso Troisi3 | Guido Matrella4 | Dolores Rollo5 | Mauro Fornari6 | Andrea Sgoifo2 1 Centro per la Cura, la Diagnosi e lo Studio dei Disturbi della Comunicazione e della Abstract Socializzazione, Ausl Parma, Parma, Italy Stress related to parenting a child with autism spectrum disorder can differently 2 Stress Physiology Lab, Department of affect caregiver's physiological reactivity to acute stress. Here, parental stress levels, Chemistry, Life Sciences and Environmental Sustainability, University of Parma, Parma, Italy psychological characteristics, and coping strategies were assessed alongside measures 3 Department of Systems Medicine, University of heart rate, heart rate variability, and cortisol during a psychosocial stress test in of Rome Tor Vergata, Rome, Italy mothers of children with ASD (M‐ASD, n = 15) and mothers of typically developing 4 Department of Engineering and Architecture, University of Parma, Parma, Italy children (n = 15). M‐ASD reported significantly higher levels of parental stress, anxi- 5 Department of Medicine and Surgery, ety, negative affectivity, social inhibition, and a larger preference for avoidance strat- University of Parma, Parma, Italy egies. M‐ASD showed larger heart rate and cortisol responses to the psychosocial 6 Collegio
    [Show full text]
  • Trait Positive and Negative Emotionality Differentially Associate Withdiurnal Cortisol Activity Karissa G
    Bryn Mawr College Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College Psychology Faculty Research and Scholarship Psychology 2016 Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate withdiurnal cortisol activity Karissa G. Miller Aidan G.C. Wright Laurel M. Peterson Bryn Mawr College, [email protected] Thomas W. Kamarck Barbara A. Anderson See next page for additional authors Let us know how access to this document benefits ouy . Follow this and additional works at: http://repository.brynmawr.edu/psych_pubs Part of the Psychology Commons Custom Citation Miller et al. "Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate withdiurnal cortisol activity." Psychoneuroendocrinology 68 (2016); 177-185. This paper is posted at Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College. http://repository.brynmawr.edu/psych_pubs/47 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Authors Karissa G. Miller, Aidan G.C. Wright, Laurel M. Peterson, Thomas W. Kamarck, Barbara A. Anderson, Clemens Kirschbaum, Anna L. Marsland, and Matthew F. Muldoon This article is available at Scholarship, Research, and Creative Work at Bryn Mawr College: http://repository.brynmawr.edu/ psych_pubs/47 Psychoneuroendocrinology 68 (2016) 177–185 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Psychoneuroendocrinology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psyneuen Trait positive and negative emotionality differentially associate with diurnal cortisol activity a,∗ a b a Karissa G. Miller , Aidan G.C. Wright , Laurel
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Abnormal Psychology the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (Hitop): a Dimensional Alternative to Traditional Nosologies Roman Kotov, Robert F
    Journal of Abnormal Psychology The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A Dimensional Alternative to Traditional Nosologies Roman Kotov, Robert F. Krueger, David Watson, Thomas M. Achenbach, Robert R. Althoff, R. Michael Bagby, Timothy A. Brown, William T. Carpenter, Avshalom Caspi, Lee Anna Clark, Nicholas R. Eaton, Miriam K. Forbes, Kelsie T. Forbush, David Goldberg, Deborah Hasin, Steven E. Hyman, Masha Y. Ivanova, Donald R. Lynam, Kristian Markon, Joshua D. Miller, Terrie E. Moffitt, Leslie C. Morey, Stephanie N. Mullins-Sweatt, Johan Ormel, Christopher J. Patrick, Darrel A. Regier, Leslie Rescorla, Camilo J. Ruggero, Douglas B. Samuel, Martin Sellbom, Leonard J. Simms, Andrew E. Skodol, Tim Slade, Susan C. South, Jennifer L. Tackett, Irwin D. Waldman, Monika A. Waszczuk, Thomas A. Widiger, Aidan G. C. Wright, and Mark Zimmerman Online First Publication, March 23, 2017. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000258 CITATION Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., Brown, T. A., Carpenter, W. T., Caspi, A., Clark, L. A., Eaton, N. R., Forbes, M. K., Forbush, K. T., Goldberg, D., Hasin, D., Hyman, S. E., Ivanova, M. Y., Lynam, D. R., Markon, K., Miller, J. D., Moffitt, T. E., Morey, L. C., Mullins-Sweatt, S. N., Ormel, J., Patrick, C. J., Regier, D. A., Rescorla, L., Ruggero, C. J., Samuel, D. B., Sellbom, M., Simms, L. J., Skodol, A. E., Slade, T., South, S. C., Tackett, J. L., Waldman, I. D., Waszczuk, M. A., Widiger, T. A., Wright, A. G. C., & Zimmerman, M. (2017, March 23). The Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP): A Dimensional Alternative to Traditional Nosologies.
    [Show full text]
  • Pdfs/Joormann Gotlib 07.Pdf
    Biological Psychology 122 (2017) 110–120 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Biological Psychology jo urnal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsycho The beneficial effects of a positive attention bias amongst children with a history of psychosocial deprivation a,∗ b h Sonya Troller-Renfree , Katie A. McLaughlin , Margaret A. Sheridan , c,d,e,f g a Charles A. Nelson , Charles H. Zeanah , Nathan A. Fox a Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, College Park, United States b Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, United States c Harvard Medical School, United States d Boston Children’s Hospital, United States e Harvard Center on the Developing Child, United States f Harvard Graduate School of Education, United States g Tulane University School of Medicine, New Orleans, United States h Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, United States a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Article history: Children raised in institutions experience psychosocial deprivation that has detrimental influences on Received 30 June 2015 attention and mental health. The current study examined patterns of attention biases in children from Received in revised form 23 March 2016 institutions who were randomized at approximately 21.6 months to receive either a high-quality foster Accepted 14 April 2016 care intervention or care-as-usual. At age 12, children performed a dot-probe task and indices of attention Available online 21 April 2016 bias were calculated. Additionally, children completed a social stress paradigm and cortisol reactivity was computed.
    [Show full text]