Use of the Hermann Grid Illusion in the Measurement of Contrast Perception in Dyslexia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Use of the Hermann Grid Illusion in the Measurement of Contrast Perception in Dyslexia CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector Vision Research 45 (2005) 1–8 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Rapid communications Use of the Hermann grid illusion in the measurement of contrast perception in dyslexia James M. Gilchrist a,*, Barbara K. Pierscionek b, William M. Mann a a Department of Optometry, University of Bradford, Richmond Road, Bradford, BD7 1DP West Yorkshire, UK b School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Cromore Road, Coleraine, BT52 1SA Northern Ireland, UK Received 12 March 2004; received in revised form 28 July 2004 Abstract We measured contrast thresholds for perception of the Hermann grid illusion, using different contrast polarities and mean lumi- nances, in dyslexics and non-dyslexics. Both groups of subjects gave significantly lower thresholds with grids having dark squares and light paths, but there was no significant threshold difference between groups. Perceived strength of illusion was also measured in grids at suprathreshold contrast levels. Dyslexics perceived the illusion to be significantly stronger than non-dyslexics when the grid had light paths and low luminance. Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Dyslexia; Contrast perception; Hermann grid; Magnocellular 1. Introduction (Livingstone, Rosen, Drislane, & Galaburda, 1991) and psychophysical studies which indicate that some Dyslexia has been defined as a specific reading diffi- dyslexics suffer reduced sensitivity to contrast, flicker culty, inexplicable by any deficit in intelligence, learning and/or motion (Cornelissen, Richardson, Mason, & opportunity, motivation or sensory acuity (Critchley, Stein, 1995; Demb, Boynton, Best, & Heeger, 1998; 1970). The signs of dyslexia are varied and include Everatt, Bradshaw, & Hibbard, 1999; Lovegrove, abnormal phonological awareness, difficulties with writ- 1991; Lovegrove, Bowling, Badcock, & Blackwood, ing, spelling, auditory discrimination and visual process- 1980). However, the magnocellular deficit theory of dys- ing (Habib, 2000). Aspects of vision that appear lexia is by no means universally accepted, particularly correlated with dyslexia are oculomotor instability, im- with regard to contrast perception. In a review of the paired magnocellular processing and increased visual evidence from contrast sensitivity, Skottun (2000) re- discomfort/disturbance due to pattern glare effects ported that, while some studies have found contrast sen- (Evans, 2001). The last two of these may be associated sitivity loss in dyslexics consistent with a magnocellular with changes in contrast perception at threshold and/ deficit, these are outnumbered by studies that show or suprathreshold levels. either no loss of sensitivity, or loss of sensitivity incon- There is evidence that a deficit of the magnocellular sistent with a magnocellular deficit (see also Stuart, system is a significant correlate of dyslexia (Stein & McAnally, & Castles, 2001). More recent studies, too, Walsh, 1997). Support for this comes from anatomical cast doubt on the basic claim for contrast sensitivity loss in dyslexia, with Williams, Stuart, Castles, and McA- * Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 1274 234630. nally (2003) finding no significant difference in con- E-mail address: [email protected] (J.M. Gilchrist). trast sensitivity between dyslexics and controls, while 0042-6989/$ - see front matter Ó 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.08.020 2 J.M. Gilchrist et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 1–8 Bednarek and Grabowska (2002) report increased con- achieved faster visual search performance at low con- trast sensitivity in subjects with poor reading and writ- trast, consistent with observations by Meares (1980), ing skills (see also Lovegrove et al., 1982). but OÕBrien, Mansfield, and Legge (2000) found that Differences in contrast perception may also be impli- the dependence of reading speed on text contrast was cated in the occurrence of visual discomfort and distur- the same for dyslexics and controls. bance that is often reported by dyslexics. Some Overall, therefore, we have no definitive picture of individuals are susceptible to such effects when viewing how the contrast responses of dyslexics differ from those repetitive patterns, including text (Wilkins, 1991, 1993, of non-dyslexics, with or without the effects of pattern 1995; Wilkins & Nimmo-Smith, 1987; Wilkins et al., glare. In this study we explore an alternative approach 1984). Wilkins et al. (1984) found headaches, visual to direct psychophysical measurement of contrast re- discomfort and illusory effects in observers viewing sponses in dyslexics and non-dyslexics using the Her- grating patterns, and demonstrated that their occur- mann grid illusion. This is the appearance of illusory rence was influenced by a range of parameters includ- grey spots at the intersections of pathways in a two ing spatial frequency, contrast, location in the visual dimensional array of squares. Baumgartner (1960, cited field, and whether viewed with one or both eyes. Visual by Spillmann & Levine, 1971) explained the illusion in discomfort and disturbance associated with repetitive, terms of receptive fields of on- or off-centre visual neu- high contrast stimuli has been called Ôpattern glareÕ rons, depending on the polarity of the grid. For a grid (Wilkins, 1993), and its effects may be significant, with dark squares and light paths (Fig. 1), it was sug- reducing the clarity and comfort of printed text (Wil- gested that an on-centre receptive field centred on a path kins & Nimmo-Smith, 1987), and the speed and accu- intersection would receive about twice as much inhibi- racy of visual search (Conlon et al., 1998). Meares tion from the field surround as an on-centre receptive (1980) was the first to report that pattern glare effects field illuminated by a light bar. Hence the firing rate are common in children with reading disabilities, and of the neuron viewing the Hermann grid would be re- she found that the reading performance of such chil- duced and the path intersection should appear darker. dren may be improved with smaller print, coloured The opposite effect would be produced in grids with paper or reduced contrast. Alleviation of pattern glare dark paths due to the responses of off-centre neurons lo- by coloured filters was proposed by Irlen (1983) who cated at path intersections. The disappearance of the also claimed that the condition is particularly common illusion on fixation at grid intersections has been attrib- in dyslexia, affecting 12% of the general population but uted to the small size of foveal receptive field centres 65% of dyslexics (Irlen, 1991). Evans, Cook, Richards, (Spillmann, 1994). Grid contrast influences detection and Drasdo (1994) also suggest that susceptibility to of the illusion (Spillmann & Levine, 1971) and sensitiv- pattern glare may be more prevalent in dyslexics, par- ity to it can be reduced in certain disease states that dis- ticularly those who also have an oculomotor deficit turb contrast perception, such as diabetes (Davies & (Evans, Busby, Jeanes, & Wilkins, 1995; Evans, Morland, 2002). Drasdo, & Richards, 1994). Like gratings, Hermann grid patterns enable a ÔpureÕ The pattern glare effect, or Meares–Irlen Syndrome measure of contrast perception, avoiding the effects of (MIS) (Evans et al., 1995), has been described as a form typography, as well as the other sensory and cognitive of physiological hyperexcitability (Wilkins, 1993) which factors involved in text reading. In addition, the grid might be demonstrated by differences in pattern contrast stimulus has general characteristics that are most likely sensitivity. However, an investigation into the aetiology to elicit a pattern glare response; a high contrast, repet- of MIS found no significant difference in static contrast itive pattern structure containing predominantly low to sensitivity between MIS subjects and controls over a medium spatial frequencies. Also, since the illusory grid range of spatial frequencies from 1 to 12cdegÀ1 (Evans response derives from the characteristics of visual et al., 1996). On the other hand, Conlon, Lovegrove, receptive fields, it is possible to selectively stimulate Barker, and Chekaluk (2001) measured perceived distor- receptive fields of different sizes by manipulating the tion and contrast sensitivity in three groups of subjects dimensions of grid paths and squares. If dyslexics are classified according to their visual discomfort level. They indeed more sensitive than non-dyslexics to the struc- found that those who reported high visual discomfort ture and contrast of printed text and other repetitive saw greater distortion with low frequency gratings, patterns, then there may be some manifestation of this and gave reduced static contrast sensitivity in the range in their responses to the Hermann grid illusion. This 1–12cdegÀ1 but no loss of contrast sensitivity for 6Hz work therefore sought to investigate whether dyslexic temporally modulated gratings. and non-dyslexic subjects differ in their contrast sensi- Studies using suprathreshold contrast levels have also tivity for perception of the Hermann grid illusion, reported results that are difficult to interpret consist- and to determine whether there is any difference in ently. Williams, May, Solman, and Zhou (1995), for the perceived strength of illusion between the two example, found that subjects with reading difficulty groups. J.M. Gilchrist et al. / Vision Research 45 (2005) 1–8 3 Fig. 1. (a) Hermann grid patterns used for Experiment 1 (threshold
Recommended publications
  • The Retina the Retina the Pigment Layer Contains Melanin That Prevents Light Reflection Throughout the Globe of the Eye
    The Retina The retina The pigment layer contains melanin that prevents light reflection throughout the globe of the eye. It is also a store of Vitamin A. Rods and Cones Photoreceptors of the nervous system responsible for transforming light energy into the electrical energy of the nervous system. Bipolar cells Link between photoreceptors and ganglion cells. Horizontal cells Provide inhibition between bipolar cells. This is the mechanism of lateral inhibition which is important to edge detection and contrast enhancement. Amacrine cells Transmit excitatory signals from bipolar to ganglion cells. Thought to be important in signalling changes in light intensity. Neural circuitry About 125 rods and 5 cones converge on each optic nerve fibre. In the central portion of the fovea there are no rods. The ratio of cones to optic nerves in the fovea is one. This increases the visual acuity of the fovea. Rods and Cones Photosensitive substance in rods is called rhodopsin and in cones is called iodopsin. The Photochemistry of Vision Rhodopsin and Iodopsin Cis-retinal +Opsin Light sensitive Light Trans-retinal Opsin Light insensitive This is a reversible reaction Cis-retinal +Opsin Light sensitive Vitamin A In the presence of light, the rods and cones are hyperpolarized mv t Hyperpolarizing potential lasts for upto half a second, leading to the perception of fusion of flickering lights. Receptor potential – rod and cone potential Hyperpolarizing receptor potential caused by rhodopsin decomposition. Hyperpolarizing potential lasts for upto half a second, leading to the fusion of flickering lights. Light and dark adaptation in the visual system The eye is capable of vision in conditions of light that vary greatly in intensity The eye adapts dynamically to lighting conditions Light and Dark Adaptation We are able to see in light intensities that vary greatly.
    [Show full text]
  • Informing Computer Vision with Optical Illusions
    Informing Computer Vision with Optical Illusions Nasim Nematzadeh David M. W. Powers Trent Lewis College of Science and Engineering College of Science and Engineering College of Science and Engineering Flinders University Flinders University Flinders University Adelaide, Australia Adelaide, Australia Adelaide, Australia [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract— Illusions are fascinating and immediately catch the increasingly detailed biological characterization of both people's attention and interest, but they are also valuable in retinal and cortical cells over the last half a century (1960s- terms of giving us insights into human cognition and perception. 2010s), there remains considerable uncertainty, and even some A good theory of human perception should be able to explain the controversy, as to the nature and extent of the encoding of illusion, and a correct theory will actually give quantifiable visual information by the retina, and conversely of the results. We investigate here the efficiency of a computational subsequent processing and decoding in the cortex [6, 8]. filtering model utilised for modelling the lateral inhibition of retinal ganglion cells and their responses to a range of Geometric We explore the response of a simple bioplausible model of Illusions using isotropic Differences of Gaussian filters. This low-level vision on Geometric/Tile Illusions, reproducing the study explores the way in which illusions have been explained misperception of their geometry, that we reported for the Café and shows how a simple standard model of vision based on Wall and some Tile Illusions [2, 5] and here will report on a classical receptive fields can predict the existence of these range of Geometric illusions.
    [Show full text]
  • A Neural Model of the Scintillating Grid Illusion: Disinhibi- Tion and Self-Inhibition in Early Vision
    LETTER Communicated by Sidney Lehky A Neural Model of the Scintillating Grid Illusion: Disinhibi- tion and Self-Inhibition in Early Vision Yingwei Yu Downloaded from http://direct.mit.edu/neco/article-pdf/18/3/521/816498/neco.2006.18.3.521.pdf by guest on 24 September 2021 [email protected] Yoonsuck Choe [email protected] Department of Computer Science, Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas 77843-3112, U.S.A A stationary display of white discs positioned on intersecting gray bars on a dark background gives rise to a striking scintillating effect—the scin- tillating grid illusion. The spatial and temporal properties of the illusion are well known, but a neuronal-level explanation of the mechanism has not been fully investigated. Motivated by the neurophysiology of the Limulus retina, we propose disinhibition and self-inhibition as possible neural mechanisms that may give rise to the illusion. In this letter, a spa- tiotemporal model of the early visual pathway is derived that explicitly accounts for these two mechanisms. The model successfully predicted the change of strength in the illusion under various stimulus conditions, indicating that low-level mechanisms may well explain the scintillating effect in the illusion. 1 Introduction The scintillating grid illusion consists of bright discs superimposed on in- tersections of orthogonal gray bars on a dark background (see Figure 1A; Schrauf, Lingelbach, & Wist, 1997). In this illusion, illusory dark spots are perceived as scintillating within the white discs. Several important prop- erties of the illusion have been discovered and reported inrecent years. (1) The discs that are closer to a fixation show less scintillation (Schrauf et al., 1997), which might be due to the fact that receptive fields in the periphery are larger than those in the fovea.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Appearance Models Second Edition
    Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Color Appearance Models Wiley–IS&T Series in Imaging Science and Technology Series Editor: Michael A. Kriss Formerly of the Eastman Kodak Research Laboratories and the University of Rochester The Reproduction of Colour (6th Edition) R. W. G. Hunt Color Appearance Models (2nd Edition) Mark D. Fairchild Published in Association with the Society for Imaging Science and Technology Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England Telephone (+44) 1243 779777 This book was previously publisher by Pearson Education, Inc Email (for orders and customer service enquiries): [email protected] Visit our Home Page on www.wileyeurope.com or www.wiley.com All Rights Reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning or otherwise, except under the terms of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 or under the terms of a licence issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, 90 Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 4LP, UK, without the permission in writing of the Publisher. Requests to the Publisher should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England, or emailed to [email protected], or faxed to (+44) 1243 770571. This publication is designed to offer Authors the opportunity to publish accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:1907.09019V2 [Cs.CV] 5 Aug 2019
    Preprint IMAGENET-TRAINED DEEP NEURAL NETWORK EX- HIBITS ILLUSION-LIKE RESPONSE TO THE SCINTILLAT- ING GRID Eric Sun Ron Dekel Department of Physics Department of Neurobiology Department of Chemistry and Chemical Biology Weizmann Institute of Science Harvard University Rehovot, PA 7610001, Israel Cambridge, MA 02138, USA [email protected] eric [email protected] ABSTRACT Deep neural network (DNN) models for computer vision are now capable of human-level object recognition. Consequently, similarities in the performance and vulnerabilities of DNN and human vision are of great interest. Here we charac- terize the response of the VGG-19 DNN to images of the Scintillating Grid visual illusion, in which white dots are perceived to be partially black. We observed a significant deviation from the expected monotonic relation between VGG-19 rep- resentational dissimilarity and dot whiteness in the Scintillating Grid. That is, a linear increase in dot whiteness leads to a non-linear increase and then, remark- ably, a decrease (non-monotonicity) in representational dissimilarity. In control images, mostly monotonic relations between representational dissimilarity and dot whiteness were observed. Furthermore, the dot whiteness level correspond- ing to the maximal representational dissimilarity (i.e. onset of non-monotonic dissimilarity) matched closely with that corresponding to the onset of illusion per- ception in human observers. As such, the non-monotonic response in the DNN is a potential model correlate for human illusion perception. 1 INTRODUCTION Deep neural network (DNN) models are capable of besting human champions in chess [18] and Go [20] and reaching superhuman levels of accuracy in image classification and object recognition tasks [3, 11, 5, 18].
    [Show full text]
  • Book XVII License and the Law Editor: Ramon F
    8 88 8 8nd 8 8888on.com 8888 Basic Photography in 180 Days Book XVII License and the Law Editor: Ramon F. aeroramon.com Contents 1 Day 1 1 1.1 Photography and the law ....................................... 1 1.1.1 United Kingdom ....................................... 2 1.1.2 United States ......................................... 6 1.1.3 Hong Kong .......................................... 8 1.1.4 Hungary ............................................ 8 1.1.5 Macau ............................................. 8 1.1.6 South Africa ......................................... 8 1.1.7 Sudan and South Sudan .................................... 9 1.1.8 India .............................................. 10 1.1.9 Iceland ............................................ 10 1.1.10 Spain ............................................. 10 1.1.11 Mexico ............................................ 10 1.1.12 See also ............................................ 10 1.1.13 Notes ............................................. 10 1.1.14 References .......................................... 10 1.1.15 External links ......................................... 12 2 Day 2 13 2.1 Observation .............................................. 13 2.1.1 Observation in science .................................... 14 2.1.2 Observational paradoxes ................................... 14 2.1.3 Biases ............................................. 15 2.1.4 Observations in philosophy .................................. 16 2.1.5 See also ...........................................
    [Show full text]
  • The Hermann Grid Illusion Revisited
    Perception, 2005, volume 34, pages 1375 ^ 1397 DOI:10.1068/p5447 The Hermann grid illusion revisited Peter H Schiller, Christina E Carvey Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA; e-mail: [email protected] Received 12 October 2004, in revised form 12 January 2005; published online 23 September 2005 Abstract. The Hermann grid illusion consists of smudges perceived at the intersections of a white grid presented on a black background. In 1960 the effect was first explained by a theory advanced by Baumgartner suggesting the illusory effect is due to differences in the discharge characteristics of retinal ganglion cells when their receptive fields fall along the intersections versus when they fall along non-intersecting regions of the grid. Since then, others have claimed that this theory might not be adequate, suggesting that a model based on cortical mechanisms is necessary [Lingelbach et al, 1985 Perception 14(1) A7; Spillmann, 1994 Perception 23 691 ^ 708; Geier et al, 2004 Perception 33 Supplement, 53; Westheimer, 2004 Vision Research 44 2457 ^ 2465]. We present in this paper the following evidence to show that the retinal ganglion cell theory is untenable: (i) varying the makeup of the grid in a manner that does not materially affect the putative differ- ential responses of the ganglion cells can reduce or eliminate the illusory effect; (ii) varying the grid such as to affect the putative differential responses of the ganglion cells does not eliminate the illusory effect; and (iii) the actual spatial layout of the retinal ganglion cell receptive fields is other than that assumed by the theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Variations on the Hermann Grid: an Extinction Illusion
    Perception, 2000, volume 29, pages 1209 ^ 1217 DOI:10.1068/p2985 Variations on the Hermann grid: an extinction illusion Jacques Ninio Laboratoire de Physique Statistique(1),Eè cole Normale Supe¨ rieure, 24 rue Lhomond, 75231 Paris cedex 05, France; e-mail: [email protected] Kent A Stevens Department of Computer Science, Deschutes Hall, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, USA; e-mail: [email protected] Received 21 September 1999, in revised form 21 June 2000 Abstract. When the white disks in a scintillating grid are reduced in size, and outlined in black, they tend to disappear. One sees only a few of them at a time, in clusters which move erratically on the page. Where they are not seen, the grey alleys seem to be continuous, generating grey crossings that are not actually present. Some black sparkling can be seen at those crossings where no disk is seen. The illusion also works in reverse contrast. The Hermann grid (Brewster 1844; Hermann 1870) is a robust illusion. It is classically presented as a two-dimensional array of black squares, separated by rectilinear alleys. It is thought to be caused by processes of local brightness computation in arrays of neurons (eg Baumgartner 1960). As reviewed by Spillmann (1994) the illusion is tolerant to many geometrical variations. The alleys need not be orthogonal; the corners of the squares may be rounded. Various ratios of alley width to square size were explored by Vasarely in his artwork (eg Supernovae, reproduced in Thurston and Carraher 1966, page 35). The Hermann grid illusion is also resistant to many manipulations of local contrast.
    [Show full text]
  • Vision Health Module Workbook Answers
    Vision Health Module Workbook Answers 1 INSTRUCTIONS This workbook is to help students review what they have learnt about the parts of the eye, how vision works, eye health and different eye conditions. It is designed to be used in conjunction with the Vision Health Module units. Activity Unit Activity 1: Drawing and labelling an eye How do our eyes work – part 1 Activity 2: Labelling the parts of the eye How do our eyes work – part 1 Activity 3: Mix and match How do our eyes work – part 1 Activity 4: Optical illusions How do our eyes work – part 2 Activity 5: Crossword – eye health Looking after our eyes Activity 6: Wordfind – eye conditions We all see differently Extension activities All 2 ACTIVITY 1: DRAWING AND LABELLING AN EYE Work in pairs or threes. Look carefully at your partner’s eye. Draw their features onto the left half of the eyes below to complete the picture. Think about the following: • What colour are your partner’s irises? • What are their eyebrows like? • Do they have long eyelashes? • How big are their pupils? Include as much detail in your drawing as possible. Label the following parts: PUPIL, IRIS, EYELID, EYELASHES, SCLERA. 3 ACTIVITY 2: LABELLING THE PARTS OF THE EYE Label the parts on this diagram of an eye 2 1 3 8 4 7 5 6 1. LENS 5. OPTIC DISK 2. SCLERA 6. IRIS 3. RETINA 7. PUPIL 4. OPTIC NERVE 8. CORNEA 4 ACTIVITY 3: MIX AND MATCH FRONT OF THE EYE Draw a line from the part of the eye to its correct function IRIS The transparent front part of the eye.
    [Show full text]
  • The Hermann-Hering Grid Illusion Demonstrates Disruption of Lateral Inhibition Processing in Diabetes Mellitus Nigel P Davies, Antony B Morland
    203 CLINICAL SCIENCE Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.86.2.203 on 1 February 2002. Downloaded from The Hermann-Hering grid illusion demonstrates disruption of lateral inhibition processing in diabetes mellitus Nigel P Davies, Antony B Morland ............................................................................................................................. Br J Ophthalmol 2002;86:203–208 Background/aim: The Hermann-Hering grid illusion consists of dark illusory spots perceived at the intersections of horizontal and vertical white bars viewed against a dark background. The dark spots originate from lateral inhibition processing. This illusion was used to investigate the hypothesis that lat- eral inhibition may be disrupted in diabetes mellitus. Method: A computer monitor based psychophysical test was developed to measure the threshold of An appendix is on the perception of the illusion for different bar widths. The contrast threshold for illusion perception at seven BJO website bar widths (range 0.09° to 0.60°) was measured using a randomly interleaved double staircase. Con- volution of Hermann-Hering grids with difference of Gaussian receptive fields was used to generate See end of article for model sensitivity functions. The method of least squares was used to fit these to the experimental data. authors’ affiliations ....................... 14 diabetic patients and 12 control subjects of similar ages performed the test. Results: The sensitivity to the illusion was significantly reduced in the diabetic group for bar widths Correspondence to: 0.22°, 0.28°, and 0.35° (p = 0.01). The mean centre:surround ratio for the controls was 1:9.1 (SD A B Morland, PhD, 1.6) with a mean correlation coefficient of R2 = 0.80 (SD 0.16).
    [Show full text]
  • Systematic Structural Analysis of Optical Illusion Art and Application in Graphic Design with Autism Spectrum Disorder
    SYSTEMATIC STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS OF OPTICAL ILLUSION ART AND APPLICATION IN GRAPHIC DESIGN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER RELATORE :PROF. ARCH. PH.D ANNA MAROTTA CORRELATORE : ARCH. PH.D ROSSANA NETTI STUDENT:LI HAORAN SYSTEMATIC STRUCTURAL ANALYSIS OF OPTICAL ILLUSION ARTAND APPLICATION IN GRAPHIC DESIGN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER Abstract To explore the application of optical illusion in different fields, taking M.C. Escher's painting as an example, systematic analysis of optical illusion, Gestalt psychology, and Geometry are performed. Then, from the geometric point of view, to analyze the optical illusion formation model. Taking Fraser Spiral Illusion and Herring Illusion as examples, through the dismantling and combination of Fraser Spiral Illusion and the analysis of the spiral angle, and Herring Illusion tilt angle analysis, the structure of the graph is further explored. This results in a geometric drawing method that optimizes the optical illusion effect and applies this method to graphic design combined with the autism spectrum. Therefore, this project will remind the ordinary people and designers to pay attention to the universal design while also paying attention to extreme people through the combination of graphic design and optical illusion. That is, focus on autism spectrum disorder (ASD), provide more inclusive design and try their best to provide autism spectrum with the deserving autonomy and independence in public space. Meanwhile, making the autism spectrum “integrate” into the general public’s environment so that their abilities match the environment and improve self-care capabilities. Keywords Autism Spectrum Disorder, Inclusive Design, Graphic Design, Gestalt Psychology, Optical Illusion, Systemic Perspective Introduction 1. Systematic analysis of the content of the thesis 1.1 The purpose and the significance of study 1.1.1 Methodology with system diagram framework 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Example Videos Pop Quiz!
    1/30/2014 Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Graphics System, Human Visual System Characteristics, and Illusions: Lighting, Surface Perception, Texture, Acuities, Receptive Fields, Brightness Illusions, Simultaneous Contrast, Constancy 1/30/2014 Perception and Illusions Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Taylor 1 Example Videos • Volume Illustration of Muscle from DT-MRI • Flowing volumetric surfaces • Visual Queries for Neurobiology – Link to folder (open queries_divx.avi with VLC) 1/30/2014 Perception and Illusions Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Taylor 2 Pop Quiz! • What is magic about Red, Green, Blue? • What is the human visual system especially good at? • What is the human visual system especially poor at? • What visual channel is used for shape detection? 1/30/2014 Perception and Illusions Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Taylor 3 1 1/30/2014 What happens in the world? • Ambient Optical Array (Gibson, 1986)== Plenoptic Function = f(x,y,z, φ,θ, λ) – Describes intensity of light passing all locations, in all directions, at all wavelengths – Is zero inside opaque objects – Takes forever to simulate • We only need a sampling of this function! – Passing through center of projection of the eye(s) – Coming from pixels on the screen – At three wavelengths (“red”, “green”, and “blue”) • This sampling is what computer graphics is about 1/30/2014 Perception and Illusions Comp/Phys/Apsc 715 Taylor 4 What’s the big deal? How about just drawing the correct number of photons from objects in space? – Works for photorealism! • It would take forever to compute… • Display devices have limited ranges – We’re
    [Show full text]