J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.236 on 1 February 2001. Downloaded from 236 J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;70:236–239

SHORT REPORT

Unusual spontaneous and training induced visual field recovery in a patient with a gunshot lesion

D A Poggel, E Kasten, E M Müller-Oehring, B A Sabel, S A Brandt

Abstract field size could not be ruled out (for example, Over a period of more than 3 years, variability of perimetric measurements, change changes in visual and neuropsychological of detection strategies, or compensatory eccen- functions were examined in a patient with tric fixation) (see also Balliet et al3 for a review). a visual field defect caused by a cerebral However, more recent, methodologically well gunshot lesion. Initially, the patient had controlled studies provided evidence for train- been completely blind, but after 6 months ing induced recovery of visual functions.45 of , he showed a Here, we report on a patient with a traumati- homonymous bilateral lower quadran- cally induced visual field defect showing tanopia and impairment of higher visual significant spontaneous recovery and consider- functions. Unexpectedly, recovery still able further progress during visual restitution continued after the first 6 months. This training. For the first time we could show in process was documented in detail by detail that spontaneous as well as training visual field examinations using high reso- induced improvement take place in the area of lution perimetry. When visual field size residual vision and that both processes seem to had stabilised almost 16 months after the be based on a common mechanism—that is, lesion, further improvement could be the modulation of perceptual thresholds in achieved by visual restitution training. partially defective areas. The duration and extent of spontaneous recovery were unusual. In spontaneous as Case report well as in training induced recovery, A 29 year old man with no history of progress was mainly seen in partially neurological disease was attacked and shot in defective areas (areas of residual vision) the back of his head. When regaining con- along the visual field border. Thus, it is sciousness in the hospital, he complained of a speculated that modulation of perceptual complete loss of vision. Initially he also had http://jnnp.bmj.com/ thresholds in transition zones of visual anosmia, reduced vigilance, and a mild left field defects contributes to spontaneous sided hemiparesis. Reduction of vigilance and and training induced recovery. hemiparesis recovered completely within days. (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;70:236–239) After 1 week, vision had recovered in the right upper quadrant, and central vision was suY- Keywords: visual field; hemianopia; recovery of function Institute of Medical cient to perform a neuropsychological exam- Psychology, ination which disclosed a slowing of reaction Otto-von-Guericke times as well as an impairment of short term on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. University, Leipziger The human brain possesses a remarkable abil- and visuospatial functions. Verbal Strasse 44, 39120 ity to regain functions after lesions of various intelligence was normal. Magdeburg, Germany aetiologies. However, recovery from visual field Six months after the incident, we examined D A Poggel defects in patients with brain injury has long E Kasten the patient for the first time in our department. E M Müller-Oehring been considered impossible due to the highly General and cardiovascular examination B A Sabel specific structure of the visual system. Mean- yielded normal results. Higher cortical func- while, it is generally accepted that spontaneous tions and cranial nerves were normal except for Department of improvement can occur in patients with visual a bilateral lower quadrantanopia and hypos- , Charité, field loss, but there is still a controversial mia. Best corrected visual acuity was 100%. Humboldt University, discussion on the possibility of increasing Schumannstrasse Muscle tone, muscle strength, and reflexes 20–21, visual field size by systematic training. Zihl and were normal and symmetric. There was 1 10117 Berlin, Germany von Cramon tried to enlarge the visual field of hypaesthesia in a region at the back of the head S A Brandt 55 patients with postgeniculate lesions using a where the bullet had entered, otherwise sensa- forced choice saccadic technique to detect light tion was normal. In a detailed neuropsycho- Correspondence to: stimuli. In most patients, visual field enlarge- Dr B A Sabel@ logical examination, his performance was nor- medizin.uni-magdeburg.de ment did not exceed 5°, but there were mal, except in visuospatial tasks and in tests individual cases with remarkable recovery. involving a considerable amount of visual Received 13 March 2000 and Nevertheless, their findings were regarded as scanning (see below). Subjectively, the patient in revised form 2 29 September 2000 artefacts by Balliet et al. because alternative had diYculties in estimating direction and Accepted 11 October 2000 explanations for the increase of intact visual speed of moving objects. This caused problems

www.jnnp.com J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.236 on 1 February 2001. Downloaded from Unusual spontaneous and training induced visual field recovery in a gunshot lesion 237

A1 3 19 85 163

10° 20° 30°

B28 50 85 0° 0° 0°

–10° –10° –10°

–20° –20° –20°

–30° –30° –30°

–20° –10° 0° 10° 20° –20° –10° 0° 10° 20° –20° –10° 0° 10° 20°

C 99 120 176

0° 0° 0°

–10° –10° –10°

–20° –20° –20°

–30° –30° –30°

–20° –10° 0° 10° 20° –20° –10° 0° 10° 20° –20° –10° 0° 10° 20°

Figure 1 Spontaneous and training induced recovery of visual field. (A) Perimetric measurements (grey=blind, white=intact); weeks 1, 3, 19, 85 (spontaneous recovery), and 163 (training induced recovery) after lesion. (B) Measurements of lower visual field with computer based high resolution perimetry at weeks 28, 50, and 85. (C) Results from the beginning of the 1st training unit (week 99), the end of the 3rd training unit (week 120), and the end of the 8th training-unit (week 176); five subsequent training sessions are superimposed in each picture; grey areas correspond to partially defective regions of visual field. riding a bicycle, pouring liquids into a glass, background=26 cd/m2) were presented in ran- and walking through crowds of people. dom order at 474 positions in a grid of 25×19 Axial CT images (2 mm slice thickness) and stimulus locations. The subject was instructed coronal reconstructions showed that the bullet to keep his eyes on the fixation point through- had entered the left occipital lobe (about 5 cm out the test and to press the space bar on the http://jnnp.bmj.com/ cranial and 3 cm lateral to the inion) and computer keyboard on detection of a stimulus followed a trajectory through the interhemi- or of a fixation control (an isoluminant change spheric fissure into the right temporal lobe, of the fixation point’s colour). Additionally, eye where the bullet is still located. A hypodense position was controlled by the experimenter area involving the upper part of the cuneus and noting the subject’s fixation behaviour in a the caudal part of the precuneus bilaterally mirror. Interstimulus intervals were ran- could be identified. The lesion extended bilat- domised to prevent guessing behaviour. Total erally to the ventral part of the upper calcarine duration of the visual field test was about 20 on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. bank, but spared striate cortex in the lower cal- minutes. The number of hits, misses, and false carine bank and the occipital poles. In the right positives as well as reaction times were hemisphere the hypodense area extended into registered by the program. Positions of cor- the posterior part of the medial temporal gyrus. rectly detected stimuli and misses were plotted as white and black squares in the stimulus grid, Methods respectively. A variation in this perimetric pro- Visual field recovery was found with diVerent cedure allowed for the testing of colour methods of quantitative perimetry (Allergan discrimination and form recognition. Humphrey 60°, Octopus 90°, Tübinger Auto- For visual field enlargement, we applied mated Perimeter 30° and 90°; see fig 1; see also computer based visual restitution training.47 Lachenmeyer and Vivell6 for description). Here, luminous stimuli were presented at ran- Additionally, we used a high resolution com- dom locations in a previously defined training puter based perimetric test78for more detailed area on a dark computer screen, each increas- examination of the central visual field. Testing ing in brightness from dark grey (30 cd/m2)to was done in a darkened room (background bright white (96 cd/m2) over a period of 2000 luminance=1.5 Lx) in front of a computer ms. Stimulus size, fixation control, mode of screen covering 54.8° horizontally and 42.3° response, and viewing distance were the same vertically. White light stimuli (luminance=96 as in the visual field test. Training was cd/m2; size=0.76°; presentation time=150 ms, performed at home for 1 hour daily. Treatment

www.jnnp.com J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.236 on 1 February 2001. Downloaded from 238 Poggel, Kasten, Müller-Oehring, et al

Start of high resolution Start of visual parallel, colour and form recogni- perimetric measurements restitution training tion recovered in the transition zone, and he 100 350 subjectively perceived an improvement of 90 visual orientation and spatial perception. 300 After visual field size had remained constant 80 for more than 7 months (week 51 to 94), visual 70 250 restitution training was started. The patient showed a remarkable but 60 200 variable further increase of visual field size over 50 the next 80 weeks of training (fig 1). Again, this 150 40 improvement was found almost exclusively in the transition zones of the scotoma with 30 Lower visual field No of detected stimuli 100 peripheral parts of the visual field defect recov- 20 Lower visual field % intact ering more and faster than central parts. Inter- 50 estingly, we found a striking discrepancy 10 Central visual field % intact between the position of the visual field border 0 0 010 3020 40 50 60 70 80 90 110100 120 130 140 150 160 170 180 in our standard perimetric measurements versus the training related perimetric results. Time since lesion (weeks) Obviously, our patient was able to detect light Figure 2 Course of spontaneous and training induced recovery. Graphs show the stimuli during the training program when they enlargement of visual field over the complete course of spontaneous and training induced recovery. The dotted line with the open circles depicts the increase of per cent intact lower were presented for 2000 ms, but he could not visual field as measured by high resolution perimetry (fixation point at upper edge of the perceive a light stimulus at the same position screen). The dashed line with the filled circles shows changes of per cent intact area of the when it appeared only for 150 ms in the central visual field measured with standard perimetry. The solid line demonstrates the parallel increase of the number of detected stimuli in high resolution perimetry of the lower perimetric test. visual field. We quantified recovery over time using two measures (fig 2): (1) increase of per cent intact results were saved on a disk so that compliance lower visual field and (2) number of detected and changes in visual field size could be stimuli. Mean size of the lower intact visual followed minutely over sessions. After each field in the period of spontaneous recovery training unit (56 sessions), the patient was (week 1 to 51) was 27.8% (SD12.9%) —that examined with perimetric tests, training results is, 137 of 499 stimuli (SD 65.2) detected in were analysed, and the training area was high resolution perimetry. During that time, adapted to changes of the visual field border. visual field size increased substantially from Neuropsychological assessment was per- 0% to 51% intact. Between weeks 51 and 94 formed with several paper and pencil tests and visual field testing yielded stable results (base- computer based methods—for example, d2, line) with an average intact lower field of 49.6% TAP, and ZVT ( and visual explora- (SD1.41%)—that is, 250/499 detected stimuli tion), HAWIE-R and SPM (intelligence), and (SD7.4). In the period of visual field training Benton test, SKT, and IST-70 (visuospatial 9 (weeks 94 to 176), mean visual field size functions). increased again with occasional peak levels reaching 66% intact visual field size (330/499 Results stimuli). Albeit, mean size of intact visual field After initial blindness, the patient reported a during training was 54.8% (SD 8.6%)—that is, http://jnnp.bmj.com/ diVuse perception of light 5 days after the inci- 273/499 stimuli (SD42.6), which was not dent. Visual field size increased rapidly, starting significantly diVerent from baseline (weeks 51 in the upper right quadrant (fig 1). In our first to 94). However, during the last two measure- examination, 6 months after the lesion, we ments of the training period his visual field size found an incomplete bilateral lower quadran- had clearly improved in comparison with the tanopia (fig 1). He showed a mild impairment baseline level. He detected luminous stimuli, in of form perception, visual orientation, recogni- particular regions of the transition zone where on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. tion of object size, and spatial relations. The he had never perceived them before. patient showed no deficits in memory, speech, Subjectively, the patient reported an im- or intelligence, nor was he slowed in a simple provement in everyday function—for example, reaction task. Moreover, we found no sign of less diYculties riding a bicycle, shaking some- visuospatial neglect, but selective attention in a body’s hand, or avoiding obstacles on the Stroop paradigm was impaired. During the ground. However, visual exploration and the first 4 weeks, he had complex pseudohallucina- perception of movements were still diYcult for tions, including people and scenarios, located him, although neither a test of visual scanning in the lower visual quadrants.10 nor tests of higher order visual functions Twenty eight weeks after lesion, baseline (object , prosopagnosia, depth percep- testing with computer based perimetry7 was tion) showed any impairment. begun. Unexpectedly, visual field size still increased over the next months so that restitu- tion training was not started. During that Discussion period, we found a gain of more than 20% in The bilateral lower quadrantanopia was a perimetric tests of the lower visual field until result of traumatic injury of both occipital measurements stabilised 16 months after the lobes and is in full agreement with earlier gunshot. This improvement was found almost descriptions of visual field defects. Our re- exclusively in partially defective areas (transi- search interest was focused on the time course tion zones) at the visual field border (fig 1). In and phenomenology of spontaneous recovery

www.jnnp.com J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry: first published as 10.1136/jnnp.70.2.236 on 1 February 2001. Downloaded from Unusual spontaneous and training induced visual field recovery in a gunshot lesion 239

from visual field defects and additional eVects size like in the case presented here. Therefore, of systematic visual field training. we conclude that partially defective neuronal Three findings are unusual in the case areas representing transition zones in the visual reported here. In most studies on spontaneous field may be crucial not only for spontaneous recovery, the maximum duration of visual field improvement but brain plasticity and func- increase ranged between 3 weeks and 6 tional recovery in these areas can also be months.11 By contrast, the patient’s spontane- induced by systematic stimulation as provided ous recovery ended only 16 months after during visual restitution training. lesion. Secondly, the amount of spontaneous improvement was striking in that he regained This work was supported by grants from the state of almost two thirds of his visual field although Saxonia-Anhalt (FKZ: 1367A/0083B, 820A/8283B) and the average recovery in patients with complete cor- Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, by grants from the 11 Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (Sa 433/ 6–1,2) and a schol- tical blindness usually amounts to about 28%. arship from the “Graduiertenkolleg Biologische Grundlagen Thirdly, even after the termination of sponta- von Erkrankungen des Nervensystems”. We thank Ulrike neous recovery and a long period of stable Bunzenthal for help with data acquisition and Christoph Ploner for helpful comments on the manuscript. visual field size, further—albeit smaller and inconstant—improvement could be achieved by systematic stimulation during visual field 1 Zihl J, von Cramon DY. Visual field recovery from scotoma in patients with postgeniculate damage. A review of 55 training starting almost 2 years after the lesion. cases. Brain 1985;108:335–65. In concordance with earlier studies,412 im- 2 Balliet R, Blood KMT, Bach-y-Rita P. Visual field rehabili- tation in the cortically blind? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry provement was mainly found at the borders of 1985;48:1113–24. visual field defects—that is, in partially defec- 3 Pambakian ALM, Kennard C. Can visual function be 13 restored in patients with homonymous hemianopia? Br J tive areas. Analysis of reaction times to light Ophthalmol 1997;81:324–8. stimuli in the perimetric test showed a consid- 4 Kasten E, Wüst S, Behrens-Baumann W, et al. Computer- based training for the treatment of partial blindness. Nat erable slowing of responses to stimuli pre- Med 1998;4:1083–7. sented in the transition zone compared with 5 Kasten E, Poggel DA, Müller-Oehring EM, et al. Restora- tion of vision II: residual functions and training-induced those appearing in completely intact areas. visual field enlargement in brain-damaged patients. Restora- Subjectively, stimuli seem to be reduced in tive Neurology and Neuroscience 1999;15:273–87. 6 Lachenmayr BJ, Vivell PMO. Perimetrie. Stuttgart: Thieme brightness and do not have clear contours. Verlag, 1992. Additionally, light stimuli cannot be perceived 7 High resolution perimetry (HRP)/ visual restitution training (VRT). Nova vision: software for diagnosis of visual field when presentation time is too short, explaining defects. Magdeburg, Germany: 1998. Nova Vision AG the diVerence between the visual field border (www.nova-vision.org) 8 Kasten E, Strasburger H, Sabel BA. Programs for diagnosis found in our standard perimetric tests and and therapy of visual field deficits in vision rehabilitation. training related perimetric measurements. Spat Vis 1997;10:499–503. 9 Brickenkamp R, ed. Handbuch psychologischer und pädago- These findings suggest that perceptual thresh- gischer Tests. Göttingen: Hogrefe Verlag, 1997. olds are increased in areas of residual vision. 10 Gruesser OJ, Landis T. Vision and visual dysfunction: visual and other disturbances of and cogni- During the process of spontaneous as well as tion. Vol 12. Houndmills: Macmillan, 1991. training induced recovery, these thresholds are 11 Tiel-Wilck, K. Rückbildung homonymer Gesichtsfelddefekte nach Infarkten im Versorgungsgebiet der Arteria cerebri posterior reduced so that perception is gradually normal- [Dissertation]. Berlin, Germany: Freie Universität, 1991. ised. This mechanism has been postulated by 12 Zihl J, von Cramon DY. Restitution of visual function in 1 patients with cerebral blindness. J Neurol Neurosurg Zihl and von Cramon as the basis of their Psychiatry 1979;42:312–22. training results, but it has not been related 13 Sabel BA. Unrecognized potential of surviving neurons: before to detailed observations of spontaneous within-systems plasticity, recovery of function, and the http://jnnp.bmj.com/ hypothesis of minimal residual structure. The Neuroscientist and training induced increase of visual field 1997;3:366–70. on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright.

www.jnnp.com