Charles Manning Child (1869-1954) [1]
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Charles Manning Child
NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES C HARLES MANNING C HILD 1869—1954 A Biographical Memoir by L Ibb I E H. HYMAN Any opinions expressed in this memoir are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Academy of Sciences. Biographical Memoir COPYRIGHT 1957 NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES WASHINGTON D.C. CHARLES MANNING CHILD 1869-1954 BY LIBBIE H. HYMAN •HARE MANNING CHILD was descended on both sides from sound \^J New England stock. His mother, Mary Elizabeth Manning, belonged to the famous Manning family of New England founded by William Manning, who about 1634 migrated from England to Massachusetts, eventually settling at Cambridge where he left a large number of descendants. Mary Elizabeth's father, William Manning, an eighth-generation descendant of the original William, was a physician of a roving tendency, who practiced during the later years of his life in various towns in Michigan. Mary Elizabeth seems to have had some leaning toward biology, for she attended Mrs. Louis Agassiz's school and Louis Agassiz himself selected a little microscope for her. The Mannings had a manor house in Cambridge, and one of Child's earliest recollections is of being lost in the garden of his great-grandmother's house in Cambridge. The house is part of the Harvard campus now and Coolidge dormitory is built around three sides of it; it was Burgoyne's head- quarters during the Revolutionary War. Mary Elizabeth married Charles Chauncey Child of Higganum, Connecticut, where the Child family had lived for several genera- tions. The family was founded there by James Child, who in 1762 came from Rhode Island and established a shipbuilding business at Higganum. -
Charles Manning Child
(Botany) H. H. Biswell, University of California, Berkeley; Jacob Verduin, Put-in-Bay, Ohio; Phillippe Bourdeau, North Carolina State College; (Zoology) Fred Test, University of Michigan; Clifford Berg, Cornell University; Henry S. Fitch, University of Kansas; Review Editor of Ecology: R. H. Whittaker, Brooklyn College; Representative to the National Research Council: H. C. Hanson, The Catholic University of America; Member of the Publications Committee: George L. Clarke, Harvard University. J. M. Aikman Reed W. Fautin John E. Potzger, Chairman Adjournment followed the approval of this report. East Lansing, Michigan September 7, 1955 John F. Reed Secretary CHARLES MANNING CHILD it seems fitting for me to express my indebtedness to my former teacher and colleague who passed away December 20, 1954 at Palo Alto, California, having reached almost 85 years of age. I was associated with him first as a student, beginning in 1902, as his teaching assistant 1904-1906, teaching some of his courses in his absence; then as a colleague in the Department of Zoology of the University of Chicago from 1907 to 1914. Professor Child was born in Ypsilanti, Michigan. He took his under graduate work, including economics under Woodrow Wilson, and master's degree at Wesleyan University, Middletown, Connecticut. His parents hav ing passed away about the time of the completion of his training in the Connecticut institution, he took his heritage and decided to go to Germany and take a doctor's degree, which was the usual way of completing one's training at that time. He gave me a copy of his thesis, which I have saved to this time. -
American Museum of Natural History
^Auseumo/^A, <^' \ 1869 THE LIBRARY » THE AMERICAN MUSEUM OF NATURAL HISTORY ;Vol. XXV January I968 FROM THE DIRECTOR'S DESK January is traditionally a time of review and forecast, a time of taking inventory, evaluating the state of an individual, an organization, a nation, the world - and in these days, even the worlds beyond. The timing of such stocktaking is artificial, related solely to a date on a calendar, but it has become a custom and one that has some merit. In a busy complex such as the Museum, there is scarcely even enough time for thoughtful contemplation of the past, or for planning of the future - and yet, both are necessary for progress. What are our most important assets? What has made the Museum great, what makes it more and more significant to science and education - to Society? We have in our collections some 15 million specimens and artifacts, including many that are rare and irreplaceable. In our exhibits we have some of the v;orld's most spec- itacular natural history displays, and many more are under design for the near future. We have one of the great natural history libraries of the world. Surely these all make the Museum unique and obviously without the collections and the exhibits there would be no museum. But, it should be emphasized that the most important resources of the Museum have always been the people who have contributed their scholarship, their imagination and creativity, their energies, their service of adventure, their time, their financial support and enthusiastic interest to the enterprise that is the Museum. -
104 Loeb and Northrop’S Experimental Manipulation of Longevity Was Deeply Related to the Discourse on Immortality at That Time
Refiguring Old Age: Shaping Scientific Research on Senescence, 1900-1960 A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY Hyung Wook Park Program in History of Science, Technology, and Medicine University of Minnesota IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Sally Gregory Kohlstedt and John M. Eyler July, 2009 © Hyung Wook Park, 2009 Acknowledgements In completing this project, I am indebted to many people. First of all, I am deeply grateful for my advisors, Professors Sally Gregory Kohlstedt and John M. Eyler. Their significance in my academic growth cannot be overstated. They have been great teachers and thoughtful mentors who showed me what is to become a historian of science and medicine. I also appreciate my dissertation committee chair and members, Professors Mark Borrello, Susan Jones, and Jennifer Gunn. Besides reading and commenting on my long dissertation, they have always been helpful whenever I sought their advice concerning my publications and presentations. I thank other scholars in our program, especially Professors Alan Shapiro, Robert Seidel, and Thomas Misa. Their classes, which I attended as a student or a teaching assistant, stimulated the growth of my scholarship. In addition, I should not omit my gratitude toward my fellow graduate student colleagues, including Sara Cammersi, Amy Fisher, Suzanne Fischer, Gina Rumore, Barbara Reiterer, Adrian Fischer, Nathan Crowe, Maggy Hofius, Don Opitz, and Nick Martin. They have been my encouraging and sincere friends. There are people outside of the University of Minnesota to whom I should express my thanks. I appreciate the archivists and librarians whom I met during my research travels from 2005 to 2007, including Willam Wallach at the University of Michigan, Clare Porter at the Nuffield Foundation, Nicholas Scheetz at Georgetown University, David Corson at Cornell University, Paul Anderson and Martha Riley at Washington University, and others whose name I do not remember. -
When Psychologists Were Naturalists: Questionnaires and Collecting Practices in Early American Psychology, 1880-1932
WHEN PSYCHOLOGISTS WERE NATURALISTS: QUESTIONNAIRES AND COLLECTING PRACTICES IN EARLY AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGY, 1880-1932 JACY L. YOUNG A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY GRADUATE PROGRAM IN PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO December 2014 ©Jacy L. Young, 2014 ii Abstract This dissertation reshapes our understanding of the earliest years of American psychology by documenting the discipline’s methodological plurality from its very inception. In tracing the use of questionnaires over the first half century of the discipline’s existence as a science, I argue that a natural historical orientation, wherein collection, analysis, and categorization are central to the scientific enterprise, has been a persistent facet of the field. Manifested in a recurrent interest in collecting information on mental life, this natural historical perspective facilitated a moral economy of data, wherein the discipline’s affect-laden norms and values sanctified the objects and practices of mass data collection. This in turn lent itself to the adoption of statistical analyses as a central component of psychological science. Although, at first glance, falling outside of the bounds of the mechanically objective practices that characterized the new psychology’s laboratory endeavours, with their use of standardized instrumentation, projects with this orientation adhered to this form of objectivity in their own way. Seeking precise accounts of mental life, including information on its physical correlates, these enterprises engaged the public in collection practices in the field. Taking up subjects with widespread interest outside of purely scientific spheres – including child study, psychical matters, and dreaming – questionnaire projects had broad appeal.