Human Nature Review ISSN 1476-1084 URL of This Document

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Human Nature Review ISSN 1476-1084 URL of This Document The Human Nature Review ISSN 1476-1084 URL of this document http://human-nature.com/nibbs/02/pinker.html Human Nature Review 2 (2002) 444-448 Essay Review Turning the Tables on the Tabula Rasa By David P. Barash A review of The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature by Steven Pinker 509 pp, Viking, 2002. First, a confession of sorts: For some time now, cism toward a presumed sociobiologic arriviste, I’ve been suspicious of anyone who comes to I’ve become an enthusiastic student of a newly- evolution some time after their primary train- revealed Master. Pinker’s thinking and writing ing. After all, to paraphrase Winston Chur- are first-rate … maybe even better than that. chill’s observation about the British people and The Blank Slate is much-needed, long overdue the RAF, never have so many said so much and - if you are interested in what might be about something they understand so little! All called the “human nature wars” - somewhere too often, born-again evolutionists tend to con- between that old stand-by, “required reading,” form to a bimodal distribution, either disparag- and downright indispensable. It is unlikely to ing evolutionary insights – often without under- change the minds of those who are rigidly standing them, or even trying to do so - or committed to the blank slate perspective, but starry-eyed true believers, whose enthusiasm for anyone whose “nature” includes even a exceeds their common sense. And so, I was modicum of open-mindedness, it should prove initially leery of Steven Pinker, trained as he a revelation. was in visual cognition and the psychology of Sometimes I read the latest book on language. His 1994 book, The Language In- human nature simply because I feel obliged to stinct, pretty much dispelled these concerns, do so, arming myself with a response when sufficiently so that I included a selection from it friends and colleagues ask my opinion. But in my reader, Ideas of Human Nature (1998). some books are far more pleasure than obliga- Next, How the Mind Works (1997) caused me tion; these I read not only for fun, but also to to switch from judgementalism to being an ea- learn something, if only a nice way of crafting ger recipient of Pinker’s lovely insights and an argument. Pinker is definitely in the latter turns of phrase. category, except that he is not only a master Now, with The Blank Slate (2002), I’ve phrase-turner, but pretty handy with concepts, come all the way: instead of directing skepti- too. Human Nature Review, Volume 2, 2002, 444 Human Nature Review 2 (2002) 444-448 He has, for instance, a marvelous facil- and Monty Python’s Flying Circus. And he ity for explaining things by analogy: isn’t loath to touch the third rail of cognitive science: intelligence. Consider, for example, If a gear with a broken tooth goes clunk this argument (which I fully intend to steal and on every turn, we do not conclude that the deploy, if only in conversation, as my own!): tooth in its intact form was a clunk- suppressor. And so a gene that disrupts a I find it truly surreal to read academics mental ability need not be a defective denying the existence of intelligence. version of a gene that is “for” that abil- Academics are obsessed with intelli- ity.” Similarly, genes can influence a gence. They discuss it endlessly in con- wide variety of complex traits (altruism, sidering student admissions, in hiring competitiveness, and so forth) without faculty and staff, and especially in their being “for” such narrowly defined pheno- gossip about one another. Nor can citi- types. zens or policymakers ignore the concept, regardless of their politics. People who There are supposedly sophisticated biologists – say that IQ is meaningless will quickly including even some politically motivated ge- invoke it when the discussion turns to neticists such as Richard Lewontin – who just executing a murderer with an IQ of 64, don’t get this, and, as a consequence, keep ask- removing lead paint that lowers a child’s ing “Where is the gene for altruism?” trium- IQ by five points, or the presidential phantly proclaiming that without a one gene- qualifications of George W. Bush. one behavior correlation there can be no gene- behavior correlations at all! Haven’t they heard Take that, Howard Gardner! of heritability, of the impact of variations in Even an old veteran of the human nature genotype upon variations in phenotype? Or of wars such as myself, who feels that by this time the observation that when one allele is substi- he has read or heard everything, gained a slew tuted for another, the net result is likely to be a of new thoughts, phrases, and even genuine mean arithmetic consequence for fitness and ideas. Thus, thanks to The Blank Slate, I often, for phenotype as well. learned that it was Dryden, not Rousseau, who Another of Pinker’s many neat turns of gave us the noble savage: “I am free as Nature phrase and concept: first made man, Ere the base laws of servitude began, When wild in woods the noble savage The megalomania of the genes does not ran.” mean that benevolence and cooperation And consider this rather courageous ob- cannot evolve, any more than the law of servation about rape; namely, that it would be gravity proves that flight cannot evolve. perfectly compatible with supposedly progres- It means only that benevolence, like sive doctrine – which, after all, argues that hu- flight, is a special state of affairs in need man beings are malleable raw material – to of an explanation, not something that just suggest that according to prevailing blank slate happens. ideology, women might be socialized to accept rape! This is, of course, absurd and unimagin- Indeed, a particularly delightful aspect able, and Pinker knows it: His point is that hu- of The Blank Slate is Pinker’s penchant for man beings are equipped with human nature, fresh insights, not to mention suitable and often and that just as part of this nature is that men hilarious quotations dredged up from such “want sex across a wider range of circum- unlikely sources as The Brothers Karamazov stances than women do,” and are sometimes Human Nature Review, Volume 2, 2002, 445 Human Nature Review 2 (2002) 444-448 despicably willing to use violence to get it, to this intellectual fashion, as was, to some ex- women abhor being raped. This, too, is almost tent, the culture-is-all school of sociology and certainly part of human nature, and for under- anthropology, which Pinker ably chronicles and standable reasons. debunks. “My goal in this book,” writes Pinker, A cognitive psychologist at heart, “is not to argue that genes are everything and Pinker is overwhelmingly concerned with mak- culture is nothing – no one believes that – but to ing sense of the structure of the human mind; explore why the extreme position (that culture by my count, the word fitness only appears is everything) is so often seen as moderate, and twice in The Blank Slate (the book, that is … the moderate position is seen as extreme.” The fitness considerations are all over the pages of result is an impressive extended argument for our actual slates, which, it should be clear, why the blank slate is a bankrupt concept. aren’t even close to blank). This is OK. After As the predictably pithy Pinker points all, Steven Pinker isn’t a biologist. He uses out, “The mind cannot be a blank slate, because evolution to understand the mind, rather than – blank slates don’t do anything.” For this, he like Richard Dawkins, for example – using his rightly gives Leibnitz due credit (although re- mind to understand evolution. grettably omitting Kant), then goes on to show In the process, he demonstrates effec- how cognitive neuroscience has further elabo- tively how three findings occasionally raised in rated this important insight. In short: People support of a kind of blank-slatism - the com- come pre-formatted, with cognitive organizing paratively small size of the human genome, principles already embedded in their hardware. neural connectivity theory and accumulating Not a ghost in the machine, but some well-oiled evidence for cerebral plasticity - do nothing of gears. Or, at the risk of yet another metaphor, the kind. Thus, to use genome size as an exam- what about a palimpsest? ple, the evident fact that Homo sapiens appears Pinker’s review of the history of blank to boast a mere 34,000 genes has been trum- slate thinking and the struggle over recognizing peted by some as evidence that we must be human nature is admittedly biased, but nonethe- more free and less genetically constrained than less fair-minded, and – to me at least - alto- evolutionary theory demands. How nonsensi- gether convincing, a worthy companion to Carl cal! As Pinker points out, the roundworm C. Degler’s fine historical review, In Search of elegans has a mere 18,000 genes (and only a Human Nature (1991). handful of neurons, not to mention behavior One quibble, however: It wasn’t only that is less than cosmically inspired). Are we to the anti-hereditarians who went “over the top.” presume, however, that because it is outfitted Thus, it would have been helpful – and more with about one-half the human genetic com- balanced – had Pinker also included a discus- plement, C. elegans has twice our free will, or sion of extremists of the “instinctivist” school, that its behavior is only one-half as “deter- some of whom are still with us.
Recommended publications
  • Has the Decline of Violence Reversed Since the Better Angels of Our Nature Was Written?
    Has the Decline of Violence Reversed since The Better Angels of Our Nature was Written? Steven Pinker Many journalists, citing recent violence in Syria, Iraq, Gaza, and Ukraine, have asked me whether the decline of violence has gone into reverse since The Better Angels of Our Nature was written. The question betrays the same statistical misconceptions that led me to write Better Angels in the first place. People always think that violence has increased because they reason from memorable examples rather than from global data. If at any time you cherry-pick the most violent place in the world, then you’ll discover that yes, it’s violent. That has nothing to do with overall rates or trends in violence. The basic problem is that journalism is a systematically misleading way to understand the world. News is about things that happen, not about things that don’t happen. You never see a reporter standing on the streets of Angola, Sri Lanka, or Vietnam saying “I’m here reporting that a war has not broken out today.” It’s only by looking at data on the world as a whole that you get an accurate picture of the trends. Objectively, there has indeed been an uptick in war deaths in 2013 compared to 2012 (it’s too early to have data for 2014), mostly due to the war in Syria. But the overall level of deaths is still far below those of the 1960s, 1970s, and 1980s, when the world was a far more dangerous place. Even putting aside the obvious examples (such as the Cuban Missile Crisis and the 3- million-death war in Vietnam), one sees that the conflicts of today are far less damaging than those of past decades.
    [Show full text]
  • The Adaptive Significance of Human Language
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 College Scholars 2000 The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2 Recommended Citation Oesch, Nathan, "The Adaptive Significance of Human Language" (2000). Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_interstp2/52 This Project is brought to you for free and open access by the College Scholars at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Senior Thesis Projects, 1993-2002 by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Adaptive Significance of Human Language Nathan Oesch Department of Psychology University of Tennessee, Knoxville noesch @ utk. edu Abstract Many experts have argued that human language is fundamentally incompatible with the principles of traditional Darwinian evolutionary theory. According to conventional Darwinian explanations, specific traits evolved among species according to gradual and incremental genetic changes, each of which that were in some way so favorable to the survival and reproduction of ancestral generations that they were ultimately preserved within successive generations of those species. Human language, it has been said, is simply to complex to be explained as a result of Darwinian explanations, since each successive step in the evolution of language would confer no obvious survival benefits to its recipients. According to this idea, language is such an "all-or­ none system," that it could not possibly have existed in any immediately beneficial intermediate forms and thus could not have evolved according to conventional Darwinian modes of explanation.
    [Show full text]
  • Balanced Biosocial Theory for the Social Sciences
    UNLV Retrospective Theses & Dissertations 1-1-2004 Balanced biosocial theory for the social sciences Michael A Restivo University of Nevada, Las Vegas Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/rtds Repository Citation Restivo, Michael A, "Balanced biosocial theory for the social sciences" (2004). UNLV Retrospective Theses & Dissertations. 1635. http://dx.doi.org/10.25669/5jp5-vy39 This Thesis is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Thesis in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/ or on the work itself. This Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Retrospective Theses & Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected]. BALANCED BIOSOCIAL THEORY FOR THE SOCIAL SCIENCES by Michael A. Restivo Bachelor of Arts IPIoridkijSjlarrhcIJiuAHsrsity 2001 A thesis submitted in partial fulfillm ent ofdœnxpnnnnenkfbrthe Master of Arts Degree in Sociology Departm ent of Sociology College of Liberal Arts Graduate College University of Nevada, Las Vegas M ay 2004 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. UMI Number: 1422154 INFORMATION TO USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
    [Show full text]
  • Throughout His Career, Whether Studying Language
    Throughout his career, whether studying language, advocating a realistic biology of mind, or examining the human condition through the lens of humanistic Enlightenment ideas, psychologist Steven Pinker has embraced and championed a naturalistic understanding of the universe and the computational theory of mind. He is perhaps the first internationally recognized public intellectual whose recognition is based on the advocacy of empirically based thinking about language, mind, and human nature. “Just as Darwin made it possible for a thoughtful observer of the natural world to do without creationism,” he says, “Turing and others made it possible for a thoughtful observer of the cognitive world to do without spiritualism.” In the debate about AI risk, he argues against prophecies of doom and gloom, noting that they spring from the worst of our psychological biases—exemplified particularly by media reports: “Disaster scenarios are cheap to play out in the probability-free zone of our imaginations, and they can always find a worried, technophobic, or morbidly fascinated audience.” Hence, over the centuries: Pandora, Faust, the Sorcerer’s Apprentice, Frankenstein, the population bomb, resource depletion, HAL, suitcase nukes, the Y2K bug, and engulfment by nanotechnological grey goo. “A characteristic of AI dystopias,” he points out, “is that they project a parochial alpha-male psychology onto the concept of intelligence. History does turn up the occasional megalomaniacal despot or psychopathic serial killer, but these are products of a history of natural selection shaping testosterone-sensitive circuits in a certain species of primate, not an inevitable feature of intelligent systems.” In the present essay, he applauds Wiener’s belief in the strength of ideas vis-à-vis the encroachment of technology.
    [Show full text]
  • The Strange Survival and Apparent Resurgence of Sociobiology
    This is a repository copy of The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/118157/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Dennis, A. orcid.org/0000-0003-4625-1123 (2018) The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. History of the Human Sciences, 31 (1). pp. 19-35. ISSN 0952- 6951 https://doi.org/10.1177/0952695117735966 Dennis A. The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology. History of the Human Sciences. 2018;31(1):19-35. Copyright © 2017 The Author(s). DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0952695117735966. Article available under the terms of the CC- BY-NC-ND licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/). Reuse This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs (CC BY-NC-ND) licence. This licence only allows you to download this work and share it with others as long as you credit the authors, but you can’t change the article in any way or use it commercially. More information and the full terms of the licence here: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/ Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ The strange survival and apparent resurgence of sociobiology Abstract A recent dispute between Richard Dawkins and Edward O. Wilson concerning fundamental concepts in sociobiology is examined.
    [Show full text]
  • Richard Dawkins
    RICHARD DAWKINS HOW A SCIENTIST CHANGED THE WAY WE THINK Reflections by scientists, writers, and philosophers Edited by ALAN GRAFEN AND MARK RIDLEY 1 3 Great Clarendon Street, Oxford ox2 6dp Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide in Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and in certain other countries Published in the United States by Oxford University Press Inc., New York © Oxford University Press 2006 with the exception of To Rise Above © Marek Kohn 2006 and Every Indication of Inadvertent Solicitude © Philip Pullman 2006 The moral rights of the authors have been asserted Database right Oxford University Press (maker) First published 2006 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reprographics rights organization. Enquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should
    [Show full text]
  • A Critical Review of Three Theories for Music's Origin a Thesis Presented
    A Critical Review of Three Theories for Music’s Origin A thesis presented to the faculty of the College of Arts and Sciences of Ohio University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree Master of Arts Kevin W. Kondik March 2010 © 2010 Kevin W. Kondik. All Rights Reserved. 2 This thesis titled A Critical Review of Three Theories for Music’s Origin by KEVIN W. KONDIK has been approved for the Department of Philosophy and the College of Arts and Sciences by Arthur Zucker Associate Professor of Philosophy Benjamin M. Ogles Dean, College of Arts and Sciences 3 ABSTRACT KONDIK, KEVIN, W.,, M.A., March 2010, Philosophy A Critical Review of Three Theories for Music’s Origin (82 pp.) Director of Thesis: Arthur Zucker This thesis compares three theories which debate whether or not the trait of music is constitutive of a biological adaptation. Steven Pinker advances a view that music cannot be an adaptation because making or responding to music utilizes faculties which evolved for other reasons. On the next view, Geoffrey Miller claims that music is a sexually selected trait which evolved primarily to seduce potential mates. Finally, Ian Cross argues that music can be seen as an extension of juvenile behaviors into adulthood and has efficacy in the consolidation of bonds within a group. I conclude that all three theories are insufficient as an explanation of why music evolved in the hominid lineage. The main reasons why these theories all fail is they all rely upon a speculative historical reconstructions and imprecise definitions of music.
    [Show full text]
  • Biological Pluralism in Service of Biolinguistics
    Biological pluralism in service of biolinguistics Pedro Tiago Martins1, Evelina Leivada2, Antonio Ben´ıtez-Burraco3, and Cedric Boeckx2,4 1Pompeu Fabra University 2University of Barcelona 3University of Huelva 4Catalan Institute for Advanced Studies Abstract The aim of this chapter is to offer a fresh perspective on what has come to be known as biolinguistics, a term which, in our view, encompasses all research and methods devoted to the unveiling of the biological foundations of human language. More specifically, our aim is twofold: first, we point out some of the shortcomings of the naive view of biology that has been in place in linguistics since the 1950s and 1960s, namely the notion of the faculty of language as a novelty and the sharp distinction between I- and E-language, which, we contend, has not provided any major insights into the biological nature of language; second, we offer some of the insights from biology, which may provide the theoretical and methodological framework which allows for a truly biological study of language, and thus for a re-hauled biolinguistics Chomsky (1957). 1 Introduction The first sign of a biological orientation for the study of language was the work of Noam Chomsky and Eric Lenneberg, among just a few others, who in the 1950s and 1960s rejected the structuralist linguistics of the time, believing instead that languages, despite meticulously described, were not explained as a natural phenomenon. The overarching assumption of their work is that 1 languages are not learned in the conventional sense of the term (i.e. the way one would learn a craft or how to play a musical instrument), but rather a product of a biologically determined and biologically constrained capacity of humans, located in the brain, which must be innate.
    [Show full text]
  • The Language Instinct Steven Pinker June 18
    The Language Instinct Steven Pinker June 18 - June 26, 2004 Mans uniqeuness in the universe has during recent history suffered many setbacks, the latest of them being the contention that not even his language ability sets him apart from his biological cousins - the Apes. Pinker easily debunks such claims as so much media- hype and wishful thinking. Clearly if the apes would have the potential for language, they would have discovered it and exploited it for their own purposes a long time ago. In fact in actual experiments deaf observers noted far fewer incidents of true handsigning then the more guillable normal-hearers, who charitably tended to interpret any movement of the hands as imbued with intentional symbolism. True, apes do communicate with each other, by sound as well as signs in the wild, but the purported incidents of language use, were invariably repetitive and shallow, no match to the sophistication of toddlers. Man learns to walk and talk without any explict instruction. So remarkable and so universal is the effortlessness of the acquisition that it is natural to consider it as instinctive. When it comes to locomotion such claims are uncontroversial, while when loquaciousness is concerned it is more disputable. Pinker presents as evidence for an innate language ability the phenomena of pidgin and creole. When people are thrown together with no common language, a pidgin inevitably emerges. This is a primitive and provisional language, using tidbits of vocabulary from ambient tongues pasted together using a rudimentary make- shift syntax. Young children exposed to pidgin instinctively make a real language out of it, imposing syntactic rules and thus rendering it expressive.
    [Show full text]
  • 2017 Klaus J. Jacobs Research Prize Recipient Prof. Paul Bloom
    2017 Klaus J. Jacobs Research Prize Recipient Prof. Paul Bloom Paul Bloom (Canadian and American, born 1963) is the Brooks and Suzanne Ragen Professor of Psychology and Cognitive Science at Yale University. He was born in Montreal, Canada, and was an undergraduate at McGill University, working with John Macnamara. He did his doctoral work at MIT with Susan Carey and Steven Pinker. He has published in scientific journals such as Nature and Science, and in popular outlets such as The New York Times and The New Yorker. He is the co-editor of the journal Behavioral and Brain Sciences, and the author of several books. Scientific work His research explores how children and adults understand the physical and social world, with special focus on morality, religion, fiction, and art. Much of his research explores moral psychology—looking at morality in babies, developing intuitions about moral responsibility, and the role that feelings such as disgust and empathy play in our moral lives. Bloom argues that humans are in fact born with an innate sense of morality. Drawing on his research at the Infant Cognition Center at Yale University, in collaboration with his colleague and wife, Karen Wynn, he demonstrates that, even before they can speak or walk, babies judge the goodness and badness of others’ actions; feel empathy and compassion; act to soothe those in distress; and have a rudimentary sense of justice. In one of Bloom’s first studies of moral evaluation, he used a three-dimensional display in which real geometrical objects, manipulated like puppets, acted out helping/hindering situations—for instance, a yellow square would help a blue circle up the hill; a red triangle would push it down.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Is Instinct: a New Paradigm in Linguistics
    LANGUAGE IS INSTINCT: A NEW PARADIGM IN LINGUISTICS A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of Graduate Studies of The University of Guelph by ALAN BELK In partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree of Master of Arts September, 1998 O Alan Belk 1998 National Library Bibliothèque nationale du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographie Services seMces bibliographiques 39!5 WeUingtori Street 395. rue Wellington OttawaON K1AW Ottawa ON K1A ûN4 canada canada The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la National Library of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distn'bute or sell reproduire, prêter, disûi'buer ou copies of this thesis in microform, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/nIm, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique. The author retains ownership of the L'auteur conserve la propriété du copyright in this thesis. Neither the droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. thesis nor substantial extracts fkom it Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otheNVIse de celle-ci ne doivent Seimprimés reproduced without the author's ou autrement reproduits sans son permission. autorisation, LANGUAGE IS INSTINCT: A NEW PARADIGM IN LINGUISTICS Alan Belk Advisor: University of Guelph 1998 Professor Michael Ruse If we can idencify a science, descnbe its paradigm, anomalies and research questions, recognize a cnsis or impending crisis and a cornpeting paradigm, then a revolution as described by Thomas Kuhn (1970) is taking place. Steven Pinker's book The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creares Language is, according to its Preface, intended to explain the current state of knowledge about language.
    [Show full text]
  • Beyond Grammar Linguistics in Language and Writing Courses
    Access Provided by Queens College (CUNY) at 08/21/11 6:51PM GMT Beyond Grammar Linguistics in Language and Writing Courses Ann Evans Over the past half century, traditional rule- based prescriptive/proscriptive teaching methods have been largely discarded. Prescriptive pedagogy had seemed so simple — tell the students what to do and have them do it — but students found it boring, and researchers and teachers thought there could be a better way. Usage rules like “don’t split an infinitive” and “never say ‘ain’t’ ” ultimately lose their integrity anyway for two reasons: (1) language is always changing, and (2) commonly accepted usage often does not follow the rules. Certain prescriptive aids are still widely and justifiably used but are no longer the main emphasis in the classroom. Noam Chomsky revolutionized the way we look at language with his book Syntactic Structures (1957). Researchers have since enhanced, chal- lenged, and expanded upon Chomsky’s work in the area of syntax, and still others have advanced our knowledge in related fields such as sociolinguistics, semantics, psycholinguistics, and historical linguistics. The results of this extensive research have not been included in general curricula, nor have they become common knowledge within the larger population. In 1964, M. A. K. Halliday, Angus McIntosh, and Peter Strevens argued that the locus for a more ample, holistic study of language should be the secondary school: “Is our language so poor and uninteresting a thing that we put it in the school curriculum only in order to fight for its lost causes, to pass pathetic judgments on some of its marginal features? We should be Pedagogy: Critical Approaches to Teaching Literature, Language, Composition, and Culture Volume 11, Number 2 doi 10.1215/15314200-1218067 © 2011 by Duke University Press 285 ashamed to let anyone leave our secondary schools knowing so little of how their language works and of the part it plays in our lives” (230).
    [Show full text]