Emotional Constraints on Intentional Forgetting

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emotional Constraints on Intentional Forgetting Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 780–786 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Emotional constraints on intentional forgetting B. Keith Payne ¤, Elizabeth Corrigan Department of Psychology, Campus Box 3270, Davie Hall, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC 27599, USA Received 20 October 2005; revised 13 July 2006 Available online 12 September 2006 Abstract One way people control the contents of their minds is intentional forgetting—voluntarily forgetting events after they have hap- pened. The events people would most like to forget are unpleasant and emotional. This study used a directed forgetting procedure with emotional and neutral pictures to examine whether people can intentionally forget emotional events as easily as mundane ones. When the to-be-forgotten list was neutral, participants showed successful intentional forgetting. But when the to-be-forgotten list was emo- tional, directed forgetting failed. Results contribute to understanding the ways that emotion constrains mental control by capturing mental processes including memory retrieval. Emotion may short-circuit attempts to forget those parts of the past people would most like to forget. © 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Forgetting; Emotion; Memory Introduction environment (Ohman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001), color the interpretation of new information (Murphy & Zajonc, In the Wlm Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind (Gon- 1993), and guide decision-making (Bechara, Damasio, & dry, 2004), a corporation named Lacuna, Inc. has developed Damasio, 2000). These cognitive adjustments make a per- technology for the focused erasure of unwanted memories. son more sensitive to cues that may pose a threat or require Customers choosing this procedure all want to erase painful action. In short, emotions are signals that an event is memories—ex-lovers, departed spouses, long-time pets. important and requires a response. Along with these other What happens when a person tries to forget an emotional cognitive adjustments, emotion may enhance memory. A past? Research suggests that the mind treats emotional strong memory for a painful event will help avoid it in the events diVerently from mundane ones, often resulting in bet- future, while a strong memory for a pleasant event will help ter recall when people try to remember. But emotional mem- seek it out again. From an adaptationist perspective, a well- ories are also unique in another way. Sometimes, people do designed cognitive system is likely to build in a preference not want to remember. This article asks whether emotional for emotional memory. memories linger when people would rather forget. In a well-known study based on this rationale, Brown and Kulik (1977) predicted that dramatic events such as the Emotion and memory assassination of John F. Kennedy should produce strong and detailed photograph-like memories. Although such Emotions are functional because they signal important “Xashbulb” memories do appear to be accompanied by events and prepare a body and mind to react (Dolan, 2002; subjective qualities such as vividness and conWdence, later Frijda, 1986; Tooby & Cosmedes, 1990). For example, emo- research showed that they are not necessarily accurate tions draw attention to the most relevant aspects of the (Neisser & Harsch, 1992; Talarico & Rubin, 2003). In some cases, autobiographical memory for emotional events is * Corresponding author. more distorted than memory for neutral events (Marsh, E-mail address: [email protected] (B. Keith Payne). Tversky, & Hutson, 2006). The fact that even highly 0022-1031/$ - see front matter © 2006 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.07.005 B. Keith Payne, E. Corrigan / Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 (2007) 780–786 781 emotional memories can be prone to errors led some theo- condition, which is prima-facie evidence for intentional for- rists to doubt whether emotion plays the strong role in getting. However, demand eVects could also cause some sub- memory that was posited (Brewer, 1992; McCloskey, 1992). jects in the Forget condition to withhold words from the Wrst The perspective emerging from recent research puts these list that they actually remember. The second result is more two views in a larger context. Even though emotional memo- diYcult to explain away with demand eVects; participants in ries are not photograph-like copies of experience, well-con- the Forget condition show better memory for the second list trolled studies have shown that emotion can enhance than participants in the Remember condition. This Wnding memory accuracy (e.g., Bradley, Greenwald, Petry, & Lang, provides evidence that participants in the Forget condition 1992; Ochsner, 2000). Memory is strengthened most for the do not have the Wrst list as a source of interference. Partici- central, emotionally meaningful aspects of events, often at pants in the Remember condition have two sets of items the expense of peripheral details (Easterbrook, 1959). Neuro- competing for retrieval, but those in the Forget condition psychological evidence suggests that interactions between the have only one. These two results—lower recall of the to-be- amygdala and hippocampus are critical (e.g., Cahill & forgotten words and higher recall of the to-be-remembered McGaugh, 1998; Hamann, 2001; McGaugh, 2003; Labar & words—make up the intentional forgetting eVect. Phelps, 1998; Ochsner & Schacter, 2000; Phelps, 2004). These The key to intentional forgetting is what happens when studies show that emotionally arousing events spark intense memories are retrieved. The importance of retrieval has amygdala activity. The amygdala is believed to alter how the been demonstrated in studies that compare recall and rec- hippocampus consolidates memories, resulting in preferential ognition memory tasks. Success on either kind of task memory for emotional events. These Wndings suggest that, all requires that items be encoded and stored. Whereas recall else equal, emotional events are more likely to be remem- tasks require active search and retrieval of a memory, rec- bered than unemotional events. We know almost nothing, ognition tests present the studied item as a direct cue to however, about how emotional memories respond when peo- memory. That item needs only to be checked against what- ple deliberately endeavor to erase them. ever representation is stored. Because the directed forget- ting eVect is strong on recall tasks but weak or absent on Intentional forgetting recognition tasks, theorists have concluded that retrieval inhibition, rather than unlearning, is key to intentional for- People spend a lot of time trying to regulate the contents getting (Bjork, 1989; Geiselman, Bjork, & Fishman, 1983; of their own minds. One way they do so is “up-front,” by but see Sheard & MacLeod, 2005). Because participants in selectively attending to certain aspects of their world the Forget condition perform well on a recognition test of (Jacoby, Kelley, & McElree, 1999). This may be one of the the to-be-forgotten words, the items must be stored in most successful ways to control the contents of memory memory. But retrieval is blocked in the Forget condition. because how people direct their attention strongly aVects Bjork (1989) provides an example of how this trick of what they later recall (Broadbent, 1957). Another way peo- memory beneWts drivers when parking in a new spot every ple regulate their thoughts is “on-line,” by intentionally day. For today, it is useful to remember where the car is suppressing particular thoughts. On-line regulation can be parked. But it is also useful to forget where the car was successful or unsuccessful depending on certain factors, parked yesterday, as it prevents confusion about where the including whether attention and a good distracter are avail- car is now. Intentional forgetting can help update memory able (Wegner, 1994). Both methods of mental control can for any changing information, like wrong directions, a be fragile. Attention can be captured, and suppressed switched meeting time, or a friend’s new phone number. thoughts can rebound. But once a person has attended to But is it equally eVective for forgetting an ex-lover’s phone an event, thought about it, and consigned it to memory, number after a painful breakup? We have only the slimmest there is still one option. He or she may choose to forget. evidence about how intentional forgetting fairs against an Research on intentional forgetting shows that people emotional memory. can forget certain information when they wish to (Bjork, 1970; Bjork & Woodward, 1973). In a typical directed for- Intentional forgetting and emotion getting experiment, participants study a list of words with A few studies have examined intentional forgetting in instructions to remember them for a later test. In one condi- the context of emotion. These studies have mainly con- tion (the Forget condition) the experimenter tells partici- cerned clinical disorders and coping styles. For example, pants to forget the Wrst list, which they have already Power and colleagues (2000) reported that clinically studied, and concentrate on learning a second list. In the depressed participants showed no directed forgetting for other condition (the Remember condition) participants are unpleasant words (see also Hertel & Girstle, 2003). told to remember both lists of words. On a later test, both Depressed participants showed improved recall for to-be- groups are asked to recall all the words on both lists, ignor- forgotten words without
Recommended publications
  • Emotional Fortitude: the Inner Work of the CEO
    FEATURE Emotional fortitude The inner work of the CEO Benjamin Finzi, Mark Lipton, Kathy Lu, and Vincent Firth Emotional fortitude: The inner work of the CEO Emotional fortitude—the ability to stay clear-headed while exploring one’s emotional reactions to sources of tension—can improve a CEO’s resilience to the stressors of decision-making and lead to better decision outcomes. HETHER AT A large, established firm or a work” that effective CEOs perform as they journey fast-growing one, making decisions through the decision-making process and live with Wwhile staring disruption in the face may the consequences. be the most grueling element of being a CEO. Data feels insufficient. Assumptions feel tenuous. Options feel How can CEOs increase their constrained. Timing feels rushed. chances of making an optimal Outcomes feel binary: The decision either takes the organization in the right decision when all of the direction or the wrong one. alternatives may not be known, Yet executives—particularly CEOs—are when time is not on their side, expected to be the most qualified people in their organization to make decisions. and when emotions play a central CEOs, perhaps more than those in any role before, during, and after the other executive role, feel enormous pressure to get it “right.” Even the most decision is made? level-headed CEO is apt to experience sleepless nights and personal doubts about the choices they make and the consequences The intellectual and emotional that result. If the decision ultimately proves to be a tensions of perilous decisions poor one, there is no one else to blame.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional Doubt and Divine Hiddenness
    Eruditio Ardescens The Journal of Liberty Baptist Theological Seminary Volume 1 Issue 2 Volume 1, Issue 2 (Spring 2014) Article 1 5-2014 Emotional Doubt and Divine Hiddenness A. Chadwick Thornhill Liberty University Baptist Theological Seminary Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.liberty.edu/jlbts Part of the Practical Theology Commons, and the Religious Thought, Theology and Philosophy of Religion Commons Recommended Citation Thornhill, A. Chadwick (2014) "Emotional Doubt and Divine Hiddenness," Eruditio Ardescens: Vol. 1 : Iss. 2 , Article 1. Available at: https://digitalcommons.liberty.edu/jlbts/vol1/iss2/1 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by Scholars Crossing. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eruditio Ardescens by an authorized editor of Scholars Crossing. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Emotional Doubt and Divine Hiddenness A. Chadwick Thornhill* Emotionally motivated doubts concerning one’s religious faith can generate severe pain and anxiety in the life of a believer. These doubts may generate both emotional and physical problems that also significantly affect their health. Os Guinness in speaking of this type of doubt asserts, “no one is hurt more than the doubter. Afraid to believe what they want to believe, they fail to believe what they need to believe, and they alone are the losers.” 1 While recent Christian scholarship has begun to be more attentive to this issue as it pertains to addressing the emotional doubts of the church community, much more work needs to be done concerning this prevalent issue. One issue in particular which may motivate emotional doubt and permit it to fester is that of divine hiddenness, or the silence of God.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: on Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 12-2001 The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: On Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love Marc Bekoff University of Colorado Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_sata Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, and the Comparative Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Bekoff, M. (2001). The evolution of animal play, emotions, and social morality: on science, theology, spirituality, personhood, and love. Zygon®, 36(4), 615-655. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Evolution of Animal Play, Emotions, and Social Morality: On Science, Theology, Spirituality, Personhood, and Love Marc Bekoff University of Colorado KEYWORDS animal emotions, animal play, biocentric anthropomorphism, critical anthropomorphism, personhood, social morality, spirituality ABSTRACT My essay first takes me into the arena in which science, spirituality, and theology meet. I comment on the enterprise of science and how scientists could well benefit from reciprocal interactions with theologians and religious leaders. Next, I discuss the evolution of social morality and the ways in which various aspects of social play behavior relate to the notion of “behaving fairly.” The contributions of spiritual and religious perspectives are important in our coming to a fuller understanding of the evolution of morality. I go on to discuss animal emotions, the concept of personhood, and how our special relationships with other animals, especially the companions with whom we share our homes, help us to define our place in nature, our humanness.
    [Show full text]
  • Harnessing the Power of Doubt
    Harnessing the power of doubt CEOs must routinely make tough decisions, yet many CEOs worry they don’t have the right information—or enough of it—to know if they’re making the right decisions. Interviews with more than 150 global CEOs suggest that the key to this dilemma is to embrace uncertainty and doubt by focusing on the feeling side of decision making and not just the thinking side. Leaders who do so can bolster collaboration, encourage a culture of learning, and make better decisions—powerful weapons in an increasingly complex and uncertain world. Four decision-making pitfalls Fearlessness Viewing the decision-making process along Hubris Myopia two dimensions—feeling and knowing—helps CEOs improve their decision-making under uncertainty. As the matrix shows, an overreliance I have taken myself out of the If you don’t doubt yourself on either dimension alone is not only inef- second-guessing game, because in a constructive, positive way, fective, but risky for both CEO and organization it will drive you crazy. you are borderline dangerous for your company. alike. Two of the four critical risks our research identified are paralysis (anxiety in the face of insufficient knowledge) and hubris (fearlessness Not given the same level of information). knowing Knowing CEOs who combine the two dimensions can I try to anticipate the pros and Crystal clear . is an artificial get more comfortable with discomfort, better cons of all of the options . construct. If you’re that distinguish constructive doubt from disruptive so I always feel slightly anxious. clear, you’ve probably missed something.
    [Show full text]
  • Module 9: Accepting Uncertainty Page 1 • Psychotherapy • Research • Training What? Me Worry!?!
    What? Me Worry!?! What? Me Worry!?! What? Me Worry!?! Module 9 Accepting Uncertainty Introduction 2 Intolerance Of Uncertainty 2 Challenging Intolerance Of Uncertainty 3 Accepting Uncertainty 4 Worksheet: Accepting Uncertainty 5 Module Summary 6 About the Modules 7 The information provided in the document is for information purposes only. Please refer to the full disclaimer and copyright statements available at www.cci.health.gov.au regarding the information on this website before making use of such information. entre for C linical C I nterventions Module 9: Accepting Uncertainty Page 1 • Psychotherapy • Research • Training What? Me Worry!?! Introduction As mentioned briefly in earlier modules, the inability to tolerate uncertainty tends to be a unique feature of people who experience generalised anxiety and excessive worrying. This module aims to examine your need for certainty, to look at how this need keeps worrying going, to describe ways of challenging this need, and to discuss how to ultimately accept uncertainty in your life. Intolerance Of Uncertainty The inability to tolerate uncertainty is an attitude many people have towards life. When one has this attitude, uncertainly, unpredictability, and doubt are seen as awful and unbearable experiences that must be avoided at all costs. People who hate uncertainty and need guarantees may: • Say things like: “I can’t cope not knowing,” “I know the chances of it happening are so small, but it still could happen,” “I need to be 100% sure.” • Prefer that something bad happens right now, rather than go on any longer not knowing what the eventual outcome will be • Find it hard to make a decision or put a plan or solution in place, because they first need to know how it will work out.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Efficacy, Self-Esteem, and Subjective Happiness of Teacher Candidates at the Pedagogical Formation Certificate Program
    Journal of Education and Training Studies Vol. 4, No. 8; August 2016 ISSN 2324-805X E-ISSN 2324-8068 Published by Redfame Publishing URL: http://jets.redfame.com Self-efficacy, Self-esteem, and Subjective Happiness of Teacher Candidates at the Pedagogical Formation Certificate Program Atilgan Erozkan1, Ugur Dogan1, Arca Adiguzel1 1Mugla Sitki Kocman University, Turkey Correspondence: Ugur Dogan, Mugla Sitki Kocman University, Turkey Received: April 9, 2016 Accepted: April 29, 2016 Online Published: May 11, 2016 doi:10.11114/jets.v4i8.1535 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.11114/jets.v4i8.1535 Abstract This study investigated the relationship between self-efficacy, self-esteem, and subjective happiness. The study group is composed by 556 (291 female; 265 male) students who were studying at the pedagogical formation program at Mugla Sıtkı Kocman University. The data were collected by using the General Self-Efficacy Scale-Turkish Form, Self-Confidence Scale, and Subjective Happiness Scale. Pearson product-moment correlation analysis was employed to study the relationship between self-efficacy, self-esteem, and subjective happiness; structural equation modeling was also used for explaining subjective happiness. Initiation, effort, and persistence subdimensions of self-efficacy and internal self-confidence and external self-confidence subdimensions of self-esteem were found to be significantly correlated to subjective happiness. A significant impact of initiation, effort, and persistence subdimensions of self-efficacy and internal self-confidence and external self-confidence subdimensions of self-esteem on subjective happiness was detected. The theoretical implications of the link between self-efficacy, self-esteem, and subjective happiness were discussed. Keywords: self-efficacy, self-esteem, subjective happiness, pedagogical formation program students 1.
    [Show full text]
  • The Grief of Late Pregnancy Loss a Four Year Follow-Up
    The grief of late pregnancy loss A four year follow-up Joke Hunfeld The grief of late pregnancy loss A four year follow-up Rouwreacties bij laat zwangerschapsverlies. Een vervolgstudie over vier jaar. Proefschrift Tel' verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam op gezag van de rector magnificus Pro£dr P.W.C. Akkermans M.A. en volgens besluit van het college voor promoties. De open bare verdediging zal plaatsvinden op woensdag 13 september 1995 om 15.45 uur door Johanna Aurelia Maria Hunfeld geboren te Utrecht. Promotiecommissie: Promotoren: Pro£ jhr dr J.w, Wladimiroff Pro£ dr E Verhage Overige leden: Pro£ dr H.P. van Geijn Pro£ dr D. Tibboel Pro£ dr Ee. Verhulst Het onderzoek dat in dit proefschrift is beschreven kon worden uitgevoerd dankzij subsidies van Ontwikkelings Geneeskunde, het Universiteitsfonds van de Erasmus Universiteit en het Nationaal Fonds voor de Geestelijke Volksgezondhcid. CIP-gegevens KDninklijke Bibliotheek, Den Haag Hunfeld, J.A.M. The grief onate pregnancy loss / Johanna Aurelia Maria Hunfeld - Delft Eburon P & L Proefschrift Erasmus Universiteit Rotterdam - met samenvatting in het Nederlands ISBN 90-5651-011-8 Nugi Trefw;: perinatal grief Distributie: Eburon P&L, Postbus 2867, 2601 CW Delft Drukwerk: Ponsen & Looijen BY, Wageningen Lay-out verzorging: A. Praamstra All rights reserved Omslagtekening © P. Picasso, 1995 do Becldrecht Amsterdam © Joke Hunfeld, 1995 Rouwreacties bij laat zwangerschapsverlics Eell vcrvolgstudie over vier jaar Contents 1 Theoretical and empirical background
    [Show full text]
  • The Experience of Men After Miscarriage Stephanie Dianne Rose Purdue University
    Purdue University Purdue e-Pubs Open Access Dissertations Theses and Dissertations January 2015 The Experience of Men After Miscarriage Stephanie Dianne Rose Purdue University Follow this and additional works at: https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations Recommended Citation Rose, Stephanie Dianne, "The Experience of Men After Miscarriage" (2015). Open Access Dissertations. 1426. https://docs.lib.purdue.edu/open_access_dissertations/1426 This document has been made available through Purdue e-Pubs, a service of the Purdue University Libraries. Please contact [email protected] for additional information. THE EXPERIENCE OF MEN AFTER MISCARRIAGE A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of Purdue University by Stephanie Dianne Rose In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2015 Purdue University West Lafayette, Indiana ii To my curious, sweet, spunky, intelligent, and fun-loving daughter Amira, and to my unborn baby (lost to miscarriage February 2010), whom I never had the privilege of meeting. I am extremely happy and fulfilled being your mother. Thank you for your motivation and inspiration. iii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I am grateful to everyone who contributed to my study. Specifically, I am indebted to my sisters Sara Okello and Stacia Firebaugh for their helpful revisions, and to my parents Scott and Susan Firebaugh for their emotional and financial support along the way. I am thankful to those who provided childcare during this project, including my family and friends. My wonderful family and friends have blessed me with much support and encouragement throughout this project. I am also very grateful to my advisor Dr. Heather Servaty-Seib for her tireless support and investment in this project.
    [Show full text]
  • Or Not to Be(Lieve)? (Skepticism & Inner Peace)
    To Be(lieve) or Not to Be(lieve)? (skepticism & inner peace) 1. Ataraxia: Happiness in Skepticism: The ancient philosophers believed that there was happiness in wisdom. If we pursued truth, we would be happy. In their pursuit of Truth and Happiness, the ancient skeptics (e.g., Pyrrho of Elis, ~300 BC ; Sextus Empiricus, ~200 AD) noticed that, for pretty much any issue, a good argument could be presented for BOTH sides. Famously, the skeptic Carneades gave a public speech commending justice and our understanding of it, only to give, the very next day, a speech refuting everything he’d said the day before! (Antithesis) The result is that we have no rational basis for believing one side rather than the other, since both are equally well-supported. (Equipollence) Therefore, we must remain agnostic, suspending belief. (Epoché) The result was interesting: By suspending belief, they achieved a state of inner peace and tranquility! (Ataraxia) It was their very dissatisfaction during their quest for happiness and truth which led the first skeptics to discover the true way to happiness. Sextus Empiricus tells a story: Indeed, what happened to the Skeptic is just like what is told of Apelles the painter. For it is said that once upon a time, when he was painting a horse and wished to depict the horse’s froth, he failed so completely that he gave up and threw his sponge at the picture – the sponge on which he used to wipe the paints from his brush – and that in striking the picture the sponge produced the desired effect.
    [Show full text]
  • TRACEY Journal ISSN: 1742-3570
    TRACEY Journal ISSN: 1742-3570 Drawing||Phenomenology: tracing lived experience through drawing 2019 Volume 14 Issue 1 THE SPACE OF DRAWING: THE PLACE OF ART IN MODERN PHILOSOPHY’S THINKING OF THE VISIBLE a b Keith Crome and Ross Clark a Manchester Metropolitan University b Imperial College, London [email protected] In this article we engage with Maurice Merleau-Ponty’s claim as it is articulated in his famous last work, ‘Eye and Mind’, that Descartes’ account of space derived from the Renaissance art of perspective. We argue that not only is this account of space an essential element of Cartesian metaphysics, but that it plays a key role in modern philosophy and modern science. In part our aim is to underscore Merleau-Ponty’s recognition of the role that art plays in the genesis of the modern conception of space. However, we also argue that by way of this recognition, Merleau-Ponty seeks to release us from the limitations of this conception of space and the view of the human subject it entails, and return us to the world upon which the acts of drawing and painting draw, namely the ambiguous world of perception replete with creative potential. ojs.lboro.ac.uk/TRACEY Introduction Maurice Merleau-Ponty published just three essays on art — ‘Cezanne’s Doubt’ (1993a [1945]), ‘Indirect Language and the Voices of Silence’ (1993b [1952]), and ‘Eye and Mind’ (1993c [1960]).1 Despite this slender output, he is regarded as one the twentieth century’s greatest philosophers of art. Not only is what he says directly about art creative and powerful, his entire philosophical endeavour, dedicated to showing that if we knowers have never really known ourselves it is because we have not truly thought our bodily-being, is informed by an artist’s sensitivity to perception and the perceived world.2 His engagement with art is not for all that simply a matter of personal inclination or disposition; 3 for Merleau-Ponty, reflection on art is what forces philosophy — the thinking of thinking — into a reconsideration of its essence.
    [Show full text]
  • Desire Doubt Deception Disobedience Aroundthem
    James 1:2-3 (NIV) Consider it pure joy, my brothers and sisters, whenever What to do you face trials of many kinds, because you know that the testing of your ► faith produces perseverance... Counter the thought. 1 Corinthians 10:13 (NLT) Remember the temptations that come Colossians 2:1-7 (ESV) For I want you to know how great a struggle into your life are no different from what others experience. I have for you and for those at Laodicea and for all who have not seen me face to face, that their hearts may be encouraged, being ► Understand your . knit together in love, to reach all the riches of full assurance of weakness 1 Peter 5:8 (CEV) Be on your guard and stay awake. Your enemy, understanding and the knowledge of God's mystery, which is Christ, the devil, is like a roaring lion, sneaking around to find someone in whom are hidden all the treasures of wisdom and knowledge. I say to attack. this in order that no one may delude you with plausible arguments. For though I am absent in body, yet I am with you in spirit, rejoicing to see ► Call on . your good order and the firmness of your faith in Christ. Therefore, as Jesus you received Christ Jesus the Lord, so walk in Him, rooted and built up in Hebrews 4:16 (NIV) Let us then approach God’s throne of grace Him and established in the faith, just as you were taught, abounding in with confidence, so that we may receive mercy and find grace to thanksgiving.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Sentience and Emotions
    Animal Sentience and Emotions: The Argument for Universal Acceptance © iStock.com/TheImaginaryDuck © iStock.com/Eriko Hume © iStock.com/Eriko © iStock.com/global_explorer © iStock.com/Delpixart Prepared by Ingrid L. Taylor, D.V.M. Research Associate, Laboratory Investigations Department 1 People for the Ethical Treatment of Animals | 2021 © iStock.com/Jérémy Stenuit than “learning and memory.”4 Thus, in observations of Introduction animal behavior, descriptive labels that did not attribute any intentionality were acceptable. Noted primatologist Frans de Though the fact of animal sentience is implicit in Waal describes how, when he observed the way chimpanzees experimentation,1 researchers have traditionally downplayed would reconcile with a kiss after a fight, he was pressured to and ignored certain aspects of it, and in nonvertebrate use the phrase “postconflict reunions with mouth-to-mouth species they have often denied it altogether. While it is contact” rather than the terms “reconciliation” and “kiss.” established that vertebrate animals feel pain and respond to He goes on to state that for three decades in primatology pain drugs in much the same ways that humans do,2 emotions research, simpler explanations had to be systematically such as joy, happiness, suffering, empathy, and fear have countered before the term “reconciliation” was accepted often been ignored, despite the fact that many psychological in situations in which primates quite obviously “monitored and behavioral experiments are predicated on the assumption and repaired social relationships.”5 De Waal notes that this that animals feel these emotions and will consistently react dependence on descriptive labels, i.e., that animals can be based on these feelings.
    [Show full text]