Speculative Hunting by an Araneophagic Salticid Spider

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Speculative Hunting by an Araneophagic Salticid Spider SPECULATIVE HUNTINGBY ANARANEOPHAGICSAL TICID SPIDER by ROBERT J.CLARK , DUANE P.HARLAND and ROBERT R.JACKSON 1,2) (Departmentof Zoology,University of Canterbury, Private Bag 4800, Christchurch, New Zealand) (Acc.3-VII-2000) Summary Portia mbriata ,anaraneophagic jumping spider ( Salticidae),makes undirected leaps ( er- raticleaping with no particulartarget being evident) in the presence of chemicalcues from Jacksonoidesqueenslandicus ,anothersalticid and a commonprey of P. mbriata. Whether undirectedleaping by P. mbriata functionsas hunting by speculation is investigatedexperi- mentally.Our rsthypothesis, that undirected leaps provoke movement by J.queenslandicus , wasinvestigated using living P. mbriata andthree types of luresmade from dead, dry arthro- pods (P. mbriata, J.queenslandicus and Muscadomestica ).When a living P. mbriata made undirectedleaps or aspring-drivendevice made the lures suddenly move up and down, sim- ulatingundirected leaping, J.queenslandicus respondedby wavingits palps and starting to walk.There was no statisticalevidence that the species from which the lure was made in u- enced J.queenslandicus ’responsein these tests. Our second hypothesis, that J.queenslandi- cus revealsits location to P. mbriata bymoving, was investigated by recording P. mbriata’s reaction to J.queenslandicus when J.queenslandicus reactedto luressimulating undirected leaping.In these tests, P. mbriata respondedby turning toward J.queenslandicus and waving its palps. Keywords: Portia mbriata , Jacksonoidesqueenslandicus ,jumpingspiders, predation, spec- ulativehunting. 1) Correspondingauthor; e-mail address: [email protected] 2) WethankPhil T aylorand David Blest for useful discussion and valuable comments on the manuscript.Financial support was provided by theNational Science Foundation ( GrantBNS 861078)and the Marsden Fund of New Zealand(Grant UOC512). c KoninklijkeBrill NV ,Leiden,2000 Behaviour137, 1601-1612 ® 1602 CLARK, HARLAND&JACKSON Introduction Ageneralproblem facing predators is howto locate prey(Curio, 1976). Whenvision is relied on,being out of the predator’sline ofsight orbeing camouaged will interfere with detection (Edmunds,1974). ‘ Huntingby speculation’(directing attacks at refugeswhere prey tend to befound or probingareas in whichprey normally hide) is apotential solution (Curio, 1976).Envisaged not as prey-capturebehaviour, but instead as atactic forlocating prey,hunting by speculation might functionfor a predator byprovoking a responsethat reveals the prey’slocation. W oodstorks,for example,may probe submerged vegetation even when no prey is visible (Kahl& Peacock,1963), Octopuscyanea Grayattacks holes in coral even in the absenceof prey(Y arnell, 1969)and lions mayrun to the topof hills, apparentlyin anticipation ofstartling unwaryprey on the otherside (Schaller, 1972).However, examples fromarthropods, and experimental studies onany predator,have been scarce. Inthe present paper,we investigate huntingby speculation in Portia m- briata (Doleschall) fromQueensland, Australia, anaraneophagic jumping spider (Salticidae) that preysespecially oftenon other salticids (Jackson& Blest, 1982). Jacksonoidesqueenslandicus Wanless (Salticidae) is especially abundantin the same habitat as P. mbriata (Jackson,1988 )andis probably the salticid species onwhich P. mbriata most oftenpreys. Preliminary stud- ies showthat chemical cues from J.queenslandicus ,evenin the absence of J.queenslandicus , prepare P. mbriata forpredation by stimulating the adoptionof a special palpposture (retracted palps) characteristic ofstalk- ingsequences against salticids as preyand by heightening P. mbriata’s at- tention to visual cues from J.queenslandicus .Preliminary studies indicate that chemical cues from J.queenslandicus also elicit intermittent undirected leaping(erratic leapingwith noparticular target beingevident) by P. mbri- ata.Theabsence of anapparent target suggests that this behaviourfunctions as speculative hunting.T wohypotheses are consideredhere: (1)undirected leaps by P. mbriata stimulate J.queenslandicus to move;(2 )bymoving, J.queenslandicus gives awayits location to P. mbriata. Methods General Standardmaintenance procedures in a controlled-environmentlaboratory (light-dark cycle, 12L:12D;lights on at0800 h) wereadopted, as detailedelsewhere (Jackson & Hallas,1986). SPECULATIVE HUNTINGBYSPIDER 1603 Adultfemales of P. mbriata and J.queenslandicus (bodylength: P. mbriata 10-11 mm; J.queenslandicus 5-6mm), from laboratory cultures, were used. Experiment1: J. queenslandicusviewing P .mbriata Thesetests were used to get baseline information on how J.queenslandicus reactedwhen P. mbriata madeundirected leaps. For a testchamber, we used a transparentperspex box (length ´ width ´ height: 211 ´ 144 ´ 44mm). Three regions of the box were de ned: region1 extended50 mm outfrom one end of thebox; region 2 extended50 mm outfrom theopposite end; region 3 wasthe space between the other two regions. There was a 5-mm widehole ( keptplugged with a cork)in the bottom of thebox in the centre of region 1 and anotherin the centre of region 2. Testspider 1 wasput in thebox 24-28 h beforetesting started and had free access to the threeregions during this pre-test interval. T estingbegan between 0800 and 1000 h. When test spider1 waswithin 50 mm ofoneend of the box, a partition(partition 1) wasput into place, therebyclosing this region ( region1) offfrom the rest of the box. At thesame time, another partition(partition 2) wasput into place, closing off region 2. Testspider 2 wasintroduced 10minlater into region 2 throughthe hole in the bottom of the cage. For transfer, rsttest spider2 wasenticed into a plastictube ( diameter5 mm),then the tube was positioned with oneend against the hole in thetest chamber. When gently prodded by inserting a softbrush throughthe other end of thetube, test spider 2 walkedslowly out into the test chamber. Testingbegan only if J.queenslandicus hadremained quiescent for the previous 5 sfacing region2. With J.queenslandicus quiescent,partition 2 followedby partition1 wasremoved. Thebehaviour of thetwo spiders was observed for the next 10 min. Control tests were the sameas experimental tests except that partition 2 remainedin place ( i.e.J. queenslandicus couldnot see P. mbriata duringthese tests). Beinginterested speci cally in how J.queenslandicus reactedto undirected leaping by P. mbriata,weconsider only those experimental and control tests in which: (1) P. mbriata madea singleundirected leap during the 10-min test interval and (2) J.queenslandicus remainedquiescent for the entire period prior to P. mbriata makingits undirected leap. The testwas aborted if: ( 1) P. mbriata movedduring an experimental test into region 3 before makingan undirectedleap or (2) P. mbriata madea secondundirected leap before the 10- mintest interval elapsed. No individual J.queenslandicus or P. mbriata wasused in more thanone successful test. Different individuals were used in experimentaland control tests. Experiment2: J. queenslandicusviewing a lure Ourobjective was to test simultaneously how J.queenslandicus reactedto a moving lure( asimulationof undirected leaps by P. mbriata) and how P. mbriata reacted to J.queenslandicus ’reactionto thelure. The test chamber ( Fig.1) wasa rectangularperspex box (length ´ width ´ height: 147 ´ 51 ´ 51mm) withtwo tubes. Its design permitted viewingof a lureby J.queenslandicus andviewing of J.queenslandicus by P. mbriata. Tube1 (internaldiameter 13 mm), made of transparent glass, tinsidethe box at one end.Except when introducing J.queenslandicus ,thehole opening to the outside was kept stoppered.Initially, a holeat the opposite end of thetube was blocked by anopaquemetal screen( partition1). Partition 1, which tintoa slitin the box, could be moved from side toside( indicatedby dotted lines in Fig.1). Tube 2 (internaldiameter 13 mm), situated on 1604 CLARK, HARLAND&JACKSON Fig.1. Apparatus used in experiment 2. Jacksonoidesqueenslandicus (inTube 1), but not Portia mbriata (inTube 2), can view the lure. P. mbriata canview the reaction of J.queenslandicus tothelure. T opand bottom of the box are opaque perspex and the sides transparentperspex. Tubes are transparent glass. Before testing begins, the opening of each tubeinto the box is blockedby aslidingpartition, as indicated by dottedlines. During testing bothtubes are unblocked by aligning the hole in thepartition with the tube opening. The lure ison top of a woodenrod connected to anelectric motor ‘ leapgenerator’ ( notshown). thetop of thechamber, housed P. mbriata.Thedistal opening of tube 2 waskept stoppered exceptwhen introducing P. mbriata.Theproximal opening of tube 2 openedinto a wider tube( internaldiameter 20 mm) whichin turn opened into the box. An opaque metal screen (partition2) coveredthe opening between the narrow and wide tube. Partition 2 tintoa slit inthewider tube, and it couldbe movedfrom side to side( indicatedby dotted lines in Fig. 1). Thewide tube was positioned so that its centre was directly above the opening of tube 1 intothe interior of thebox. The top of thebox was opaque. This meant that the only part of thebox’ sinteriorvisible to P. mbriata duringa testwas around the opening of tube 1. At thefar end of the box, positioned in front of tube 1, there was a holein the bottom ofthe box through which a lurecould move. Each lure was made by positioning a dead J.queenslandicus , P. mbriata or house y (Muscadomestica L.)in a lifelikeposture on a corkdisk (diameter 17 mm; height22
Recommended publications
  • Molecular Phylogeny, Divergence Times and Biogeography of Spiders of the Subfamily Euophryinae (Araneae: Salticidae) ⇑ Jun-Xia Zhang A, , Wayne P
    Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 68 (2013) 81–92 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Molec ular Phylo genetics and Evolution journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ympev Molecular phylogeny, divergence times and biogeography of spiders of the subfamily Euophryinae (Araneae: Salticidae) ⇑ Jun-Xia Zhang a, , Wayne P. Maddison a,b a Department of Zoology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4 b Department of Botany and Beaty Biodiversity Museum, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4 article info abstract Article history: We investigate phylogenetic relationships of the jumping spider subfamily Euophryinae, diverse in spe- Received 10 August 2012 cies and genera in both the Old World and New World. DNA sequence data of four gene regions (nuclear: Revised 17 February 2013 28S, Actin 5C; mitochondrial: 16S-ND1, COI) were collected from 263 jumping spider species. The molec- Accepted 13 March 2013 ular phylogeny obtained by Bayesian, likelihood and parsimony methods strongly supports the mono- Available online 28 March 2013 phyly of a Euophryinae re-delimited to include 85 genera. Diolenius and its relatives are shown to be euophryines. Euophryines from different continental regions generally form separate clades on the phy- Keywords: logeny, with few cases of mixture. Known fossils of jumping spiders were used to calibrate a divergence Phylogeny time analysis, which suggests most divergences of euophryines were after the Eocene. Given the diver- Temporal divergence Biogeography gence times, several intercontinental dispersal event sare required to explain the distribution of euophry- Intercontinental dispersal ines. Early transitions of continental distribution between the Old and New World may have been Euophryinae facilitated by the Antarctic land bridge, which euophryines may have been uniquely able to exploit Diolenius because of their apparent cold tolerance.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Selection Research on Spiders: Progress and Biases
    Biol. Rev. (2005), 80, pp. 363–385. f Cambridge Philosophical Society 363 doi:10.1017/S1464793104006700 Printed in the United Kingdom Sexual selection research on spiders: progress and biases Bernhard A. Huber* Zoological Research Institute and Museum Alexander Koenig, Adenauerallee 160, 53113 Bonn, Germany (Received 7 June 2004; revised 25 November 2004; accepted 29 November 2004) ABSTRACT The renaissance of interest in sexual selection during the last decades has fuelled an extraordinary increase of scientific papers on the subject in spiders. Research has focused both on the process of sexual selection itself, for example on the signals and various modalities involved, and on the patterns, that is the outcome of mate choice and competition depending on certain parameters. Sexual selection has most clearly been demonstrated in cases involving visual and acoustical signals but most spiders are myopic and mute, relying rather on vibrations, chemical and tactile stimuli. This review argues that research has been biased towards modalities that are relatively easily accessible to the human observer. Circumstantial and comparative evidence indicates that sexual selection working via substrate-borne vibrations and tactile as well as chemical stimuli may be common and widespread in spiders. Pattern-oriented research has focused on several phenomena for which spiders offer excellent model objects, like sexual size dimorphism, nuptial feeding, sexual cannibalism, and sperm competition. The accumulating evidence argues for a highly complex set of explanations for seemingly uniform patterns like size dimorphism and sexual cannibalism. Sexual selection appears involved as well as natural selection and mechanisms that are adaptive in other contexts only. Sperm competition has resulted in a plethora of morpho- logical and behavioural adaptations, and simplistic models like those linking reproductive morphology with behaviour and sperm priority patterns in a straightforward way are being replaced by complex models involving an array of parameters.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perception in Jumping Spiders (Araneae,Salticidae)
    Visual Perception in Jumping Spiders (Araneae,Salticidae) A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Biology at the University of Canterbury by Yinnon Dolev University of Canterbury 2016 Table of Contents Abstract.............................................................................................................................................................................. i Acknowledgments .......................................................................................................................................................... iii Preface ............................................................................................................................................................................. vi Chapter 1: Introduction ................................................................................................................................................... 1 Chapter 2: Innate pattern recognition and categorisation in a jumping Spider ........................................................... 9 Abstract ....................................................................................................................................................................... 10 Introduction ................................................................................................................................................................ 11 Methods .....................................................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Predatory Behavior of Jumping Spiders
    Annual Reviews www.annualreviews.org/aronline Annu Rev. Entomol. 19%. 41:287-308 Copyrighl8 1996 by Annual Reviews Inc. All rights reserved PREDATORY BEHAVIOR OF JUMPING SPIDERS R. R. Jackson and S. D. Pollard Department of Zoology, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand KEY WORDS: salticids, salticid eyes, Portia, predatory versatility, aggressive mimicry ABSTRACT Salticids, the largest family of spiders, have unique eyes, acute vision, and elaborate vision-mediated predatory behavior, which is more pronounced than in any other spider group. Diverse predatory strategies have evolved, including araneophagy,aggressive mimicry, myrmicophagy ,and prey-specific preycatch- ing behavior. Salticids are also distinctive for development of behavioral flexi- bility, including conditional predatory strategies, the use of trial-and-error to solve predatory problems, and the undertaking of detours to reach prey. Predatory behavior of araneophagic salticids has undergone local adaptation to local prey, and there is evidence of predator-prey coevolution. Trade-offs between mating and predatory strategies appear to be important in ant-mimicking and araneo- phagic species. INTRODUCTION With over 4000 described species (1 l), jumping spiders (Salticidae) compose by Fordham University on 04/13/13. For personal use only. the largest family of spiders. They are characterized as cursorial, diurnal predators with excellent eyesight. Although spider eyes usually lack the struc- tural complexity required for acute vision, salticids have unique, complex eyes with resolution abilities without known parallels in animals of comparable size Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1996.41:287-308. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org (98). Salticids are the end-product of an evolutionary process in which a small silk-producing animal with a simple nervous system acquires acute vision, resulting in a diverse array of complex predatory strategies.
    [Show full text]
  • Euryattus Thorell, 1881
    Euryattus Thorell, 1881 Taxonomy Euryattus distribution is centred on Papua New Guinea, extending to Indonesia, the Pacific Islands and Australia which has three species. Euryattus bleekeri is widespread across mainland Examples of live Euryattus Illustrator (and ©) I.R. Macaulay, G.J. Australia, E. wallacei is found in central to northern Queensland and E. ventralis has been Anderson (BM, BL) reported in far north Queensland by Whyte and Anderson (2017). Other species are likely to occur in northern Queensland. Description Euryattus spp. are large spiders, body length ranging from 9 to 12 mm. The head, viewed from above, is rounded, widest at or just behind the posterior lateral eyes and with characteristically strongly-marked white or cream sides. The carapace is high. The abdomen is large and elongate-ovate in shape. Chelicerae are geniculate with a single, bicuspid (fissident) retromarginal tooth and three or more teeth on the promargin. The first pair of legs is longer than the other legs but not massive in build. Aspects of the general morphology of Euryattus Illustrators (and ©) B.J. Richardson (CSIRO), The male embolus arises on the distal edge of the tegulum where it forms an anti-clockwise half M. Zabka (diag.) (QMB) circle. It is divided into two, closely-aligned branches along much of its length. The tegulum is rounded without a proximal lobe. The palpal tibia has a single, blunt retro-lateral tibial apophysis, with a curved or scooped end. The females have a single epigynal atrium, partially divided posteriorly. The spermathecae are spherical, located within the posterior sections of the atrium.
    [Show full text]
  • Portia Perceptions: the Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping Spider
    Portia Perceptions: The Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping 1 Spider Duane P. Harland and Robert R. Jackson The Personality of Portia Spiders are traditionally portrayed as simple, instinct-driven animals (Savory, 1928; Drees, 1952; Bristowe, 1958). Small brain size is perhaps the most compelling reason for expecting so little flexibility from our eight-legged neighbors. Fitting comfortably on the head of a pin, a spider brain seems to vanish into insignificance. Common sense tells us that compared with large-brained mammals, spiders have so little to work with that they must be restricted to a circumscribed set of rigid behaviors, flexibility being a luxury afforded only to those with much larger central nervous systems. In this chapter we review recent findings on an unusual group of spiders that seem to be arachnid enigmas. In a number of ways the behavior of the araneophagic jumping spiders is more comparable to that of birds and mammals than conventional wisdom would lead us to expect of an arthropod. The term araneophagic refers to these spiders’ preference for other spiders as prey, and jumping spider is the common English name for members of the family Saltici- dae. Although both their common and the scientific Latin names acknowledge their jumping behavior, it is really their unique, complex eyes that set this family of spiders apart from all others. Among spiders (many of which have very poor vision), salticids have eyes that are by far the most specialized for resolving fine spatial detail. We focus here on the most extensively studied genus, Portia. Before we discuss the interrelationship between the salticids’ uniquely acute vision, their predatory strategies, and their apparent cognitive abilities, we need to offer some sense of what kind of animal a jumping spider is; to do this, we attempt to offer some insight into what we might call Portia’s personality.
    [Show full text]
  • Natural Prey of the Jumping Spider Menemerus Semilimbatus (Hahn, 1827) (Araneae: Salticidae), with Notes on Its Unusual Predatory Behaviour
    EUROPEAN ARACHNOLOGY 2003 (LOGUNOV D.V. & PENNEY D. eds.), pp. 93100. © ARTHROPODA SELECTA (Special Issue No.1, 2004). ISSN 0136-006X (Proceedings of the 21st European Colloquium of Arachnology, St.-Petersburg, 49 August 2003) Natural prey of the jumping spider Menemerus semilimbatus (Hahn, 1827) (Araneae: Salticidae), with notes on its unusual predatory behaviour Åñòåñòâåííàÿ äîáû÷à ïàóêà ñêàêóí÷èêà Menemerus semilimbatus (Hahn, 1827) (Araneae: Salticidae) ñ çàìåòêàìè î åãî íåîáû÷íîì õèùíè÷åñêîì ïîâåäåíèè E.F. GUSEINOV Ý.Ô. ÃÓÑÅÉÍΠInstitute of Zoology, Azerbaijan Academy of Sciences, block 504, passage 1128, Baku 370073, Azerbaijan. email: [email protected] Èíñòèòóò çîîëîãèè ÍÀÍ Àçåðáàéäæàíà, êâàðòàë 504, ïðîåçä 1128, Áàêó 370073, Àçåðáàéäæàí. email: [email protected] ABSTRACT. Prey composition and the hunting behaviour of the jumping spider, Menemerus semilimbatus, which inhabits stone walls was studied. Less than 10% of the specimens in the population studied were observed feeding. Adult males fed significantly less frequently than adult females and juveniles. Diptera, the dominant prey group, accounted for more than 70% of all prey consumed. No other single prey type was present in significant numbers. M. semilimbatus adopts a specialized predatory behaviour towards flies that is unusual for salticids. This behaviour depends on how the fly is orientated towards the spider. If the fly is facing away from the spider, M. semilimbatus approaches it directly. When the fly is facing the spider, M. semilimbatus keeps its distance and encircles it until the prey is facing away from the spider. Only then, will the spider start to approach the fly directly. The specific habitat of M.
    [Show full text]
  • SA Spider Checklist
    REVIEW ZOOS' PRINT JOURNAL 22(2): 2551-2597 CHECKLIST OF SPIDERS (ARACHNIDA: ARANEAE) OF SOUTH ASIA INCLUDING THE 2006 UPDATE OF INDIAN SPIDER CHECKLIST Manju Siliwal 1 and Sanjay Molur 2,3 1,2 Wildlife Information & Liaison Development (WILD) Society, 3 Zoo Outreach Organisation (ZOO) 29-1, Bharathi Colony, Peelamedu, Coimbatore, Tamil Nadu 641004, India Email: 1 [email protected]; 3 [email protected] ABSTRACT Thesaurus, (Vol. 1) in 1734 (Smith, 2001). Most of the spiders After one year since publication of the Indian Checklist, this is described during the British period from South Asia were by an attempt to provide a comprehensive checklist of spiders of foreigners based on the specimens deposited in different South Asia with eight countries - Afghanistan, Bangladesh, Bhutan, India, Maldives, Nepal, Pakistan and Sri Lanka. The European Museums. Indian checklist is also updated for 2006. The South Asian While the Indian checklist (Siliwal et al., 2005) is more spider list is also compiled following The World Spider Catalog accurate, the South Asian spider checklist is not critically by Platnick and other peer-reviewed publications since the last scrutinized due to lack of complete literature, but it gives an update. In total, 2299 species of spiders in 67 families have overview of species found in various South Asian countries, been reported from South Asia. There are 39 species included in this regions checklist that are not listed in the World Catalog gives the endemism of species and forms a basis for careful of Spiders. Taxonomic verification is recommended for 51 species. and participatory work by arachnologists in the region.
    [Show full text]
  • 19 3 153 188 Proszynski for Inet.P65
    Arthropoda Selecta 19(3): 153188 © ARTHROPODA SELECTA, 2010 Description of some Salticidae (Araneae) from the Malay Archipelago. I. Salticidae of the Lesser Sunda Islands, with comments on related species Îïèñàíèå íåêîòîðûõ Salticidae (Araneae) èç Ìàëàéñêîãî Àðõèïåëàãà. I. Salticidae Ìàëûõ Çîíäñêèõ îñòðîâîâ ñ êîììåíòàðèÿìè î áëèçêèõ âèäàõ Jerzy Prószyñski*, Christa L. Deeleman-Reinhold** É. Ïðóøèíüñêèé*, Ê. Äèëåìàí-Ðåéíîëüä** * Museum and Institute of Zoology, Polish Academy of Sciences, ul. Wilcza 64, 00-679 Warszawa, Poland. E-mail: jerzy.Prószyñ[email protected] ** 4619GA Ossendrecht, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected] KEY WORDS: Salticidae, new species, diagnostic characters, geographical distribution, Indonesia, Bali, Flores, Lombok, Sumba, Sumbawa. ÊËÞ×ÅÂÛÅ ÑËÎÂÀ: íîâûé âèä, äèàãíîñòè÷åñêèå ïðèçíàêè, ðàñïðîñòðàíåíèå, Èíäîíåçèÿ, î. Áàëè, î. Ôëîðåñ, î. Ëîìáîê, î. Ñóìáà, î. Ñóìáàâà. In memoriam Bohdan Pisarski, friend of J. Prószyñski and com- panion in the Java and Bali collecting trip in 1959, for many years the Director of the Institute of Zoology PAN. ABSTRACT. This paper provides preliminary ref- Myrmarachne MacLeay, 1839 is discussed. Comple- erence diagnostic drawings for selected Oriental gen- mentary diagnostic drawings are added for the fol- era and species, to complement the existing scanty lowing species: Artabrus erythrocephalus (C.L. Koch, literature. The following new taxa are described: new 1846), Harmochirus brachiatus (Thorell, 1877), genus Katya gen.n., new species: Burmattus Hasarius adansoni (Audouin, 1826), Myrmarachne pachytibialis sp.n., Carrhotus sundaicus sp.n., Chrysilla hirsutipalpi [?] Edmunds & Prószyñski, 2003, Spar- deelemani sp.n., Cosmophasis valerieae sp.n., Cytaea taeus spinimanus (Thorell, 1878), Thiania bhamoen- whytei sp.n., Euryattus [?] junxiae sp.n., Katya flore- sis Thorell, 1887.
    [Show full text]
  • Clark Thesis.Pdf (8.256Mb)
    THE ROLE OF CHEMICAL CUES , IN THE PREDATORY AND ANTI-PREDATORY BEHAVIOUR OF JU~PING SPIDERS (ARANEAE, SAL TICIDAE) A Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in, Zoology at the University of Canterbury by Robert John Clark 2000 CONTENTS Abstract 1· Chapter 1: Introduction 3 Chapter 2: Theoretical background 9 Chapter 3: Chemical cues elicit prey capture in P. fimbriata 56 Chapter 4: Web use during predatory encounters between P. fimbriata, an araneophagic 91 jumping spider, and its preferred prey, other jumping spiders Chapter 5: Speculative hunting by an araneophagic jumping spider 108 Chapter 6: Chemical cues from ants influence predatory behaviour in Habrocestum pulex 125 (Hentz), an ant eating jumping spider (Araneae, Salticidae) Chapter 7: Reactions of Habrocestum pulex, a myrmecophagic salticid, to potential 147 kairomones from ants Chapter 8: Dragllnes and assessment of fighting ability in cannibalistic jumping spiders 160 Chapter 9: Relationship between violent aggression in saltlcids and use of pheromones to 178 obtain information on conspeclfics Chapter10: Discussion 189 Acknowledgements 198 References 199 2 9 MAR 2000 1 ABSTRACT The role of chemical cues in prey-capture behaviour is studied in jumping spiders (Salticldae). Prior to this study, little attention has been given to how chemical cues influence the predatory behaviour of these spiders with complex eyes and visual acuity unrivalled In any other animals of comparable size. Three categories of predation are considered: salticids preying on conspecifics (cannibalism), salticids preying on non-conspecific spiders (araneophagy) and salticids preying on ants (myrmecophagy). Primary study animals are Portia spp.
    [Show full text]
  • Jumping Spiders in Space: Movement Patterns, Nest Site fidelity and the Use of Beacons
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2006, 71, 109–116 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.033 Jumping spiders in space: movement patterns, nest site fidelity and the use of beacons CHAD D. HOEFLER* &ELIZABETHM.JAKOB† *Organismic and Evolutionary Biology and Department of Plant, Soil, and Insect Sciences, University of Massachusetts, Amherst yDepartment of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst (Received 30 April 2004; initial acceptance 19 July 2004; final acceptance 24 March 2005; published online 15 December 2005; MS. number: A9869) We studied movement patterns and the use of navigational beacons in the jumping spider Phidippus clarus (Salticidae). Phidippus clarus does not build prey-capture webs, but does construct thick silken nests used as nighttime retreats. We measured the degree of nest site fidelity with individually marked spiders and a grid of artificial nest tubes. Females in August were most site-faithful, followed by July females and then by males. Spiders moved long distances through complexly structured habitats, suggesting that nests are likely to be out of sight of the spiders during the day, and that spiders must use cues other than their nest to navigate. We then tested whether females used beacons to find their nests. In one experiment, spiders col- onized nest tubes that were either associated with beacons (orange wooden dowels) or not. They were then released near a test beacon. Spiders experienced with beacons readily approached either their own beacon or a similar, novel beacon in a different location. In contrast, spiders with no experience with beacons were significantly less likely to approach the test beacon. In a second test, spiders tested with novel beacons of the same colour as their own were significantly more likely to approach it than were spiders tested with novel beacons of a different colour.
    [Show full text]
  • The Duration of Copulation in Spiders: Comparative Patterns
    Records of the Western Australian Museum Supplement No. 52: 1-11 (1995). The duration of copulation in spiders: comparative patterns Mark A. Elgar Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia Abstract - The duration of copulation in spiders varies both-within and between species, and in the latter by several orders of magnitude. The sources of this variation are explored in comparative analyses of the duration of copulation and other life-history variables of 135 species of spiders from 26 families. The duration of copulation is correlated with body size within several species, but the pattern is not consistent and more generally there is no inter-specific covariation between these variables. The duration of copulation within orb-weaving spiders is associated with both the location of mating and the frequency of sexual cannibalism, suggesting that the length of copulation is limited by the risk of predation. Finally, entelegyne spiders copulate for longer than haplogyne spiders, a pattern that can be interpreted in terms of male mating strategies or the complexity of their copulatory apparatus. INTRODUCTION after the copulatory organ has been inserted. In It is widely recognised that there are conflicts of species in which females mate with several males, interest between males and females in the choice of copulation may provide the male with the mating partner and the frequency of mating (e.g. opportunity to manipulate the sperm of other Elgar 1992). Thus, while the principal function of males that previously mated with that female. For copulation is to transfer gametes, the act of mating example, copulating male damselflies not only may have several additional functions, such as transfer their own sperm, but also remove the mate assessment or ensuring sperm priority, and sperm of rival males (e.g.
    [Show full text]