Verb Chains in Nizaa

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Verb Chains in Nizaa Verb chains in Nizaa Bjørghild Kjelsvik Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Cand. Philol. Department of Linguistics University of Oslo Oslo November 2002 II Verb chains in Nizaa Bjørghild Kjelsvik Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Cand. Philol. Department of Linguistics University of Oslo Oslo 5th November 2002 IV TABLE OF CONTENTS 1 INTRODUCTION 3 1.1 THE NIZAA LANGUAGE 3 1.2 STRUCTURE OF THE THESIS: 3 1.2.1 THE MAIN POINTS OF THE THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK AND HOW IT IS USED: 3 1.3 THE CORPUS 4 1.3.1 THE FORMALISM OF THE EXAMPLES 5 1.3.2 OTHER FORMALISMS: 6 1.3.3 ABBREVIATIONS 7 1.4 ON THE TEXTS 7 2 A SHORT OVERVIEW OF NIZAA GRAMMAR 9 2.1 PHONOLOGY 9 2.1.1 SYLLABLE-STRUCTURE 9 2.1.2 VOWELS 9 2.1.3 CONSONANTS 10 2.1.4 MORPHOPHONOLOGY 11 2.1.5 TONES 12 2.2 WORD ORDER AND SENTENCE TYPES. 13 2.3 NOUNS 17 2.4 PRONOUNS 18 2.5 VERBS AND VERB MORPHOLOGY 18 2.5.1 VERBAL DERIVATIONS 18 2.5.2 INFLECTIONS: 20 3 MULTIPLE VERBS IN NIZAA CLAUSES 22 3.1 FORMAL CHARACTERISTICS 22 3.2 FREQUENCIES 23 3.2.1 COMMENTS ON THE TABLES: 26 3.3 DISTRIBUTION PATTERNS IN 2-VERB CHAINS 27 3.3.1 COMMENTS ON THE DISTRIBUTION PATTERN TABLES: 30 3.4 SERIAL VERBS 30 3.4.1 NIZAA CHAINS - SERIAL VERBS OR COMPOUNDS? 31 I TABLE OF CONTENTS 4 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK OF THE THESIS 34 4.1 EVENT INTEGRATION 34 4.1.1 MACRO-EVENT, FRAMING EVENT AND CO-EVENT 35 4.1.2 THE FRAMING EVENT 36 4.1.3 THE CO-EVENT 39 4.2 FRAMING EVENTS IN A TYPOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVE 41 4.2.1 SATELLITES 41 4.2.2 SATELLITE-FRAMED AND VERB- FRAMED LANGUAGES 42 4.2.3 ILLUSTRATION OF THE TWO TYPES 43 4.3 LEXICALISATION AND CONSTRUCTION; SYNTACTIC CONSIDERATIONS 45 4.3.1 LEXICALISATION PATTERNS 45 4.3.2 SYNTAX 47 4.3.3 PATTERNS LICENSING GENERIC VERBS AND PLEONASTIC CONSTRUCTIONS 48 4.4 SUMMARY 48 5 APPLYING THE THEORY 52 5.1 MACRO-EVENTS AND VERB CHAINS 52 5.2 ICONICITY AND ECONOMY 53 5.2.1 ICONICITY 53 5.2.2 ECONOMY 54 5.3 BACKGROUNDING IN THE VERBAL COMPLEX 55 5.4 EVENT SEMANTICS AND MOTION 56 5.4.1 SEMANTIC ‘BUILDING-BLOCKS’ 56 5.4.2 NIZAA 57 5.5 HYPOTHESES 60 6 ANALYSIS OF THE 2-VERB CHAINS 63 6.1 MOTION AS FRAMING EVENT 63 6.1.1 PRECURSION 64 6.1.2 ENABLEMENT 65 6.1.3 CAUSE AND MANNER 66 6.1.4 CONCOMITANCE 73 6.1.5 CONCURRENT RESULT 73 II TABLE OF CONTENTS 6.1.6 SUBSEQUENCE 73 6.1.7 CONSTITUTIVENESS 75 6.1.8 CHAINS WITH TWO MOTION-VERBS 76 6.1.9 SUMMARY OF MOTION AS FRAMING EVENT 81 6.2 TEMPORAL CONTOURING AS FRAMING EVENT 81 6.2.1 ‘TO BEGIN DOING’ 83 6.2.2 ‘TO STAY DOING’ 85 6.2.3 ‘TO FINISH DOING’ 86 6.2.4 ‘TO RETURN TO DOING’ 87 6.2.5 SUMMARY OF TEMPORAL CONTOURING AS FRAMING EVENT 89 6.3 STATE CHANGE AS FRAMING EVENT 89 6.3.1 PRECURSION 91 6.3.2 ENABLEMENT 91 6.3.3 CAUSE 92 6.3.4 MANNER 95 6.3.5 CONCOMITANCE 96 6.3.6 SUBSEQUENCE 97 6.3.7 CONSTITUTIVENESS 98 6.3.8 SUMMARY OF STATE CHANGE AS FRAMING EVENT 100 6.4 ACTION CORRELATING 100 6.4.1 COMITATIVE WITH X£û£ûØ ‘BE.TOGETHER’ 102 6.4.2 DATIVE WITH MH ‘GIVE’ 104 6.4.3 OTHER ACTION CORRELATING VERBS 109 6.4.4 SUMMARY OF ACTION CORRELATING AS FRAMING EVENT 112 6.5 REALIZATION 113 6.5.1 SUMMARY OF REALIZATION AS FRAMING EVENT 118 6.6 SUMMARY OF TWO-VERB CHAINS 118 7 ANALYSIS OF 3-AND 4- VERB CHAINS 121 7.1 FREQUENCIES AND DISTRIBUTION PATTERNS 121 7.1.1 DISTRIBUTION PATTERNS IN 3- AND 4-VERB CHAINS 121 7.2 THREE-VERB CHAINS 123 7.2.1 MOTION 124 7.2.2 STATE CHANGE 128 7.2.3 ACTION CORRELATING 129 7.3 FOUR-VERB CHAINS 135 III TABLE OF CONTENTS 7.4 SUMMARY OF 3- AND 4-VERB CHAINS 137 8 CONCLUSIONS 138 8.1 MACRO-EVENTS IN VERB CHAINS 138 8.1.1 FRAMING EVENTS AND CO-EVENTS 138 8.1.2 THE ‘STORY-LINE’ CONNECTION 139 8.1.3 THE CHAINS AND BACKGROUNDING 140 8.1.4 FURTHER COMMENTS ON THE FRAMING EVENT TYPES 140 8.2 GRAMMATICALIZATION ISSUES 141 8.3 VERB-FRAMED OR SATELLITE-FRAMED? 143 IV ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I first arrived in Cameroon in 1992, coming to work with the Nizaa language in a language project directed by the Eglise Evangélique Lutherienne du Cameroun. This African church has long traditions of work in local languages, together with several mission society partners such as The Norwegian Mission Society, the American Lutheran Church, Lutheran Bible Translators and the SIL. From 1993 the Norwegian Agency for Development Cooperation has been funding much of the work. The next 7 years, then, I was busily occupied with creating primers and reading material, publishing story collections and health booklets, and getting started on a translation of the New Testament. When my term was over, it felt necessary to reflect somewhat more profoundly on the structures of this language, and so I started on the process that has lead to the present thesis. It has been a most rewarding experience to finally have time for thinking through some of the data that I had gathered during my stay in Cameroon. Needless to say, this thesis would never have come about without the efforts of several other people. I would like to thank first of all my collaborators and friends in the Littérature Nizaa. Without their patience, I would never have learned Nizaa, and without their faithful work there would not have been any of written material now existing in this formerly oral language. Each of them contributed in their own way, and I thank each of them : Hamadicko Daniel and Bouba Jean, both active in translation, Patouma Sambo Jacqueline and Doudou Marie, doing secretary work and literacy classes, and Baba Pierre, also literacy work. Furthermore, I must thank my knowledgeable and always patient supervisor, Rolf Theil Endresen. He is in this case a ‘first mover’ in a very real sense of the word: it was he who first analysed the Nizaa language and created its alphabet. It has been a most gratifying experience to have him as supervisor, both because of his extensive familiarity with African and general linguistics, his knowledge of Nizaa, and his love of language that makes him take interest in the most minute details of his students work. I have also been so lucky as to have had a great group of fellow students around me. It is a great boon to have people actively working in different fields of linguistics around, when working on such a project! Thanks to you all, for discussions, conversations and jokes, and everything else you have contributed with! Finally I must thank my husband Hjalmar, and Martha Joy, Iselin and Per Arne for their support and for much patience during the writing process. Without their willingness to take care of the running chores of the household, things would have been quite more unpleasant. Bjørghild Kjelsvik 1 2 INTRODUCTION 1 INTRODUCTION 1.1 The Nizaa language About 10.000 people in Cameroun speak the Nizaa language. They live for the most part in and around Galim, a village of some 2000 inhabitants in the Faro et Deo Departement of the Adamaoua province. The area may cover about 100 km2, stretching eastward from the Nigerian border. Cameroun is truly a conglomorate of langauges (about 250) and so several other languages are spoken in the same area, notably Fula (Foulfoulde) and Haussa, but also Mbum, Tchamba (Samba), Gbaya, Pere and others. Most Nizaa speakers know Fula in addition to their own language, since it is the prevalent lingua franca of northern Cameroun. As for the genetic classification of Nizaa, we base ourselves on Endresen 1990/91 and 1992. In these articles he comprehensively evaluates the different classifications made, and refute them as not being founded on well-established comparative methods of classification. Endresen 1990/91 reconstructs earlier stages of Nizaa and shows regular correspondences with proto-Bantu forms. From this treatment it is clear that Nizaa belongs to a Bantoïd sub-branch of the Benue-Congo branch of the Niger-Congo family, but otherwise it is not satisfactorily accounted for. 1.2 Structure of the thesis: In this thesis we shall work with a particular structure of multiple verbs in Nizaa, called verb chains. We will show in chapter 1 that such complex predicates are frequently used in Nizaa, covering about 34 % of the total occurrences of clauses with verbs in the corpus. Chapter 2 describes the frequency of the different verbs in the corpus and groups them according to distributional patterns. A theory of complex events is presented in chapter 4 and further expanded and applied to Nizaa in chapter 5. In the two following chapters, 6 and 7, we then will examine Nizaa verb complexes from the point of view of this theoretical framework of complex event, before concluding on the finds in chapter 8. Appendix A give whole-sentence versions of all the language examples, and Appendix B lists the different types of complex predicates of the corpus.
Recommended publications
  • UNIVERSITY of MALAYA Universitykuala LUMPUR of Malaya
    MIMETIC WORDS AND LEXICALIZATION PATTERNS IN A JAPANESE NOVEL AND ITS ENGLISH TRANSLATION NG KAR PEK FACULTY OF LANGUAGES AND LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA UniversityKUALA LUMPUR of Malaya 2017 i MIMETIC WORDS AND LEXICALIZATION PATTERNS IN A JAPANESE NOVEL AND ITS ENGLISH TRANSLATION NG KAR PEK DISSERTATION SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF LINGUISTICS FACULTY OF LANGUAGES AND LINGUISTICS UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA KUALA LUMPUR University of Malaya 2017 i UNIVERSITY OF MALAYA ORIGINAL LITERARY WORK DECLARATION Name of Candidate: NG KAR PEK Registration/Matric No.: TGC 120007 Name of Degree: MASTER OF LINGUISTICS Title of Dissertation (“this Work”): MIMETIC WORDS AND LEXICALIZATION PATTERNS IN A JAPANESE NOVEL AND ITS ENGLISH TRANSLATION Field of Study: COGNITIVE LINGUISTICS I do solemnly and sincerely declare that: (1) I am the sole author/writer of this Work; (2) This Work is original; (3) Any use of any work in which copyright exists was done by way of fair dealing and for permitted purposes and any excerpt or extract from, or reference to or reproduction of any copyright work has been disclosed expressly and sufficiently and the title of the Work and its authorship have been acknowledged in this Work; (4) I do not have any actual knowledge nor ought I reasonably to know that the making of this work constitutes an infringement of any copyright work; (5) I hereby assign all and every right in the copyright to this Work to the University of Malaya (“UM”), who henceforth shall be the owner of the copyright in this Work and that any reproduction or use in any form or by any means whatsoever is prohibited without the written consent of UM having been first had and obtained; (6) I am fully aware that if in the course of making this Work, I have infringed any copyright whether intentionally or otherwise, I may be subject to legal action or any other action as may be determined by UM.
    [Show full text]
  • William Snyder the Compounding Parameter (TCP)
    To appear in Violeta Demonte and Louise McNally (eds.) Telicity, Change, and State . Oxford: OUP. 1 PARAMETER THEORY AND MOTION PREDICATES William Snyder The Compounding Parameter (TCP) of (Snyder 1995, 2001) has been linked to a number of the same points of cross-linguistic variation that Leonard Talmy (1985, 1991, 2000) addresses in his well-known verb-framed / satellite-framed typology. Talmy differentiates between languages in which path-of-motion is normally encoded in the main verb of a clause ("verb-framed" languages), and those in which path-of-motion is more commonly expressed outside the verb ("satellite-framed" languages). Both the English type of verb-particle construction (where the particle can be separated from the verb by a phrasal constituent, as in Mary pulled the lid off ) and the English adjectival resultative construction ( John wiped the table clean ) have been argued to depend on the positive setting of TCP (e.g. Snyder 2001, Sugisaki & Isobe 2000); and both are typical of Talmy's satellite-framed languages (e.g. Talmy 1985:68,104). Likewise, researchers have argued that a spatial PP (such as under the bridge ) can convert a pure manner-of-motion activity predicate (Sue swam for/*in 15 minutes ) into an accomplishment ( Sue swam under the bridge in 15 minutes ) only in [+ TCP] languages (Beck & Snyder 2001, Gehrke 2008), and only in satellite-framed languages (cf. Aske 1989:6-7). This chapter examines the precise role of TCP in the linguistic expression of motion events. One of my central claims will be that TCP should be understood as the availability, or unavailability, of a specific rule of semantic composition, which I term To appear in Violeta Demonte and Louise McNally (eds.) Telicity, Change, and State .
    [Show full text]
  • Proceedings of the Third Workshop on Computational Typology and Multilingual NLP, Pages 1–9 June 10, 2021
    SIGTYP 2021 The 3rd Workshop on Research in Computational Typology and Multilingual NLP Proceedings of the Workshop June 10, 2021 ©2021 The Association for Computational Linguistics Order copies of this and other ACL proceedings from: Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL) 209 N. Eighth Street Stroudsburg, PA 18360 USA Tel: +1-570-476-8006 Fax: +1-570-476-0860 [email protected] ISBN 978-1-954085-34-3 ii SIGTYP 2021 is the third edition of the workshop for typology-related research and its integration into multilingual Natural Language Processing (NLP). The workshop is co-located with the 2021 Annual Conference of the North American Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics (NAACL 2021), which takes place virtually this year. Our workshop includes a shared task on robust language identification from speech. The final program of SIGTYP contains 4 keynote talks, 3 shared task papers, 10 archival papers, and 14 extended abstracts. This workshop would not have been possible without the contribution of its program committee, to whom we would like to express our gratitude. We should also thank Claire Bowern, Miryam de Lhoneux, Johannes Bjerva, and David Yarowsky for kindly accepting our invitation as invited speakers. The workshop is generously sponsored by Google. Please find more details on the SIGTYP 2021 website: https://sigtyp.github.io/ws2021. html iii Organizing Committee: Ekaterina Vylomova, University of Melbourne Elizabeth Salesky, Johns Hopkins University Sabrina Mielke, Johns Hopkins University Gabriella Lapesa, University of Stuttgart Ritesh Kumar, Bhim Rao Ambedkar University Harald Hammarström, Uppsala University Ivan Vulic,´ University of Cambridge Anna Korhonen, University of Cambridge Roi Reichart, Technion – Israel Institute of Technology Edoardo M.
    [Show full text]
  • Revising Talmy's Typological Classification of Complex Events
    Revising Talmy’s typological classification of complex event constructions William Croft, University of New Mexico, USA, [email protected] Jóhanna Barðdal, University of Bergen, Norway, [email protected] Willem Hollmann, University of Lancaster, UK, [email protected] Violeta Sotirova, University of Nottingham, UK, [email protected] Chiaki Taoka, Kobe College, Japan, [email protected] 1. Introduction In this chapter, we critically examine Talmy’s typological classification of complex event constructions. Talmy first proposed a typological classification of motion event constructions over thirty years ago (Talmy 1972, 1974, 1985); he later extended his typological classification to event constructions in general, particularly, constructions expressing events with resulting states (Talmy 1991, 2000). Talmy’s extension of his typological classification reflects a parallel generalization of the analysis of resultative constructions to include constructions of motion events with a path to a destination (e.g. Goldberg 1995, Rappaport Hovav and Levin 2001). Talmy’s typological classification of complex event constructions has been extremely influential in linguistics and psycholinguistics. More recently, however, it has started to be modified, in order to account for languages that do not quite fit into the classification. New types have been proposed, by Talmy himself and by others. We developed a similar but more detailed typology independently of the analyses offered by other researchers. We propose two revisions to Talmy’s typological classification (a brief outline is found in Croft 2003:220-24). The first is given in (1): (1) Talmy’s typological classification of complex event constructions must be elaborated to include additional types.
    [Show full text]
  • A Look Into the Acquisition of English Motion Event Conflation by Native Speakers of Chinese and Japanese
    PACLIC 24 Proceedings 563 A Look into the Acquisition of English Motion Event Conflation by Native Speakers of Chinese and Japanese Ryan Spring Graduate School of International Cultural Studies, Tohoku University Aoba-ku Kawauchi 41, Sendai City, 980-8576, Japan [email protected] Abstract. Since Talmy (2000 a&b) introduced his linguistic typology based on event conflation, it has been the source of much debate and ongoing research. One area that can particularly benefit from such research is the field of Second Language Acquisition. Cadierno (2008), Inagaki (2002) and many others have attempted to unveil the differences and difficulties that occur when learning a second language that is of a different type than one's native language. However, the current research in this area has thus far only dealt with satellite-framed and verb-framed languages. According to Slobin (2004), Chen and Guo (2008) and others, languages such as Chinese can be considered to be of a third type, known as equipollently-framed languages. This paper presents research that has attempted to observe the differences and similarities in the acquisition of a satellite-framed language (English) by native speakers of a verb-framed language (Japanese) and an equipollently- framed language (Chinese). Keywords: Second Language Acquisition, Motion Events, Event Conflation, Cognitive Linguistics, Equipollently-framed Language 1 Introduction Talmy (2000 a&b) introduced the idea of complex events that he termed 'macro-events'. He said that such events can be conceptualized as being comprised of two simpler events and the relation between them. For example, (1a), shown below, can be conceptualized as one event, and thus expressed in one sentence or phrase, but is actually made up of the two events, shown in (1b) and (1c).
    [Show full text]
  • Semantic Specificity of Perception Verbs in Maniq
    Semantic specificity of perception verbs in Maniq © Ewelina Wnuk 2016 Printed and bound by Ipskamp Drukkers Cover photo: A Maniq campsite, Satun province, Thailand, September 2011 Photograph by Krittanon Thotsagool Semantic specificity of perception verbs in Maniq Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. dr. J.H.J.M. van Krieken, volgens besluit van het college van decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op vrijdag 16 september 2016 om 10.30 uur precies door Ewelina Wnuk geboren op 28 juli 1984 te Leżajsk, Polen Promotoren Prof. dr. A. Majid Prof. dr. S.C. Levinson Copromotor Dr. N. Burenhult (Lund University, Zweden) Manuscriptcommissie Prof. dr. P.C. Muysken Prof. dr. N. Evans (Australian National University, Canberra, Australië) Dr. N. Kruspe (Lund University, Zweden) The research reported in this thesis was supported by the Max-Planck-Gesellschaft zur Förderung der Wissenschaften, München, Germany. For my parents, Zofia and Stanisław Contents Acknowledgments ............................................................................................. i Abbreviations ................................................................................................ vii 1 General introduction ........................................................................................ 1 1.1 Aim and scope ................................................................................................... 1 1.2 Theoretical background to verbal semantic specificity
    [Show full text]
  • EMPIRICAL STUDY Particle Placement in Learner Language
    Language Learning ISSN 0023-8333 EMPIRICAL STUDY Particle Placement in Learner Language Stefanie Wulff and Stefan Th. Gries University of Florida and University of California at Santa Barbara This study presents the first multifactorial corpus-based analysis of verb–particle con- structions in a data sample comprising spoken and written productions by intermediate- level learners of English as a second language from 17 language backgrounds. We annotated 4,911 attestations retrieved from native speaker and language learner corpora for 14 predictors, including syntactic complexity, rhythmic and segment alternation, and the verb framing of the speaker’s native language. A multifactorial prediction and deviation analysis using regression (Gries & Deshors, 2014), which stacks multiple regression analyses to compare native speaker and learner productions in identical con- texts, revealed a complex picture in which processing demands, input effects, and native language typology jointly shape the degree to which learners’ choices of constructions are nativelike or not. Keywords verb–particle construction; particle placement; learner corpus research; multifactorial regression; mixed-effects model; second language Introduction The acquisition of multiword units constitutes a challenging aspect of learning a language, be it a learner’s first language (L1) or second language (L2). One type of multiword unit in English is verb–particle constructions (VPCs; also called transitive phrasal verbs), that is, combinations of a verb and a particle such as pick up NP, take out NP, or throw away NP (where NP stands for a noun phrase). VPCs are particularly difficult to acquire for several reasons. For one, they, although ubiquitous in English, are not particularly frequent in non- Germanic languages, so many L2 learners of English do not benefit from having We are grateful to the three anonymous reviewers and to Associate Editor Scott Crossley for their helpful and insightful comments.
    [Show full text]
  • Book of Abstracts NAMED 2019 on Fictive Motion in Language
    Book of Abstracts NAMED 2019 On fictive Motion in Language May 23rd-24th, 2019 – Paris, France 1 PROGRAM Thursday May 23 9:30-10:00 am Welcome 10:00-11:00 am Plenary talk Jean-Michel Fortis (HTL, CNRS & Université Paris Diderot, France) Motion and the cognitive perspective on language: historical and critical notes 11:00-11:30 am Coffee Break 11:30-12:00 am Fabien Capelli (CLLE-ERSS, CNRS & Université Toulouse Jean Jaurès, France) Some figures and grounds about fictive motion in French 12:00-12:30 am Daria Lapenkova (National Research University ‘Higher School of Economics’, Moscow, Russia) Motion verbs in Avar 12:30-2:00 pm Lunch Break 2:00-3:00 pm Plenary talk Tuomas Huumo (University of Turku | UTU · School of Languages and Translation Studies, Finland) Scalarity, scanning, and fictive motion: What degree modifiers reveal about the meaning of Finnish adpositions 3:00-3:30 pm Rusudan Gersamia (School of Arts and Sciences, Institute of Linguistic Studies, Ilia State University, Tbilisi, Georgia) Fictive Motion Construction in Megrelian (Road, Sun, Time) 3:30-4:00 pm Alexandre François (Lattice, CNRS-ENS PSL & Université Sorbonne Nouvelle) Literal and figurative motion in some Oceanic languages 4:00-4:30 pm Coffee Break 4:30-5:00 pm Anastasia Yakovleva (National Research University ‘Higher School of Economics’, Moscow, Russia) Cardinal points and fictive motion strategies: a diachronic perspective 5:00-5:30 pm Castrenze Nigrelli (Università degli Studi di Palermo (Palermo, Italy) – Dipartimento di Scienze Umanistiche) Aspectual features
    [Show full text]
  • Serialising Languages: Satellite-Framed, Verb-Framed Or Neither
    Ghana Journal of Linguistics 2.1: 19-38 (2013) SERIALISING LANGUAGES: SATELLITE-FRAMED, VERB-FRAMED OR NEITHER Felix K. Ameka and James Essegbey Abstract The diversity in the coding of the core schema of motion, i.e., Path, has led to a traditional typology of languages into verb-framed and satellite-framed languages. In the former Path is encoded in verbs and in the latter it is encoded in non-verb elements that function as sisters to co-event expressing verbs such as manner verbs. Verb serializing languages pose a challenge to this typology as they express Path as well as the Co-event of manner in finite verbs that together function as a single predicate in translational motion clause. We argue that these languages do not fit in the typology and constitute a type of their own. We draw on data from Akan and Frog story narrations in Ewe, a Kwa language, and Sranan, a Caribbean Creole with Gbe substrate, to show that in terms of discourse properties verb serializing languages behave like Verb-framed with respect to some properties and like Satellite-framed languages in terms of others. This study fed into the revision of the typology and such languages are now said to be equipollently-framed languages. Keywords: serial verb constructions, translational motion, verb-framed, satellite- framed, equipollently-framed. Preamble The body of this article is a write-up of a paper we gave at the 32nd Annual Conference on African Linguistics (ACAL 32) held at the University of California, Berkeley in April 2001. It was accepted for publication in the proceedings of that conference, but the proceedings never saw the light of day.
    [Show full text]
  • Booklet of Abstracts
    NAMED 2020: De/constructing motion events July 1 st - 2nd Booklet of abstracts Plenary talks Michel Aurnague Lattice, CNRS, ENS - PSL, Université Sorbonne Nouvelle Paris 3, France Are path and manner appropriate analysis tools? Searching for the conceptual bases of motion eventualities in French For the past twenty-five or thirty years, and following Talmy’s (1985, 2000) seminal work on lexicalization patterns, analyzing motion processes or eventualities in language (and across languages) in terms of path and manner has become a nearly universal approach that has generated a tremendous amount of studies –both in linguistics and psycholinguistics. Yet, in the conclusion of a collective book investigating the “grammars of space” of a dozen languages in the world (including non-European ones), Levinson and Wilkins (2006: 527-530) highlighted the problems and limitations of the path vs. manner contrast for the cross-linguistic analysis of motion expression and indicated that “we need a better understanding of the underlying components of motion conceptualization , before we can get much further with a typology of how these are differentially conflated in different language types” (my emphasis). They even insisted that a crucial issue is “the notion of motion itself” (Levinson & Wilkins 2006: 531). In echo with the latter statements, this contribution will trace the main outlines of a framework developed the last ten-twelve years (see e.g. Aurnague 2008, 2011) in order to scrutinize the expression of motion processes in French. The notions of change of placement and change of basic locative relation (Boons 1987) used for analyzing motion eventualities will first be recalled, as well as the categories of verbs and processes that follow from their interaction.
    [Show full text]
  • Linguistic Typology and the Expression of Motion Events Dan I. Slobin
    In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (Eds.) (2004), Relating events in narrative: Vol. 2. Typological and contextual perspectives (pp. 219-257). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. The many ways to search for a frog: Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events Dan I. Slobin Contents 1. Typology 2. Manner of motion 2.1. The emergence of the owl across languages 2.2. Lexical availability and construction types 2.3. Serial-verb languages 2.4. Attention to manner: Narrative style 2.5. Conflation Patterns 2.6. Beyond lexicon and morphology 2.6.1. Alternative descriptions of manner 2.6.2. Posture and manner 2.6.3. Ideophones expressing manner 2.6.4. Gestural depictions of manner 2.7. Beyond spoken language 2.8. Codability and habitual expression 3. Path of motion 3.1. Analysis and packaging of path components 3.2. Typological factors 3.3. Language-specific constraints 3.4. Morphosyntactic factors 3.4.1. Locative casemarking 3.4.2. Directional morphemes 3.5. Cultural factors 3.6. Modality factors 3.7. Typology and the encoding of path information 4. Grounds in motion events 4.1. Path segmentation 4.2. Locative casemarking 4.3. Dependent-marking versus head-marking languages 4.4. The said and the unsaid 5. Typology revisited 5.1. A revised typology 5.2. Habitual expressions of path 5.3. Habitual expressions of manner: Degree of manner salience 5.4. A diachronic and synchronic model for the emergence of manner salience 5.5. Low-manner-salient languages 5.6. Conclusion Slobin – Typology and Motion Events 2 1. TYPOLOGY The chapters in this volume, along with the extensive list of frog story studies in the Appendix, provide a rich database for the exploration of particular questions of language use and acquisition.
    [Show full text]
  • ON PREDICTING RESULTATIVE ADJECTIVE CONSTRUCTIONS – Draft Version – 10 of June 2016
    th H.H. – ON PREDICTING RESULTATIVE ADJECTIVE CONSTRUCTIONS – draft version – 10 of June 2016 On predicting resultative adjective constructions source: https://s-media-cache-ak0.pinimg.com/736x/63/35/53/633553f2712601d48772544099300cba.jpg „Fishing the oceans empty“ Hubert Haider [orcid-id 0000-0001-7052-7607] Dept. of Linguistics & Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience Univ. Salzburg 10/06/2016 1 th H.H. – ON PREDICTING RESULTATIVE ADJECTIVE CONSTRUCTIONS – draft version – 10 of June 2016 Abstract1 Cross-linguistically, languages with adjectives as depictive predicates are ubiquitous. Languages with resultative adjectives are much more limited. This paper formulates a prediction based on a structural correlation: If a language makes available resultative complex verbal predicates (i.e. particle verbs or serial verbs), it thereby also makes available resultative adjective constructions, that is, complex verbal predicates with adjectives that receive a resultative interpretation. This generalization is supported by a sample of sufficiently well-analyzed languages. The focus of the paper rests on languages with/out particle verbs in combination with/out resultative adjectives. In analytic terms, a resultative adjective is part of a complex predicate. Competing analyses with the resultative adjective as the head of a predicate phrase (‘small clause’) fail. In such an analysis, the observed correlations could not be captured adequately. It would wrongly predict depictive and re- sultative adjective constructions to generally coexist cross-linguistically, but obviously they don´t. 1. Introduction Numerous languages admit adjectival secondary predicates with a depictive reading (see Schultze-Berndt & Himmelmann 2004), such as “They left the party drunk”. Less numerous are languages with resultative adjectival predicates, as in “They pounded the lump flat.” An obvious question to ask is this.
    [Show full text]