Thinking-And-Speech.Pdf

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Thinking-And-Speech.Pdf Thinking and Speech. L. S. Vygotsky 1934 Author’s Preface This book deals with one of the most complex and difficult problems of experimental psychology, the problem of thinking and speech. To our knowledge, there has as yet been no systematic attempt to address this problem experimentally. An extended series of empirical studies is required. These studies must address issues such as the nature of concepts formed in experimental contexts, the nature of written speech, and the relationship of written speech to thinking. These studies are a necessary and important first step in resolving this general problem. Experimental research, however, must be supplemented with theoretical and critical analysis. First, we must analyse the empirical data which have been accumulated and attempt to assess their more general implications. Through an analysis of available information on phylogenesis and ontogenesis, we must attempt to identify the most useful point of departure for the resolution of the problem; we must attempt to develop a general theory of the genetic roots of thinking and speech. Second, to develop preliminary working hypotheses and contrast our own theoretical perspectives with those of others, we must conduct a critical analysis of the best contemporary theories of thinking and speech. In our view, current theories are in need of serious reassessment and improvement. Theoretical analysis is necessary in two other contexts. First, the problem of thinking and speech has been addressed by scholars in several closely related fields. We must compare these differing perspectives on various aspects of the problem. In this book, we have compared data on the psychology of speech with related work in linguistics. We have also compared our own data on concept formation with current educational theory on the development of concepts. In general, we found it most useful to address these problems in theoretical terms, without attempting a detailed analysis of empirical data. Thus, in describing our studies on the development of scientific concepts, we have presented a working hypothesis on the relationship of instruction and development. This hypothesis was developed in a separate work and is based on empirical data that we do not review in detail here. We also relied on a theoretical mode of analysis in our attempt to construct a theoretical perspective that would incorporate all our experimental data. The content and structure of our research have been varied and complex. Nonetheless, each task we have faced in our work has been subordinated to our general goal and connected to the work that preceded and followed it. Though we have broken the problem down into several components, it is our hope that our studies constitute a unified whole. All our work has focused on a single basic problem, on the genetic analysis of the relationship between thought and word. 39 40 Thinking and Speech This single basic task has defined the structure of our general research program and defines that of the present book as well. We begin this book with an attempt to formulate a clear statement of the problem and identify appropriate research methods. This is followed by a critical analysis of the two most powerful and complete contemporary theories of the development of speech and thinking, those advanced by Jean Piaget and William Stern. The purpose of this analysis was to contrast our statement of the problem and our research methods with more traditional perspectives and methodologies. Then, as a preface to the discussion of our experimental research, we were forced to address theoretical issues concerning the genetic roots of thinking and speech. This was necessary in order to identify a point of departure for research on the genesis of verbal thinking. Two empirical studies constitute the core of the book. The first is a study of the development of word meaning in childhood; the second a comparison of the nature and development of scientific and spontaneous concepts in ontogenesis. The final chapter represents an attempt to integrate our findings, to present the process of verbal thinking in a connected and unified form. In any innovative research effort, an attempt must be made to identify novel aspects of the research, since these require particularly careful analysis and experimental validation. If, for the moment, we ignore the new approach inherent in our statement of the problem and what is, in effect, a new research method, we can summarize the contributions of our research effort in the following way: (1) we have provided experimental evidence indicating that word meaning develops in childhood and identified the basic stages of this process; (2) we have identified the process involved in the development of scientific concepts and demonstrated how this process differs from the development of spontaneous concepts; (3) we have clarified the psychological nature of written speech as a distinct speech function and explored its relationship to thinking; and (4) we have made a contribution to the experimental study of inner speech and the relationship between inner speech and thinking. These represent contributions to the general theory of thinking and speech, contributions that derive from new experimental data and from the working hypotheses and theories that inevitably arose in the process of interpreting, explaining, and analysing these data. Of course, it is neither the right nor the obligation of the author to evaluate the significance or validity of these theories and data. We leave this task to our readers and critics. This book is the product of nearly ten years work. Many of the questions that emerged in the investigation were not apparent to us when we began. We were frequently forced to reconsider our positions during the investigation. Consequently, the results of a great deal of hard work had to be discarded. Much of the remainder had to be redone, restructured, redefined, or rewritten. Nevertheless, the overall direction of our research developed steadily, on a foundation that was basic to our work from the outset. In this book, we have attempted to make explicit much that remained implicit in previous work. Still, there is a great deal that we once believed to be correct that has been excluded from this book because it represented simple delusion on our part. Several sections of the book were taken from earlier works published in manuscript form as the basis for a university course (Chapter 5). Others were published as introductions to the works of the authors on which they focus (Chapter 2 and Chapter 4). The remaining chapters, and the book as a whole, are published here for the first time. We are only too aware of the limitations of this first step we have taken in developing a new approach to the study of the relationship between thinking and speech. In our view, our effort has been justified in that it has improved our understanding of the problem, demonstrated the importance of the problem to the whole of human psychology, and provided us with a new theory of the psychology of consciousness. This latter issue, however, is addressed only briefly in the concluding words of the book. The investigation is broken off at its threshold. Thinking and Speech. Vygotsky 1934 Chapter 1 The Problem and the Method of Investigation The first issue that must be faced in the analysis of thinking and speech concerns the relationship among the various mental functions, the relationship among the various forms of the activity of consciousness. This issue is fundamental to many problems in psychology. In the analysis of thinking and speech, the central problem is that of the relationship of thought to word. All other issues are secondary and logically subordinate; they cannot even be stated properly until this more basic issue has been resolved. Remarkably, the issue of the relationships among the various mental functions has remained almost entirely unexplored. In effect, it is a new problem for contemporary psychology. In contrast, the problem of thinking and speech is as old as psychology itself. However, the issue of the relationship of thought to word remains the most confused and least developed aspect of the problem. The atomistic and functional forms of analysis that dominated psychology during past decade led to the analysis of the mental functions in isolation from one another. Psychological methods and research strategies have developed and matured in accordance with this tendency to study separate, isolated, abstracted processes. The problem of the connections among the various mental functions ‒ the problem of their organization in the integrated structure of consciousness ‒ has not been included within the scope of the research. There is, of course, nothing novel in the notion that consciousness is a unified whole, that the separate functions are linked with one another in activity. Traditionally, however, the unified nature of consciousness ‒ the connections among the mental functions ‒ have simply been accepted as given. They have not been the object of empirical research. The reason for this becomes apparent only when we become aware of an important tacit assumption, an assumption that has become part of the foundation of psychological research. This assumption (one that was never clearly formulated and is entirely false) is that the links or connections among the mental functions are constant and unvarying, that the relationships between perception and attention, memory and perception, and thought and memory are unchanging. This assumption implies that the relationships among functions can be treated as constants and that these constants do not have to be considered in studies that focus on the functions themselves. As we mentioned earlier, the result has been that the 41 42 Thinking and Speech problem of interfunctional relationships has remained largely unexplored in modern psychology. Inevitably, this had a serious impact on the approach to the problem of thinking and speech.
Recommended publications
  • An Analysis of Total Personality As Advocated by Holistic Theorists and Its Effect Upon Healthy Personality
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 039 610 24 CG 005 481 AUTHOR Frick, Willard B. TITLE An Analysis of Total Personality as Advocated by Holistic Theorists and Its Effect upon Healthy Personality. Final Report. INSTITUTION Albion Coll., Mich. SPONS AGENCY Office of Education (DHEW) , Washington, D.C. Rureau of Research. BUREAU NO BR-8-E-076 PUB DATE 30 Apr 70 GRANT OrG-5-9-325076-0004 NOTE 57p. EDRS PRICE FDRS Price MF-$0.25 HC-$2.0F DESCRIPTORS Behavior Theories, Concept Formation, *Developmental Psychology, Individual Characteristics, *Individual Psychology, Personal Growth, *Personality, *Personality Theories, *Psychology ABSTRACT This conceptual work is concerned with the development of a holistic theory of personality. The following were selected for their strong orientation in this direction: Gordon Allport, Andras Angyal, Kurt Goldstein, Prescott Lecky, Abraham Maslow, Bardner Murphy, and Carl Rogers. The four themes which emerged from an analysis of their writings are concerned with organization, motivation, process and potentiality. With three minor exceptions, the seven holistic theorists were virtually identical in their basic principles and thematic concerns. The four themes form a supportive structure for the development of a holistic theory of healthy personality and are offered as evidence that the selected theorists represent a shared orientation toward the development of the human personality. An analysis and subsequent synthesis of the seven individual theories is presented. Implications for the theory and practice of psychotherapy and education are cffered.(Author/CJ) 1-"Anal Report Project No, 8.-E -076 Grant No, OEG-5-9-325076-0004 U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH, EDUCATION & WELFARE OFFICE OF EDUCATION THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRODUCED EXACTLY ASRECEIVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIGINATING IT.
    [Show full text]
  • Um Estudo Sobre Goldstein E Vigotski Arquivos Brasileiros De Psicologia, Vol
    Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia ISSN: 0100-8692 [email protected] Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro Brasil Pereira dos Santos, Hernani; Batista Martins, Joao A estrutura da personalidade: um estudo sobre Goldstein e Vigotski Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, vol. 68, núm. 2, 2016, pp. 99-113 Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro Rio de Janeiro, Brasil Disponível em: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=229048487009 Como citar este artigo Número completo Sistema de Informação Científica Mais artigos Rede de Revistas Científicas da América Latina, Caribe , Espanha e Portugal Home da revista no Redalyc Projeto acadêmico sem fins lucrativos desenvolvido no âmbito da iniciativa Acesso Aberto ARTIGOS A estrutura da personalidade: um estudo sobre Goldstein e Vigotski Hernani Pereira dos SantosI João Batista MartinsII A estrutura da personalidade: um estudo sobre Goldstein e Vigotski RESUMO Este estudo teórico busca compreender a relação entre as ideias de Vigotski e de Goldstein. O objetivo é oferecer uma aproximação das ideias dos dois autores através da exposição e análise das teses fundamentais de cada quadro interpretativo no que diz respeito ao conceito de personalidade. Consiste em três partes. Na primeira, faz-se uma exposição das pesquisas de Gelb e de Goldstein sobre a afasia. Na segunda, é feita uma apresentação das teses gerais da teoria organísmica de Goldstein. E na terceira, desenvolve-se uma análise de pontos de divergência e de convergência da teoria de Vigotski com respeito a determinadas teses de Goldstein. Como conclusão, argumenta-se que as duas teorias divergem em termos de metodologia e de ontologia, mas se complementam, em diferentes níveis de complexidade, e se aproximam em seu motivo-condutor epistemológico.
    [Show full text]
  • Culture, History, and Psychology in New York City
    Pickren, W. (2011). New York state of mind: culture, history, and psychology in New York City. Memorandum, 21, 21-38. Recuperado em __ de _______________, ____, seer.ufmg.br/index.php/memorandum/article/view/9774 21 New York state of mind: culture, history, and psychology in New York City Wade Pickren Pace University - New York United States Abstract Place is important for understanding knowledge and scientific practices. In turn, knowledge and practice influence the place they occur. Place is not simply the stage where the real action happens, but it is itself constitutive of systems of human interaction, thus ideas are produced and shaped in resonance with their environments. Here the author argues that New York City was an important site for the growth and diversification of application of psychology during the decade of the 1920s. The city both shaped the science and practice that grew there and was shaped by it. Keywords: place; geography of knowledge; construction of scientific practice; applied psychology; New York City Over the last decade, historians of science have shown how knowledge and practice have to become local knowledge and local practice in order to find an enduring place in a Memorandum 21, out/2011 Belo Horizonte: UFMG; Ribeirão Preto: USP ISSN 1676-1669 seer.ufmg.br/index.php/memorandum/article/view/9774 Pickren, W. (2011). New York state of mind: culture, history, and psychology in New York City. Memorandum, 21, 21-38. Recuperado em __ de _______________, ____, seer.ufmg.br/index.php/memorandum/article/view/9774 22 society (e.g. Livingstone, 2003; Raj, 2007; Safier, 2008).
    [Show full text]
  • Building a “Cross-Roads Discipline” at Mcgill University: a History of Early Experimental Psychology in Postwar Canada
    BUILDING A “CROSS-ROADS DISCIPLINE” AT MCGILL UNIVERSITY: A HISTORY OF EARLY EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY IN POSTWAR CANADA ERIC OOSENBRUG A dissertation submitted to the Faculty of Graduate Studies in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Program in Psychology. Graduate Program in Psychology York University Toronto, Ontario October 2020 © Eric Oosenbrug, 2020 Abstract This dissertation presents an account of the development of psychology at McGill University from the late nineteenth century through to the early 1960s. The department of psychology at McGill represents an alternative to the traditional American-centered narrative of the cognitive revolution and later emergence of the neurosciences. In the years following World War II, a series of psychological experiments established McGill as among the foremost departments of psychology in North America. This thesis is an institutional history that reconstructs the origins, evolution, and dramatic rise of McGill as a major center for psychological research. The experiments conducted in the early 1950s, in the areas of sensory restriction, motivation, and pain psychology, were transformative in their scope and reach. Central to this story is Donald O. Hebb, author of The Organization of Behavior (1949), who arrived at McGill in 1947 to find the charred remains of a department. I argue that the kind of psychology Hebb established at McGill was different from most departments in North America; this is developed through a number of interwoven storylines focused on the understanding of a particular character of McGill psychology - a distinctive “psychological style” - and its broader historical importance for Canadian psychology, for North American psychology, and for psychology across the globe.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurologie & Rehabilitation
    DEZEMBER 2015 | 21. JAHRGANG | ISSN 0947-2177 | G 13574 | HERAUSGEBER: C. DETTMERS | P. W. SCHÖNLE | C. WEILLER Organ der NEUROLOGIE & DGNR DGNKN OEGNR REHABILITATION SGNR Neuroprotektion | Neuroplastizität | Neurologische Langzeittherapie 6 | 2015 Zum 50. Todesjahr von Kurt Goldstein (1878–1965) Kontroverse zwischen Viktor von Weizsäcker, Kurt Goldstein und Otfrid Foerster zum Lokalisationsprinzip 1930 | W. Rimpau Einsame Entscheidungen. Zur Wiederkehr einer Problemkonstellation im Werk von Kurt Goldstein | G. Kreft Roland Kuhn: Zum Problem der ganzheitlichen Betrachtung in der Medizin | H. G. Müldner Die Suche der Neuropsychologie nach sich selbst | S. Frisch Kurt Goldsteins Konzeption der neurologischen »Katastrophen- reaktionen« aus heutiger Perspektive | F. W. Stahnisch Kurt Goldstein’s innovative approach of neuropsychological assessment | P. Eling The narrative hinge between the neurology and the psychology of neurotraumatic responses | M. I. Medved, J. Brockmeier Was bedeuten aphasische Symptome? Die Position von Goldstein und Gelb | H. Grötzbach, L. Spitzer Kurt Goldstein und Adhémar Gelb: Haben sie eine Aktualität für die heutige Neurorehabilitation und Neuropsychologie? P. Frommelt Gastherausgeber: P. Frommelt, H. Grötzbach INHALT 6INHALT | 2015 KURT GOLDSTEIN ZUM 50. TODESJAHR Die funktionelle Schluckuntersuchung in der Neurologie (FEES) – ein Videolehrgang Die Fiberendoskopische Evaluation des Schluckens (FEES) mit einem flexiblen Laryngoskop hat sich seit der Erstbeschreibung durch Susam Langmore zu ei- ner Standarduntersuchung
    [Show full text]
  • Abraham H. Maslow an Intellectual Biography
    ABRAHAM H. MASLOW (1908-1970) AN INTELLECTUAL BIOGRAPHY ROY JOSE DECARV ALHO SOME PSYCHOLOGISTS DURING THE "GOLDEN AGE" OF BEHAVIORISM after World War II, discontented with behaviorism's view of human nature and method, drew upon a long tradition linking psychology with humanities and in a rebellious manner institutionally founded humanistic psychology. They believed they were a "third force," an alternative to the dominant behavioristic and psychoanalytical orientation in psychology. Some of the best minds of the psychological world of the 1960s, such as Gordon Allport, Carl Rogers, Rollo May, and Henry Murray, adhered to the movement. Maslow was at the forefront of this group of founders of humanistic psychology in the mid-I960s. BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH Abraham H. Maslow was born on April 1, 1908 in New York City, the first of seven children. Maslow's relationship with his parents, Russian-Jewish immigrants from Kiev, was neither intimate nor loving. He attended New York City public schools. At the age of nine he moved to a non-Jewish neigh­ borhood and, since he looked quite Jewish, discovered anti-semitism there. He described himself during his first twenty years as extremely neurotic, shy, nervous, depressed, lonely, and self-reflecting. He isolated himself at school THOUGHT Vol. 66 No. 260 (March 1991) ABRAHAM H. MASLOW 33 and, since he could not stand being at home, practically lived in the library. At school he was an achiever. Later, upon the advice of his father he enrolled in law school. He lost interest, and never finished the freshman year. At the end of 1928, then twenty years old, he married Bertha, a cousin, whom he had courted for a long time.
    [Show full text]
  • Educational Learning Theories: 2Nd Edition Molly Zhou Dalton State College, [email protected]
    GALILEO, University System of Georgia GALILEO Open Learning Materials Education Open Textbooks Education Spring 2015 Educational Learning Theories: 2nd Edition Molly Zhou Dalton State College, [email protected] David Brown Dalton State College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks Part of the Educational Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Zhou, Molly and Brown, David, "Educational Learning Theories: 2nd Edition" (2015). Education Open Textbooks. 1. https://oer.galileo.usg.edu/education-textbooks/1 This Open Textbook is brought to you for free and open access by the Education at GALILEO Open Learning Materials. It has been accepted for inclusion in Education Open Textbooks by an authorized administrator of GALILEO Open Learning Materials. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Educational Learning Theories Molly Y. Zhou David Brown 2 Educational Learning Theories edited by Molly Y. Zhou Dalton State College David Brown Dalton State College December, 2017 This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike 4.0 International license (CC BY-NC-SA). Cite the book: Zhou, M., & Brown, D. (Eds.). (2017). Educational learning theories. Retrieved from [link] 3 Permission to Use Acknowledgements Grateful acknowledgement is made to the following sources for Permission to Use by Creative Commons licenses or authors or proper copy right holders: Chapter 1 Behaviorism New World Encyclopedia. (2016, May 26). Behaviorism. Retrieved from http://web.newworldencyclopedia.org/entry/Behaviorism Standridge, M. (2002). Behaviorism. In M. Orey (Ed.), Emerging perspectives on learning, teaching, and technology. Retrieve from http://epltt.coe.uga.edu/index.php?title=Behaviorism Chapter 2 Stages of Cognitive Development Wood, K.
    [Show full text]
  • Olivier Walusinski François Boller Victor W. Henderson
    Bogousslavsky J, Boller F, Iwata M (eds): A History of Neuropsychology. Front Neurol Neurosci. Basel, Karger, 2019, vol 44, pp 192–229 (DOI: 10.1159/000494964) Shining a Light on Some of the Most Famous 19th and 20th Century’s Neuropsychologists a b c Olivier Walusinski François Boller Victor W. Henderson a b Private Practice, Brou, France; Department of Neurology, George Washington University Medical School, c Washington, DC, USA; Departments of Health Research and Policy (Epidemiology) and of Neurology and Neurological Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA Abstract This chapter pays homage to the masters who made neuropsychology an esteemed and legitimate field in the 19th and 20th centuries. Here we offer a brief biography for each of them and an analysis of their discoveries: Théophile Alajouanine (1890–1980), Henry Charlton Bastian (1837–1915), Arthur L. Benton (1909–2006), Julian de Ajuriaguerra (1911–1993), Ennio De Renzi (1924–2016), Norman Geschwind (1926–1984), Kurt Goldstein (1878–1965), Henry Head (1861–1940), Henry Hécaen (1912–1983), Pierre Janet (1859–1947), François Lhermitte (1921–1998), Jean Lhermitte (1877–1959), Hugo Karl Liepmann (1863–1925), Heinrich Lissauer (1861–1891), Alexander Romanovich Luria (1902–1977), Brenda Milner (1918–), Théodule Ribot (1839–1916), Charles Richet (1850–1935), Paul Sollier (1861–1933), and Carl Wernicke (1848–1905). © 2019 S. Karger AG, Basel Introduction Neuropsychology, a field at the crossroads between neurology and psychology, is rooted in Egyptian and Greek Antiquity (Imhotep and Eratistrate, respectively). By pondering intelligence, Descartes and Spinoza, the most famous philosophers of the 17th century, conceived a way of thinking about memory and emotion, the foundations of the concepts that contemporary neuroscience endeavors to explore with precision.
    [Show full text]
  • Pediatric Neuropsychological Intervention: a Critical Review Of
    This page intentionally left blank Pediatric Neuropsychological Intervention Pediatric neuropsychology is the practice of understanding and elucidating brainÀbehavior relationships as applied to children and adolescents. This volume examines current trends in the assessment and treatment of common disorders including traumatic brain injuries, brain tumors, epilepsy, and autistic spectrum disorders. Its primary aim is to help practitioners and researchers identify and understand the evidence to support interventions with a range of acquired or congenital neuropsychological disorders. The text is divided into three sections. Section one provides a foundation by considering general issues relevant to assessment and treatment in pediatric neuropsychology. Section two guides practitioners in their approach to the use of interventions in a range of conditions and disorders, and the book closes with a section focusing on medical and experimental initiatives with an emphasis on interdisciplinary issues. This book is essential reading for pediatric neuropsychologists, child clinical psychologists and school psychologists, as well as pediatric neurologists and psychiatrists. Scott J. Hunter is the Director of Pediatric Neuropsychology and an Assistant Professor of Psychiatry and Pediatrics at the University of Chicago. He is also the coordinator for Child Psychology Training in the Department. Dr. Hunter received his Ph.D. in Clinical and Developmental Psychology, with concentrations in Developmental Neuroscience and Developmental Disabilities, from the University of Illinois at Chicago in 1996. He completed a postdoctoral residency in Pediatric Neuropsychology and Developmental Disabilities in the Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics at the University of Rochester. Dr. Hunter’s primary research interests include the diagnosis and management of neurodevelopmental disorders and their behavioral and cognitive sequelae; and the efficacy of pharmacological and surgical interventions for epilepsy, neurofibromatosis, and hydrocephalus.
    [Show full text]
  • Hans -Werner Gessmann Humanistic Psychology and Humanistic Psychodrama
    Hans -Werner Gessmann Humanistic Psychology and Humanistic Psychodrama summary My aim is to show in this paper why the Humanistic Psychodrama aware called humanistic. It is the philosophy of existence suggests that explores the individual existence , and fits in with the school of humanistic psychology . After outlining the historical development of these psychological main direction I will reference the most important philosophers of existence with its central idea and make known thereafter with the psychologist , who represent a humanistic approach . In this I refer to the standard work for Humanistic Psychology by Helmut Quitmann , which for more than three decades with the common history of ideas and philosophical backgrounds and the representatives ( the protagonist ) has of Humanistic Psychology . Quitmann it comes to taking stock of the resultant in the U.S. Humanistic Psychology . I remain aware of close to Quitmanns text that has now become a classic for students and practitioners , as I follow the logical structure and the structure of quotations , paraphrases and supplements in my remarks . The disclosure of quotations of readability, is omitted due to the close proximity to Quitmanns book. It should be especially on the pages 64 - 250 referenced the 3rd revised and expanded edition ( Quitmann , H.: Humanistic Psychology Psychology, Philosophy, Organizational Development 3rd Edition, Hogrefe -Verlag 1996. ) . Quitmanns book made at the beginning of the 80s the basis for a lecture on Psychotherapeutic Institute Bergerhausen in Duisburg at the opening of a new training center , which now finds its transcript by the integration of the Humanist psychodrama is emphasized in Humanistic Psychology . At the end of the article I put key statements of Humanistic Psychology Humanistic Psychodrama in the relationship.
    [Show full text]
  • Kurt Goldstein and the Neurology of Movement During the Interwar Years
    Kurt Goldstein and the Neurology of Movement during the Interwar Years Physiological Experimentation, Clinical Psychology and Early Rehabilitation1 Frank W. Stahnisch / Thomas Hoffmann […] a pseudo-social organization may be compared with an organ- ism in disease, and we may speak of such a society [in which the nor- mal relations are no longer sustained] as sick. Normal society means a type of organization through which the fullest possible actualiza- tion on the part of all individuals is assured. […] If we acknowledge and utilize social organization as an instrument by means of which all individuals may actualize themselves to an optimal degree, then a genuine social life becomes possible. Only under these conditions is a social organization capable of doing justice to every individual; only this makes it a real organization and secures its duration. […] Only in this way can we discover the concrete causes of failure in a given situation and the appropriate ways to correct the failure.2 1 Both authors are grateful for the editorial assistance of Mrs. Beth Cusitar who proofread and adjusted the English language of the paper manuscript. Frank W. Stahnisch further wishes to thank the Hotchkiss Brain Institute and the Calgary Institute for Population and Public Health for their support of this research project. 2 The citation is taken from Kurt Goldstein’s Harvard lecture series, published in the middle of WWII as: Kurt Goldstein: Human Nature in the Light of Psychopathology, Cambridge, MA 1940, p. 222 f. 283 1. Introduction The initial citation is taken from the “William James Lectures”, which German émigré-neurologist Kurt Goldstein (1878-1965) gave at Harvard University be- tween 1937 and 1938, after having been invited by its Department of Education.
    [Show full text]
  • Kurt Goldstein's Neurolinguistic Foundation of a Holistic Philosophy
    Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. 48(1), 40–54 Winter 2012 View this article online at Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/jhbs.21517 C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. LANGUAGE AND HUMAN NATURE: KURT GOLDSTEIN’S NEUROLINGUISTIC FOUNDATION OF A HOLISTIC PHILOSOPHY DAVID LUDWIG Holism in interwar Germany provides an excellent example for social and political in- fluences on scientific developments. Deeply impressed by the ubiquitous invocation of a cultural crisis, biologists, physicians, and psychologists presented holistic accounts as an alternative to the “mechanistic worldview” of the nineteenth century. Although the ideo- logical background of these accounts is often blatantly obvious, many holistic scientists did not content themselves with a general opposition to a mechanistic worldview but aimed at a rational foundation of their holistic projects. This article will discuss the work of Kurt Goldstein, who is known for both his groundbreaking contributions to neuropsychology and his holistic philosophy of human nature. By focusing on Goldstein’s neurolinguistic research, I want to reconstruct the empirical foundations of his holistic program without ig- noring its cultural background. In this sense, Goldstein’s work provides a case study for the formation of a scientific theory through the complex interplay between specific empirical evidences and the general cultural developments of the Weimar Republic. C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Holism in interwar Germany is often perceived as an irrationalist and antimodernist movement (Cooper, 1996, p. 102f; Popper, 1971, p. 63; Royce & Mos, 1981, p. 13). Dis- orientated and troubled by the explanatory success of nineteenth-century science, the rapid industrialization of the German society, and a lost war of unknown proportions, Weimar era scholars turned to the ideal of wholeness as an alternative foundation of science and culture in general.
    [Show full text]