Masaryk University Faculty of Arts

Department of English and American Studies EnglishLanguageandLiterature HelenaJezdinská Nominalizations in English: Particle Shift in Gerundive Nominalizations Bachelor ’sDiplomaThesis Supervisor:Mgr.JanChovanec,Ph.D. 2008

IdeclarethatIhaveworkedonthisthesisindependently, usingonlytheprimaryandsecondarysourceslistedinthebibliography. ...... HelenaJezdinská Acknowledgement Aboveall,IwouldliketothankprofessorPetrKarlíkforallhisadvice,including recommendationofthesources,valuableandinterestingdiscussionsandmoralsupport. Iwouldalsoliketothankdr.JanChovanecforbeingmysupervisorandhelpingmeto copewiththeinadequaciesofthethesis.Last,butnotleast,Ithankdr.Jarmila Fictumová,HonzaandJanafortheirhelpingmetoworkwiththeBNC. Table of Contents LISTOFABBREVIATIONS...... 1 INTRODUCTION...... 2 1.NOMINALIZATIONINCONTEMPORARYENGLISHGRAMMARS...... 4

1.1. ENGLISH GRAMMAR BOOKS...... 4 1.1.1.QUIRK...... 5 1.1.1.1.NOMINALIZATION...... 5 1.1.1.2.–ing CLAUSES...... 7 1.1.2.HUDDLESTON&PULLUM...... 7

1.2. –ing FROM ACZECH GRAMMARIAN’S POINT OF VIEW...... 9 2.GENERATIVEAPPROACH...... 12

2.1. LEES: TRANSFORMATIONAL RULES...... 13

2.2. CHOMSKY: LEXICALIST HYPOTHESIS...... 15

2.3. MARANTZ: CRITICISM OF CHOMSKY...... 20

2.4. PARTICLE SHIFT IN “MIXED” NOMINALIZATION...... 25

2.5. SUMMARY...... 32

2.6. SOURCES FOR THE ANALYSIS...... 34 2.6.1.THEBRITISHNATIONALCORPUS...... 34 2.6.2.DICTIONARYOFPHRASAL...... 35 3.ANALYSIS:PARTICLESHIFTIN“POSS ing DP”...... 36

3.1. DATA FOR ANALYSIS...... 36

3.2. THE WAY OF ANALYSING THE DATA...... 38

3.3. BNC QUERIES OF PHRASAL VERBS AND “POSS VERBing DP”...... 38 3.3.1.PHRASALVERBS...... 38 3.3.2.“POSSVERBing DP”...... 43 3.3.3.ADDITIONALRESEARCHES...... 46 3.3.3.1.“LONG”...... 46 3.3.3.2.ADDITIONALRESEARCH:“POSSVERBing DP”WITHPRT SHIFT...... 46

3.4. CONCLUSION...... 48 4.WORKSUSEDANDCITED...... 51

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS

Sincethefactsinthispaperwillbesimplifiedforthereasonoflucidityandbriefness, and some of the individual structures will be demonstrated by tree diagrams, the list belowprovidesthemeaningsofallusedabbreviations:

ADV(alsoparticle)

BNCtheBritishNationalCorpus 1

D

DPdeterminerphrase

FPfunctionalprojection

N,unit

NPnounphrase

ON nominalobject

OP pronominalobject

PoS partsofspeech

PRON

Prt particle

Ssentencelikeunit vfunctionalofvP

Vverb;verbalunit vPfunctionalprojectionofV

VPverbphrase

1BritishNationalCorpus .. 1 INTRODUCTION

ThegoalofthispaperfocusesonaspecificproblemofEnglishnominalization,thatis, ashiftofanadverbialparticleafterobjectingerundivenominalizations,formedfrom phrasalverbs(i.e.verbswithadverbialparticles):

(0) a. hislookinguptheword (gerundivenominalizationwithoutparticleshift)

b. hislookingthewordup (gerundivenominalizationwithparticleshift)

ManyspeakersofEnglishlanguage,bothasamothertongueandasasecondlanguage, unconsciously use nominalized structures every day. This paper will illustrate how nominalizationsareusedinpractice,and,moreover,itwillalsoofferadeeperinsight into the internal structure of such noun phrases. Not only will the thesis focus on particle shift but it will also introduce the most important theories, as well as a contemporary view of nominalization. The thesis will be divided into two parts, theoreticalandpractical.

The theoretical part will start with the descriptive English grammars, such as

Quirk (1985). This part will deal with the generalized explanations of what English nominalizationis,aswellaswhattypesofnominalizationcanbefoundinEnglish.It will also present the problem of verbparticle structures and how the contemporary grammarsanddictionariescopewithit.Inordertogodeeperintotheinternalstructure ofnominalphrase,Chapter2ofthetheoreticalpartwillprovidethedevelopmentofthe

“generative”nominalizationtheoriesfromLees(1968)tothemoderntheories,suchas

Marantz (1997), comparing the most opposing approaches, i.e. lexicalism vs. the frameworkofDistributed.

The practical part will draw upon the paper by Harley and Noyer (1998)

2 regarding particle shift in “mixed” nominalization. Both verbal and nominalized structures will be examined, aiming at the analysis of a particular type of nominalizations, i.e. gerundive nominalizations with possible particle shift. The practical part will be based on the British National Corpus and verified by Collins

CobuildDictionaryofPhrasalVerbs (1990).

NOTE:allexamplesusedinthispaperareoriginallyexamplesfromthecorresponding worksorfromtheBritishNationalCorpus.

3 1. NOMINALIZATION IN CONTEMPORARY BOOKS

Beforetheproblemofnominalizationwillbediscussed,itshouldbeclearwhattheterm

“nominalization”means.Agooddefinitioncanbefoundin TheOxfordCompanionto theEnglishLanguage (1992)where“nominalization”isexplainedas:

Theprocessorresultofforminganounfromawordbelongingtoanotherword

class: writing/writings and shaving/shavings derived from write and shave by

adding ing ; sanity derivedfrom sane bytheadditionofthenounforming

ity ; nominalization derivedfrom nominalize byadding ation .(2)Theprocessor

result of deriving a noun phrase by a transformation from a finiteclause " their

rejectingmycomplaint or theirrejectionofmycomplaint from Theyrejectedmy

complaint .(p.702)

1.1. ENGLISH GRAMMAR BOOKS

Books on English grammar can illustrate how the concept of “nominalization” is apprehendedandhowitisexplainedtoabroaderaudience,suchasstudentsofEnglish language.Theybringaverydetaileddescriptionofhowandunderwhatconditionsand rules these structures can be applied to English. For the following subchapters both

EnglishandCzechgrammarianshavebeenchosen,inordertoproposetheindividual viewsof“nominalization”inEnglish.

4 1.1.1. QUIRK 2

1.1.1.1. NOMINALIZATION

In this work, the authors depict in detail, both morphologically and syntactically, the commontypesofEnglishnominalizations.Inaddition,theyparticularlysingleoutthe semantics of the individual structures. The term “nominalization” is determined as

“anounphrase[…]whichhasasystematiccorrespondencewithaclausestructure[…].

Thenounheadofsuchaphraseisnormallyrelatedmorphologicallytoaverb(1)orto an(2)”(p.1288):

(1) his refusal tohelp~He refuses tohelp.

(2) the truth ofherstatement~Herstatementis true .

Theauthorspointoutthatthereisadifferenceinmeaningbetweenadeverbalnoun, suchas(3)and(4),andtheircorrespondingverbalnoun(5)and(6):

(3) somepaintingsofBrown’s

~(a)somepaintingsthatBrownowns,or(b)somepaintingspaintedbyBrown

(4) Brown’spaintingsofhisdaughter

~ (a) paintings depicting his daughter and painted by him, or (b) paintings

depictinghisdaughterandpaintedbysomeoneelsebutownedbyhim.

(5) ThepaintingofBrown isasskilfulasthatofGainsborough.

~(a)Brown’smodeofpainting,or(b)Brown’sactionofpainting.

(6) Brown’sdeftpaintingofhisdaughter isadelighttowatch

~ItisadelighttowatchwhileBrowndeftlypaintshisdaughter.

Atthisstage,theauthorsclaimthatthein(3)and(4)withpluralendingsandthe

2Quirk,Randolph,etal.(1985). AComprehensiveGrammaroftheEnglishLanguage .London: Longman. 5 possibilityofbeingreplacedbyothernouns,suchas pictures or photos ,areanexample of “aperfectly regular concrete count noun, related only to the verb paint by word formation” (p. 1291) while (5) and (6) show the properties of a noun by having the definite in (5), whereas in(6) both a genitive construction and the adjective premodifier deft canbefound.However,theauthorsassertthat“ painting herecouldnot be replaced by picture or photo , but only by abstract nouns like representation, portrayal […]” (p. 1291) which means that it is the noun “that can be formed from verbsby adding –ing and inserting of before the nounphrase that corresponds tothe subjectiftheobjectisnotexpressed”(p.1291).

Ontheotherhand,thegenitiveconstructionin(7)and(8)ispossiblebutthereis adverbial modification by deftly in (7) instead of the adjective deft like in (6).

Furthermore,in(8)thenounphrase hisdaughter isfollowing painting directly,instead ofthe of phraselikein(6).

(7) Brown’sdeftlypaintinghisdaughter isadelighttowatch.

~(5b)or(6)inmeaning.

(8) Idislike Brown’spaintinghisdaughter .

~Idislikeeither(a)thefactor(b)thewaythatBrowndoesit.

Theauthorsaddthat“traditionallythismixtureofnominalandverbalcharacteristicsin the–ing formhasbeengiventhename‘’asopposedtothe‘(present)’”

(p.12911292).

6 1.1.1.2. –ing CLAUSES

The authors also highlight the distinction between “nominalization” and structures

which they call “–ing clauses”. They classify “–ing clauses” as the“clauses that are

sometimes called ‘gerundive’ or ‘gerundival clauses’. Their verb is commonly called

a‘gerund’”(p.1064).

The authors assume that nominal –ing clauses may function as a subject

(realized by the item either in the genitive case, objective case, or common case),

adirectobject,asubjectcomplement,anappositive,anadjectivalcomplementation,and

as a prepositional complement. From the point of view of semantics, the difference

betweentheindividual–ing clausesmaybedeterminateeitherasafact(9)oranaction

(10):

(9) YourdrivingacartoNewYorkinyourconditiondisturbsmegreatly.

(10) YourdrivingacartoNewYorktooklongerthanIexpected .

1.1.2. HUDDLESTON & PULLUM 3

Theauthorsconsidernominalizationaprocessofwordformation,which“prototypically

involvestheformationofanounfrombasesofotherclasses,byaffixation,conversion,

or phonological modification” (p. 1696). They are especially interested in affixation.

Theyclassifytheindividualtypesofnominalizationintothefollowinglexicalsemantic

groupswiththeircorresponding:

3Huddleston,R.andPullumG.K.(2002). TheCambridgeGrammaroftheEnglishLanguage . Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. 7 1) Person/instrumentnominalizations :

a)ant/ent (assistant,correspondent)

b)ard (drunkard)

c)arian (humanitarian)

d)ee (employee)

e)eer (engineer)

f)er/or/ar (baker,instructor,liar)

g)ist (anglicist)

h)nik (beatnik)

i)ster (gangster)

j)processofconversion(bore,spy)

2) Action/state/processnominalizations :

a)age (breakage)

b)–al (refusal)

c)–ance/ence (acceptance,violence)

d)–ation/ion/ition/sion/tion/ution (atomization,confusion,perdition,

compulsion,absorption,solution)

e)dom (boredom)

f)–hood (livelihood)

g)–ing (killing)

8 h)–ism (archaism)

i)–ity/ety/ness (actuality,business)

j)–ment (settlement)

k)–ship (apprenticeship)

l)–th (growth)

m)–ure (departure)

n)–y (difficulty), acy/cy/sy (privacy,idiocy,hypocrisy),ty (certainty), ery/ry

(bakery,surgery), ancy/ency (vacancy,decency)

o)minorsuffixes(e.g.laugh ter ,merg er ,hat red ,complain t)

p)phonologicalmodification–stress('digest)

1.2. –ing FROM A CZECH GRAMMARIAN’S POINT OF VIEW4

This section will present an example of how a Czech grammarian of English can grapple with the conception of ing nominalization. It is necessary to point out that

Dušková’s Mluvnicesoučasnéangličtiny:napozadíčeštiny (1994)couldbetranslated as A Contemporary English Grammar Against a Background of Czech , i.e. not only doestheauthorexplaintherulesofEnglishgrammarbutshealsoaddsimportantfacts about Czech language which can be connected with the corresponding English grammar.

Dušková assumes that the gerund is originally a noun. This fact should be obviousfromthesyntacticenvironmentofit;therefore,sheaddsthatthegrammatical 4Dušková,L.(1994). Mluvnicesoučasnéangličtiny:napozadíčeštiny .Praha:Academia.

9 functionofagerundcanbeeitherasubject,anobject,anominalpartofa,an attribute,or,finally,anadverbial.,ontheotherhand,havenotonlynominal butalsoverbalproperties,suchasgovernment,adverbialmodification,tenseandvoice.

Dušková, similarly as Quirk (1985), sees the difference between a gerund and adeverbal noun although sometimes they can look exactly the same. For instance,

Duškováclaimsthat(11)canbeinterpretedeitheras(a)aresultofaverbalaction~his drawing(i.e. picture )fascinatedme,(b)anactionofaverbalaction~theway,inwhich he drew it, fascinated me, or (c) a fact of a verbal action ~ the fact that he drew it fascinatedme:

(11) Hisdrawingfascinatedme

In the case of the meaning (a), there is a deverbal noun which has all properties of nouns; i.e. it is possible to make a plural, it can be modified by an adjective and determinedbyanarticle,anditcanalsobesupplementedwiththegenitive(insteadof thearticle),asisdemonstratedin(12)orwith of, inordertomarktheaccusativecaseof thedirectobject,asin(13):

(12) Hisdrawingsfascinateme.

(13) Olddrawingsoftownsfascinateme.

In the case of (b), we cannot use plural for drawing although the construction of drawing isnominal–itcanhaveanarticlebutitcannothaveapastandapassiveform:

(14) Hisrapiddrawingofthepicturefascinatedme .

(15) *hishavingdrawnofthepicture

(16) *itsrapidbeingdrawn

10 AnotherimportantnotethatDuškováaddsisthefactthatonlyverbsofaction

(i.e.processverbs)havethenominalconstruction,incontrasttothenonactionverbs:

(17) *hishavingofnomoney

(18) *theirnotbelievingoftherumour

In the third case – the fact that something was done – the gerund preserves the propertiesofverbs:government,differencesintenseandvoice,andapossibilitytobe modifiedbyanadverb.Thus,wecangetsentencessuchas(19)–(21):

(19) Hisdrawingthepicturesorapidlyfascinatedme.

(20) Herhavingdrawnthepicturerapidlyleftitsmarksonit.

(21) Itshavingbeendrawnrapidlydoesnotdetractfromitsvalue.

Gerundialconstructionscanbeformedfromallverbs,evenfromthenonactiveverbs, suchas have or believe in(22)and(23):

(22) hishavingnomoney

(23) theirnotbelievingtherumour

11 2. GENERATIVE APPROACH

Thepreviouschapterhasestablishedthattheproblemofnominalizationhasbeenonly sketchedbythe“classic”grammarswhichonlydescribetheformsoflanguage,without aclarificationofwhyandhowsuchconstructionswork.Therefore,thepaperincludes also this section which is the ground of the theoretical part, as well as an important guidepost for the practical part of the thesis. In order to go deeper into the internal structure of a nominal phrase, a generative viewpoint (which, in contrast to “classic” grammars,doesnotdescribetheformsofalanguagebutrather,ittriestofindprinciples whichcreatetheseforms)mustbeconsidered.Itisgenerativegrammaranditslinguists who are able with their techniques and methods to analyse and examine empirically what is happening inside nominalization. The following theories will be ordered diachronically and will describe the individual understanding and views of nominalization:

1)transformationalistrulesbyLees(1968),

2)lexicalistapproachbyChomsky(1970),andfinally,

3)nonlexicalistapproachbyMarantz(1997).

Last, but not least, Harley and Noyer’s paper (1998) which endorses the third (i.e. nonlexicalist) approach, will be brought in to show the specific problem of English nominalization: particle shift in “mixed” nominalization formed from verbparticle constructions.

12 2.1. LEES: TRANSFORMATIONAL RULES 5

This chapter starts with Lees’s The Grammar of English Nominalizations (1968)

because, in fact, this is the first piece of linguistic work where the process of

nominalization has been systematically described to such an extent and where the

authorwasalsoconcernedwithunitssmallerthanaword.InhisworkLeesintroduces

thebackgroundandrulesforconstructingEnglishsentencesandsupportstheformation

of the sentences by bringing out the transformational rules. The derivations are

representedbybranchingdiagrams.

Leesclaimsthattheprocessofnominalizationsimplyhappenswhenasentence

likeunittransformstomakeanominalunitS→Nbecause“transformedsentencesmust

occur as nominals within other sentences” (p. 54). Furthermore, “the nominals

generatedbytherules[…]arenotthemselvessentencesbutrather,theyarenounlike

versionsofsentences”(p.54).

In addition, Lees highlights the importance of the passive transformation

because “passive transforms enter as source into various nominalization

transformations” (p. 34). Lees divides the “–ing ” nominalizations into three types:

actionnominals(24),gerundivenominals(25),andatypeofgerundusedasaconcrete

noun(26).

(24) His drawing fascinatedmebecausehealwaysdiditlefthanded.

(25) His drawing fascinated me because I didn’t know he could be persuaded so

easily.

(26) His drawing fascinatedmebecauseitwassolarge.

5Lees,R.(1968). TheGrammarofEnglishNominalisations. TheHague:Mouton.

13 AlthoughLeesmentionsthesethreetypes,hisstudyconcernsonlythefirsttwotypes.

He gives details of the properties of the two types and points out the differences betweenthem.Lees’sobservationsaresummarizedinthefollowingtable:

Action nominal Gerundive nominal

Auxiliaries No Yes *His having broughtupofthebox His having broughtupthebox

Modification by an Yes No adjective His rapid drawingofthepicture *His rapid drawingthepicture

Modification by an No Yes adverb *Hisdrawingofthepicture rapidly Hisdrawingthepicture rapidly

Article after Yes No deletion of the The rapiddrawingofthepicture *The drawingofthepicturerapidly genitive modifier

Another parallel Yes No nominal Hisstrongobjecting...=Hisstrong Hisobejctingstrongly...≠*His objection objection strongly

Transitive verb Requires a preposition before the Does not require a preposition before object the object Hisrapiddrawing of thepicture Hisdrawingthepicturerapidly

Certain verbs, such No Yes as have, resemble, *His having ofahat His having ahat catch and “non- *His resembling ofhismother His resembling hismother action” verbs *His believing ofit His believing it (believe, admire... ) *His admiring ofher His admiring her

Convertibility to a Yes, if the preposition is of No preposed genitive Thecommitee'sappointmentofJohn John'sdrawingthepicture construction →John'sappointmentbythecommitee →*Thepicture'sdrawingbyJohn Thecommitee'sobjectiontoJohn →*John'sobjection(to)bythe commitee

Extra notes Of phraseisnota“”genitive Asimilaritywithanominalconstructed with“to”

14 2.2. CHOMSKY: 6

Chomsky’s Remarks on Nominalization (1970) is included in this paper because it representsalexicalistapproachtonominalization.Thelexicalist“philosophy”isbased ontheexistenceofthecomputationalLexiconasacomponentofgrammaticalstructure, whichcomes“before”the.AccordingtoMarantz(1997),lexicalismfollowsthe ideathatsomeofthesound/meaningunitsarealreadypresentintheLexicon,whereas theotherphonological/semanticoperationsperforminandaftersyntax.Themodelof suchanapproachcanbedescribedbythisscheme:

Lexicon Syntax

SoundMeaningSoundMeaning

Chomsky(1970)dividesEnglishnominalizationsonlyintotwotypes:gerundive nominals(28),andderivednominals(29)asthetransformationsofthestructurein(27):

(27) Johnhasrefusedtheoffer.

(28) John’srefusingtheoffer

(29) John’srefusaloftheoffer

Chomsky determines the three principal differences between the two types: productivity, semantic relations between the nominal and its proposition, and the internalstructureofthenominal.

As for the productivity, Chomsky claims that the “gerundive nominals can be formed fairly freely from the propositions of subjectpredicate form” (p. 187) while

“thereare,however,manyrestrictionsontheformationofderivednominals”(p.188).

Forinstance,gerundivenominals(31)canbeformedfromthepropositionsof(30).On

6Chomsky,N.(1970).RemarksonNominalization.InR.JacobsandP.Rosenbaum, ReadingsinEnglish TransformationalGrammar, pp.184221 .Waltham(MA):Ginn. 15 theotherhand,derivednominalsof(32),asatransformationofthesamepropositions, areconsideredungrammatical.

(30) a.Johniscertain(likely)towintheprize

b.Johnamused(interested)thechildrenwithhisstories

(31) a.John’sbeingcertain(likely)towintheprize

b.John’samusing(interesting)thechildrenwithhisstories

(32) a.*John’scertainty(likelihood)towintheprize

b.*John’samusement(interest)ofthechildrentoplease

However,Chomskyassumesthattherearemanyderivednominals,suchasin

(33), which are similar to those of (32) but the construction of the sentence must be completely changed, both syntactically and semantically. Moreover, they do not correspondtothemeaningofgerundivenominals(34)equivalentlyeverytime:

(33) a.John’scertaintythatBillwintheprize

b.John’samusementat(interestin)thechildren’santics

(34) a.John’sbeingcertainthatBillwintheprize

b.John’sbeingamusedat(interestedin)thechildren’santics

Chomskydoesnotdiscusstheproblemofthedifferenceofmeaningsbetween derivedandgerundivenominalstoalargeextent.Heonlypointsoutthat“therelation ofthemeaningbetweenthenominalandthepropositionisquiteregular”(p.187)for the gerundive nominals while “the semantic relations between the associated proposition and the derived nominal are quite varied and idiosyncratic” (p. 188). To support this assumption, he offers a list of a few derived nominals, such as laughter, 16 marriage,construction,action,revolution,belief,doubt .

Chomskyassertsthattheinternalstructureofanounphraseistypicalonlyfor derivednominals.AsgerundivenominalsdonothavetheinternalstructureofanNP, the Saxon genitive John’s cannot be replaced by any determiner, such as that or the .

Moreover,thegerundivenominalscannotbemodifiedby.Ontheotherside, derived nominals, such as (35), can be formed without restrictions, in contrast to the gerundivenominalof(36)whichisungrammatical.Anotherdifferenceintheinternal structureisthefactthatthederivednominalsdonotcontainaspect;therefore,thereis nocorrespondingderivednominaltothegerundivenominalof(37).Ontheotherhand, manyderivednominalscanbepluralizedand“occurwiththefullrangeof

(p.189),suchas(38).

(35) theproofofthetheorem

(36) *theprovingthetheorem

(37) John’shavingcriticizedthebook

(38) John’sthreeproofsofthetheorem,severalofJohn’sproofsofthetheorem

Othercharacteristicofthederivednominals,incontrasttothegerundivenominals,is afreeappearance“inthefullrangeofNPstructures”(p.189).Forinstance,thedouble passiveof(39)ispossible(40):

(39) JohngaveBilladvice

(40) a.advicewasgiven(to)Bill

b.Billwasgivenadvice

17 Inaddition,Chomskyassertsthat“thestrongestandmostinterestingconclusion thatfollowsfromthelexicalisthypothesisisthatderivednominalsshouldhavetheform of base sentences, whereas gerundive nominals may in general have the form of transforms”(p.212).Asforthegerundivenominalsandtheirtransformation,Chomsky claimsthatoneoftheformsofNPintroducedbyrulesofthecategoricalcomponentof thebaseis(41),andthatgeneralrulesofplacementgivefreelygeneratedsurface formsofthegerundivenominal:

(41) [SNP nom (Aspect)VP] S

ChomskycommentsonLees’s TheGrammarofEnglishNominalizations(1965) andcriticizesonthisworkbecausethetheorywasdeveloped

[…]onlyintermsofgrammaticaltransformations.Therewasnootherwayto

expressthefactthatthecontextsinwhich refuse appearsasaverband refusal as

a noun are closely related. However, when the lexicon is separated from the

categorical component of the base and its entries are analyzed in terms

ofcontextual features, this difficulty disappears. We can enter refuse in the

lexicon as an item with certain fixed selectional and strict subcategorization

features,whichisfreewithrespecttothecategoricalfeatures[noun]and[verb]

(p.190).

Chomskyalsopointsouttheproblem withverbparticleconstructions,suchas lookup(theinformation),defineaway(theproblem) Accordingtohim,they“undergo gerundive nominalization freely (his looking up the information, his looking the informationup,hisdefiningawaytheproblem,hisdefiningtheproblemaway)(p.193).

As for the derived nominals, they are, according to Chomsky, “rather marginal, and

18 hencenotveryinformative”(p.193).Heregardstheformsin(42)asderivednominals, preferableto(43):

(42) a.hislookingupoftheinformation

b.hisdefiningawayoftheproblem

(43) a.*hislookingoftheinformationup

b.*hisdefiningoftheproblemaway

However, Chomsky sees the problem of (42), as well as nominals in (44) being consideredasderivednominalswhichseemrathercurious,clumsyandnotobviousto be derived nominals. These forms are assumed by Chomsky to have the internal structureofNP,therefore“thepossessivesubjectcanbereplacedbyadeterminer[…].

On the other hand, adjective insertion seems quite unnatural in this construction”

(p.214).

(44) a.John’srefusingoftheoffer

b.John’sprovingofthetheorem

c.thegrowingoftomatoes

This third type of nominalization (44), which has properties of both derived and gerundivenominals,suchaspossessivepronounorSaxongenitive(i.e.apropertyof anoun)andendingin–ing (=apropertyofaverb),Chomskycalls“mixed”nominals.

However, Chomsky is rather careful, therefore, he does not handle with this type of nominalizationtoalargeextent.

19 2.3. MARANTZ: CRITICISM OF CHOMSKY 7

Marantz’spaper(1997),basedonthetheoryofDistributedMorphology 8isincludedin thisthesisbecauseitpresentsadifferentviewofEnglishnominalization.Thedifference is the exact opposite of Chomsky’s Lexical Hypothesis just mentioned above. The philosophyofDistributedMorphologyisbasedonthefactthatitdeniestheexistenceof thelexiconbeforesyntax.

Inhiswork,Marantzveryemotivelycriticizesanddumpslexicalismbysaying strongwords,suchas“thispaperbringsthereaderthefollowingnews:Lexicalismis dead, deceased, demised, no more, passed on” (p. 202). He supports the arguments againsttheLexiconwiththeframeworkofDistributedMorphology(Halle&Marantz,

1993).Moreover,heattackstheideathatChomskyisalexicalist.AccordingtoMarantz,

Chomskyisonlyinterpretedandconsideredbyotherlinguistsasalexicalistlinguist.

Marantz asserts that “any theory must include one or more lists of atomic elements that the computational system of grammar might combine into larger units.

Distributed Morphology explodes the Lexicon and includes a number of distributed, noncomputational lists as Lexiconreplacements […]” (p. 203). Marantz offers three suchlists,asasubstitutionfortheLexicon:

 List 1, the “Narrow Lexicon”: is generative and “contains the atomic roots of the

languageandatomicbundlesofgrammaticalfeatures”(p.203).Setsofgrammatical

featuresareformedfreely.

 List2,“Vocabulary”:“providesthephonologicalformsfortheterminalnodesfrom

the syntax […]” (p. 204). In addition, it “includes the connections between sets 7Marantz,A.(1997).Noescapefromsyntax:Don’ttrymorphologicalanalysisintheprivacyofyour ownlexicon. PennWorkingPapersinLinguistics,4.2 ,201225. 8Halle,M.andMarantzA.(1993).DistributedMorphologyandthePiecesof.InK.Hale&S. Keyser(eds.),TheViewfromBuilding20, 111176.Cambridge,MA:MIT. 20 ofgrammatical features and phonological features, and thus determines the

connections between terminal nodes from the syntax and their phonological

realization”(p.204).List2isnongenerative.

 List 3, “Encyclopedia”: similarly as the List 2, List 3 is nongenerative but

expandable.It“liststhespecialmeaningsofparticularroots,relativetothesyntactic

contextoftheroots,withinlocaldomains”(p.204).

Inordertoprovideomodelofgrammarfromthenonlexicalistpointofview,Marantz proposesthefollowingscheme(45)asastructureofgrammarwithouttheLexicon:

(45) Structureofgrammar:

Inthechapterconciselycalled“ RemarksonNominalization killslexicalismto death” Marantz deals with the behavior of the individual nominalizations. He claims that there is acontradiction in the distribution of grammatical categories. By nominalizing(46a.)totheNPin(46c.),theverb destroy “sharessomedistributional propertieswithverbs–theabilitytotakecomplementsandsubjects[…]whilesharing other(e.g.morphologicalproperties)withnouns”(p.213).

(46) a.thatJohndestroyedthecity

b.*thatthecitydestroyed

c.John’sdestructionofthecity

d.Thecity’sdestruction

21 e.John’sdestroyingthecity

Heassertsthatthereisaconnectionbetweentheindividualnominalizationsand the meaning of the category neutral roots in (47) – (49). If the roots are located in anominalenvironment,thentheresultisanominalization.Ontheotherside,whenthe rootsareplacedinaverbalenvironment,theybecomeverbs.

(47) √DESTROY ~ change of state, not internally caused (so, implies external

causeor)

(48) √GROW~changeofstate,internallycaused

(49) √BREAK~result(ofchangeofstate)

Marantzdescribesthepropertiesoftheindividualrootstoabiggerextent.Forinstance, verbsoftheroottype(47)areonlytransitive,whereasthenominalizationscanbeeither transitive(46c.)orintransitive(46d.).

Verbs of the √GROW are either transitive (50 a.) or intransitive (50 b.) but their nominalizationsareonlyintransitive(50d.):

(50) a.thatJohngrowstomatoes

b.thattomatoesgrow

c.*John’sgrowthoftomatoes

d.thetomatoes’growth

Asforofthegroupofthe√BREAK,theycanbeeithertransitive(51a.)orintransitive

(51b.)buttheirnominalizationsdonothaveanyargument(51c.)–(51e.).

(51) a.thatJohnbreakstheglass

b.thattheglassbreaks

c.*John’sbreakoftheglass

d.*theglass’sbreak

22 e.*thebreakoftheglass

Marantz asserts that there are two kinds of functional heads.The first type is functionalhead"v1"which,asahead,"projectsanagent"(p.217).Ontheotherhand, thesecondtype"v2"operatesasaheadwhichdoesnotprojectanagent.Duetothe choice of these functional heads, the root then can become either a verb or a noun, dependingontheirenvironments.In(52)thenominaluse(becauseofthemergeofthe rootwithaD)of√DESTROYisdemonstrated:

(52) thedestructionofthecity,thecity'sdestruction

AccordingtoMarantz,ifthereis"theagentprojectingv1,whichservesto'verbalize' rootsinitsenvironment"(p.218),thenstructure(53)canbeformed,aswellastheing nominalizationof(54)whichMarantzclaimstobeatrue"nominalization",becauseit containsboththeverbalisingv1andthenominalenvironmentofaD.

(53) Johndestroyedthecity

23 (54) John'sdestroyingthecity(likelymoreheadsbetweenDandv1,e.g.foring )

Ontheotherhand,when√GROW"isplacedinthenominalenvironment[...],thereis no agentive argument for the possessive phrase, and we get only 'the growth of the tomatoes'or'thetomatoes'growth'"(p.219)asshowedin(55),whereaswecannothave anagentintheverbalenvironment(56).

(55) growthofthetomatoes

(56) Johngrowstomatoes,tomatoesaregrowing

The verbal environment of √BREAK which, according to Marantz, is an end statebrought"syntactically,achangeofstateandconsequentlyatheme,plusoptionally anexternalagent(ifv1ischosen)",asshowedin(51a.b.)while"thenominalforms

24 namestheendstate,a'break',andtakesnocomplements"(p.220).

The other example that Marantz offers to support the argument against the

lexicon was the fact that "the root √RISE belongs to the √GROW class and/or the

√BREAKclass"(p.221):

(57) a.theelevatoris ris ing[v2](internallycausedchangeofstate

b.Johnis rais inghisglass[v1](noimplicationofaninternalcause)

2.4. PARTICLE SHIFT IN “MIXED” NOMINALIZATION 9

Lastbutnotleast,HarleyandNoyer’s(1998)papershouldbeintroducedinorderto

exploreamorespecificproblemofEnglishnominalization,and,thatisparticleshiftin

theprocessofnominalizationfromverbparticleconstructions.

Theauthorshavetodealwiththefactthat“particleshiftisavailableintheVP

andthegerundbutnotinthemixednominalization”(p.4),ascanbeseenin(58a.d.).

(58) a.Chris’s writing up ofthepaper.(mixednominalization)

b.*Chris’s writing ofthepaper up .

c.Chris wrote thepaper up .

d.Chris wrote up thepaper.

To expand the distinctions between the different syntactical constructions, the

authorscomewiththreetypesofnominalizations:gerunds ,“ derived”nominalizations ,

and “ mixed” nominalizations . They describe the differences between them and are

concerned with the third type of nominalizations which seem to “exhibit all the

properties of derived nominalizations” (p. 3), but, on the other hand, they affix –ing ,

9Harley,H.&NoyerR.(1998).Mixednominalizations,shortverbmovementandobjectshiftin English. ProceedingsofNELS,28. 25 likegerunds.ThepropertiesofthethreetypesofnominalizationofferedbyHarleyand

Noyeraresummarizedinthistable:

Gerunds “Derived” (true) “Mixed” nominalizations Nominalizations

Examp Thebarbarianarmy('s) Thebarbarianarmy's Belushi'smixingofdrugs le suddenlydestroyingthe suddendestructionofthe andalcoholprovedfatal. cityupsetCaesar cityupsetCaesar.

Type DP V-ing DP DP's V-Nom of DP DP's V-ing of DP

VPsinallrespects(both Syntacticallydistinct Allpropertiesofderived syntacticallyand fromVPsinvarious nominalizationsbut interpretively) ways formedwith ing like gerunds

Propertiessimilarto Noauxiliariesor relatedVPs,e.g.may expletives containauxiliariesand expletives

Donothaveadjectival Adjectivalmodification modifiersornumber andnumbermaking makingaccusativecase; possible;noaccusative

Properties Properties instead,adverbial case,noradverbial modificationpossible modifiers

Morphophonologically Morphophonological transparentformationin conditioning ing (destroy → destruction)

AllVPshavea correspondinggerund

Themeargumentcase: markedby of 26 The process of short object movement and the consequent particle shift of constructionsof(58c.d.)areprovedbystructuresin(59).HarleyandNoyerexplain by(59a.)thattheverbhasto movefromthelowerVPtotheuppervPwhereasthe object thepaper isbeingcheckedbythefunctionalprojectionFPinordertoobtainan accusativecaseofadirectobject.

(59) a.Chriswrotethepaperup. b.Chriswroteupthepaper. vP vP DP DP Chris v FP Chris v FP

DP VP DP VP the the paper V PrtP paper V PrtP V Prt write t Prt t i t write+ up j up j

Theprocessinsidethestructure Chriswroteupthepaper (59b.),withoutthesplitofthe verbandtheparticle,looksalmostthesameastheprocess(59a.),“withtheadditional optionalphenomenonoftheparticlecliticizingtotheverbviaheadmovement”(p.5).

TheparticlethenoccursintheheadofvPwithobjectfollowingthewholeverb+particle complex.

The authors continue with description of the wellknown struggle between

Distributed Morphology (i.e. the framework of Halle and Marantz, 1993) and

Lexicalism.HarleyandNoyergointhewaythatDistributedMorphologydoes;i.e.they arenonlexicalist.Theydonotaccept“aLexiconwhosefunctionistoproducetheitems which project syntactic structure”, and therefore they assert that“particleshiftisnot

27 availableinnominalizationsbyconsigningnominalizationstotheLexicon”(p.9).They believe that “collections of morphosyntactic features are manipulated by the syntax, and, after all syntactic operations are complete, such features are then realized when morhophonological forms, taken from a set called the Vocabulary , are insterted at syntacticterminalnodes”(p.7).Asaresult,theauthorsexplainthattheycould

[...] analyze –ing as a polyfunctional Vocabulary Item, inserted both as the

gerundive affix and the default or Elsewhere nominalising affix. In gerundive

syntax the relevant feature is always realized as –ing . In nominal syntax,

however,avarietyofmorphophonologicalforms,suchas–(at)ion,ment,al and

soforthcompetewith–ing forinsertion.Ifavocabularyitemselectaspecialized

nominalization suffix, this suffix (which may trigger other readjustments) is

inserted,blockingthe–ing form[…].(p.9).

Consequently,theresultofnominalizationcanbeasin(60):

(60) a.Chris’s admiration ofMary

b.*Chris’s admiring ofMary

The other difference between the individual nominalizations is meaning. For instance, mixture in (61 a.) means “the resulting substance”, while mixing in (61 b.) means“theactivityof”.

(61) a.Belushi’s lethal mixture of drugs and alcohol provedfatalwhenhedrankit.

b.Belushi’s foolish mixing of drugs and alcohol provedfatal.

Harley and Noyer also note that “a morphologically specialized nominalization has aspecializedmeaning”(p.8).Moreover,theyprovidedafewexamplesofsuchcases:

28 (62) admire admirationof ?admiringof

destroy destructionof ?destroyingof

inspect inspectionof ?inspectingof

mix mixtureof mixingof

move movementof movingof

marry marriageof marryingof

According to Harley and Noyer, categorical status is syntactically determined.

Theyexplainthatsyntacticcategoriesarenotsyntacticprimitivesbutthereis“asingle contentful category [√P = RootP] which may appear in different syntactic contexts”

(p.9);i.e.theycanberealizedinitsverbalforms(ifdominatedbyvP)orinitsnominal forms (if dominated by DP). Therefore, a root such as √destroy can be realized as destruction ,ifitisunderDor,ontheotherhand,as destroy intheenvironmentofv.As aresult,vPmustbepresentingerundivesbecausegerundives“taketheverbalformof theroot,ratherthannominal( ThebarbariansdestroyingthecityupsetCaesar ,not *The barbariansdestructingthecityupsetCaesar )”(p.10).

Asforthetruenominalizations,theresultis,forinstance, destruction ,becauseit occursinaDPenvironment.BecausethereisnovPorFP(betweentwoVheads)in suchconstructions, of becomesthecasemarker(i.e.itcheckstheaccusativecase).The authorsassertthatbecause“mixed”nominalizationshavethesamesyntacticproperties as true nominalizations, they have also the same syntax. Hence “the –ing spellout simply reflects the Elsewhere realization in the nominal context” (p. 12). The comparisonofthetrueandmixednominalizationisshownin(63):

29 (63) a.Thebarbarians’destructionofthecity;b.Belushi’smixingofdrugsand

alcohol DP DP DP DP D D The ¦P Belushi ¦P barbarians 's 's

¦ DP ¦ DP destruction thecity mixing drugs of and of alcohol [Thebarbarians'[destructionofthecity]]

Moreover,HarleyandNoyermakeclearthattheobjectmovementinstructures,suchas

Chris’swritingupofthepaper isnotpossiblebecausetheobjecthasnowheretogo, sincethereisnovPnorFPinthestructure.BecauseoftheabsenceofFP,theaccusative caseoftheobjectcannotbecheckedbyitbutitispresentwithin of .

Furthermore,HarleyandNoyercomeupwiththreeotherissues:

 Theinsertionof of mustbeonlyaftersyntax

The authors demonstrate this assertion by theWhmovement test (64).They explain, thatthisisbecause“Whmovementoccursinthesyntax[…]needsCasemarking.None isavailable,soinsertionof of atSpellOutoccurs”(p.15).

(64) a.Whichgasdidyouencouragetheturningoffof t?

b.*Ofwhichgasdidyouencouragetheturningoff t?

 Impossibility of forming nominalizations with double object (from ditransitive

verbs)

(65) a.*Chris’sbettingofRobinahundreddollars

30 b.*Pat’snearlyimmediatewhippingupofChrissomedinner

 AdjunctionoftovPand√P

The authors cope with an additional subject: resultatives versus verb+particle constructions (i.e. phrasal verbs). There are three important properties of the constructionswithverbandparticle:

 if the particle shifts, there is no change in meaning in comparison with the

constructionwithoutparticleshift

 verb+particleconstructionshaveusuallyaspecialmeaningthantheindividualverb

plustheindividual“preposition”(i.e.adverbparticle)

 objecthasanaccusativecase,requiringthepresenceof of

Iftheobjectisontheleftoftheparticle,thenithasaresultativemeaning,suchasin

(66a.).However,ifitappearsontherightoftheparticle,thereisnoresultativemeaning

(66b.,c.):

(66) a.Chriswalkedthedogaround.(i.e.sequenceofV+DP+Prep)

b.*Chriswalkedaroundthedog.(sequenceV+P+DP;circumambulationonly)

c.*Chris’swalkingaroundofthedog.

Furthermore, verbparticle constructions can be compared with the constructions of

V+PP.Intheformercase,theobjectcanappearonlytotherightoftheP.Theauthors summarize the individual constructions (i.e. verbparticle, Presultatives, and V+PP idioms):

31 VerbParticle PResultative VerbPPidiom

VPDP ok * ok

VDPP ok ok *

Vnom of DPP * ok *

VnomDPP * * *

VnomPDP * * ok

VnomP of DP ok * *

2.5. SUMMARY

As is now apparent, linguist can see one and the same problem very differently.

Regardlessofthedifferentapproaches,fortheoncominganalysisinthepracticalpart ofthisessay,thefollowingfactshaveabigsignificance.Tosumup,accordingtothe mentionedlinguists,thereexistthreebasictypesofnominalizationwiththeseproperties inEnglishlanguage: i) derived nominalization, e. g. John’s destruction of the city

pattern“DP’sVNomofDP”;aformofbasesentences

innerstructureofNPanddifferentfromVP(e.g.theydonotcontainaspect)

modificationbyadjectives,noadverbialmodifiers,numberpossible(i.e.can

bepluralized),determiners

themeargumentcasemarker of

impossibilitytomakefromauxiliaries,expletivesandnonactionverb

32 possibilityofadoublepassive ii) gerundive nominalization, e. g. John’s refusing the offer

pattern“DP’sVing DP”,aformoftransforms

innerstructureofVP,suffix–ing asverbs

modificationbyadverbs,notbyadjectives,nonumbermaking

possibletomakefromauxiliariesandexpletives,aswellasfromnonaction

verbs

impossibletocontainotherdeterminersinsteadofpossessivepronounsor

nouns

accordingtoChomsky,particleshiftinnominalizationsfromphrasalverbs

possible iii) “mixed” nominalization, e. g. Belushi’s mixing of drugs and alcohol

pattern“DP’sVing ofDP”

hasallpropertiesof“derived”nominalization

formedwith–ing as“gerundive”nominalization

impossibilitytoshifttheparticleontherightofanobjectinverbparticle

structures

Thesubjectoftheanalysisofthefollowingsectionwillbethesecondtypeof nominalization, i.e. the “gerundive” nominalization. Even though “gerundive” nominalizationshavepropertiesofverbs,theyalso“behave”inasimilarwaythatnouns do(e.g.theycanoccuringrammaticalfunctions,inwhichnounsdo);therefore,they also are “mixed” in some ways. As a result, they will be referred to as

33 “POSSVERBing DP” nominalization, in order to not tocome to misunderstandings andtopreservebothaccuracyandobjectivity.

2.6. SOURCES FOR THE ANALYSIS

Lastbutnotleast,thesourcesforthefollowinganalysisshouldbementioned.Thereare two highly useful sources for the practical part of this thesis: The British National

Corpus and Collins Cobuild Dictionary of Phrasal Verbs . The sources were chosen becausetheyofferasatisfactoryaccuracyandcorrectness.

2.6.1. THE BRITISH NATIONAL CORPUS 10

TheBritishNationalCorpus(BNC)isadatabaseofbothwrittenandspokenEnglish language.Itconsistsofapproximately100millionwords(about90millionfromwritten sourcesand10millionfromspokenmaterial).Eventhoughthereisalsocorpus,suchas

OxfordEnglishCorpus,whichoffersevenmorethantwobillionwords,theaccessto suchacorpusislicensedonlytoselectedusers.Anothersourcefortheanalysiscould have been the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA) which contains morethan385millionwordsbuttheonlineversiondoesnotreceiveaqueryofmore thanfive“general”partofspeech(PoS)segmentsnecessaryfortheanalysis.Therefore, theBNCwaschosenforthispaperasthebestmaterialforthepracticalpart.

ThesearchintheBNCisentirelyeasyanditisonlyaquestionoftimewhena usercanbefamiliarwiththecorpusquerylanguage(CQL).TheadvantageoftheBNC isthatitisabletofindanyqueryofanysequenceofPoS.Ontheotherhand,theBNC 10 BritishNationalCorpus ..

34 has also its drawbacks.Although, as mentioned above, it contains about 100 million words, even this number is not sometimes large enough to include less common structuresandthisisthemaindisadvantageoftheBNC.Moreover,theBNCsometimes containsmistakes,probablyarisenfromtheprocessoftranscriptionofthematerialinto theBNC.

2.6.2. DICTIONARY OF PHRASAL VERBS 11

CollinsCobuildDictionaryofPhrasalVerbs (1990),abbreviatedinthispaperCCDPV, offersalistofphrasalverbs.Therearethousandsofphrasalverbswhicharedivided into several types, according to their syntactic use. For the following analysis it is necessarytofocusonatransitivephrasalverb,wheretheparticleshiftispossible.The dictionarypredictsthatsuchaverbcanhavethreesyntacticpatterns:

V+ADV+N Iwascleaningoutmydeskattheoffice.

V+N+ADV Ispentthreedayscleaningmyofficeout.

V+PRON+ADV TheColonelliftedhiscup,glaredatit,setitdownagain.

Thedictionarywaschosenfortheanalysisbecausephrasalverbsaresometimesrather problematictobedistinguishedfrom“common”verbswith“common”prepositionsand thereforeshouldbeverifiedbyareliablesource.

11Sinclair,J.,HanksP.&MoonR.etal(Eds.).(1990). CollinsCobuildDictionaryofPhrasalVerbs. Glasgow:Collins. 35 3. ANALYSIS: PARTICLE SHIFT IN “POSS VERB-ing DP”

Thegoalofthepracticalpartwillbeexaminationandproofofthepossibleparticleshift

(i.e. a movement of a particle to the right of object) in “POSS VERBing DP” nominalizations, formed from transitive phrasal verbs (i.e. verbs accompanied by particles).Asmentionedabove,“POSSVERBing DP”typeofnominalizationhasthe followingconsequenceofelements:

Possessive pronoun or Saxon genitive – verb -ing – object (either a noun or apronoun)

InthefollowingchapterIwilldealnotonlywithnominalizationswithparticle shift but also with phrasal verbs themselves, taking into consideration the fact that

“POSS VERBing DP” nominalizations have a lot in common with verbs. The relationship and connections between phrasal verbs and “POSS VERBing DP” nominalizationswillbeexplored.

3.1. DATA FOR ANALYSIS

Ifwewanttoknowmoreaboutparticleshiftin“POSSVERBing DP”nominalizations, we require two main materials for the analysis: 1) phrasal verbs and 2) nominalized structuresfromphrasalverbs.Moreover,weneedsubmaterials–bothphrasalverbsand nominalizationswithandwithoutparticleshift.Therefore,therewillbefourprincipal groupsofdifferentstructures:

36 phrasal verbs without particle shift

with particle shift

“POSS VERB-ing DP” without particle shift

with particle shift

OnthebasisofthefirstresultsfromtheBNC,aswellasofmyownempirical intuition,Ifindoutthatthefourjustmentionedgroupsmightnothavetobeenoughfor a proper analysis. The reason for that stems from the presumption that particle shift could depend on the type of its object which can be either a noun or a pronoun.

Therefore, the goal of the research should be expanded with two other sub sub materials.Thus,thefinaldataforanalysis,consistingofeightdifferentstructures,are likethis: phrasal verbs without Prt shift object=noun

object=pronoun

with Prt shift object=noun

object=pronoun

“POSS VERB-ing DP” without Prt shift object=noun

object=pronoun

with Prt shift object=noun

object=pronoun

37 3.2. THE WAY OF ANALYSING THE DATA

At this stage it is necessary to point out how the individual data will be analysed in ordertoprovetheaccuracyandcorrectnessoftheprocessofanalysing.

ExaminingphrasalverbsintheBNCcanberatherproblematicandtricky.Even though the BNC can distinguish an adverbial particle of phrasal verbs from common prepositions,notalwaysdoesitprovidetheexactdatathatareneededforthisanalysis.

Forinstance,theBNCfindsnotonlyphrasalverbsbutalsoverbsjustaccompaniedby adverbialprepositions,whichaltogetherdonotperformasphrasalverbs.Similarly,ing nominalizations are sometimes in the BNC interchanged by mistake with the progressiveing formofverbs.Moreover,theBNCitselfcancontainmistakes,suchas your insteadof you're.

To avoid this, all results found in the BNC should be checked one by one

“manually”,whichalsobringsanotherproblem–theresultsdonothavetobeentirely accurate.Therefore,theresultswillbeverifiedandcountedatleasttwice.Furthermore, unknownphrasalverbswillberegardedas“phrasalverbs”withthehelpofCCDPV.

3.3. BNC QUERIES OF PHRASAL VERBS AND “POSS VERB-ing DP”

3.3.1. PHRASAL VERBS

In order to avoid and exclude unnecessary elements, the query shouldbe as exact as possible.Byreasonoftheopportunitytochecktherelevanceoftheresults,therearethe queriesfortheindividualstructurescreatedbymyself(theabbreviationsinthebrackets showtheabbreviationsoftheCorpusQueryLanguage):

38 1) Phrasal verbs without particle shift and a nominal object = V-Prt-ON:

Forsuchastructure,wearelookingforanyformofaverb(VV.),followedtightlybyan adverbial preposition (AVP), followed by a determiner 12 (eitherAT0, DPS, or DT0), followed by any noun (NN.). The BNC found more than 45,000 results. In order to exclude elements which are not necessary in this structure, three additional negative filtersmustbeselected:wedonotneedanymarkofseparation(PUN),anycoordinating (CJC)orsubordinatingconjunction(CJS).Afterfiltering,morethan4,500 resultsarefoundbutitisstilltoomuchforthemanualverification;therefore,arandom sampleof3,500hitsischosen.Thequeryofthestructurethenlookslikethis:

Hits:3500

Querylc,[tag="VV."][tag="AVP"][tag="AT0|DPS|DT0"][]{1,2}[tag="NN."]

Negativefilter551[tag="PUN"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJC"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJS"]

Randomsample3500 13

2) Phrasal verbs without particle shift and pronominal object = V-Prt-OP:

SuchastructurerequiresalmostthesamesequenceasVPrtONwithadifferencein object.Insteadofadeterminerandanoun,apersonalpronoun(PNP)followingtightly theAVP is needed. This query finds 5,223 results. Negative filters are therefore also 12 BecauseIpressuposedthattherewillbealotofresultsofthisstructure,Itriedtorestrictthenumberof resultsbyavoiding“solitary”NPswithoutadeterminer(suchasnounsinplural) 13Thequerylc,[tag="VV."][tag="AVP"][tag="AT0|DPS|DT0"][]{1,2}[tag="NN."]hastensthousandsof results.Formyanalysis,arandomsampleof3,500resultsshouldshowenoughfeaturesand characteristicsofthestructure. 39 necessarytoavoidredundantelementsandstructures.

Hits:831

Querylc,[tag="VV."][tag="AVP"][tag="PNP"]

Negativefilter551[tag="PUN"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJC"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJS"]

3) Phrasal verbs with particle shift and nominal object = V-ON-Prt:

Thethirdstructureisalsoalmostthesameasthefirstone.Itdiffersinthepositionof the object andparticle:the nominal objectcomes tightly afterthe verbpreceding the particle.Negativefiltersmentionedabovewerealsoused.

Hits:357

Querylc,[tag="VV."][tag="AT0|DPS|DT0"][]{1,2}[tag="NN."][tag="AVP"]

Negativefilter551[tag="PUN"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJC"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJS"]

4) Phrasal verbs with particle shift and pronominal object = V-Prt-OP:

VPrtOPneedsasimilarconsequenceofelementsasVONPrt.Incontrastto VON

Prt, theobjectispronominal.

40 Hits:1773

Querylc,[tag="VV."][tag="PNP"][tag="AVP"]

Negativefilter551[tag="PUN"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJC"]

Negativefilter551[tag="CJS"]

AfteradoubleverificationofthedatafoundintheBNC,thereare:

2832 relevantresultsoutof3500for V-Prt-ON ;i.e.81%ofresultsfoundintheBNC

5 relevantresultsoutof831for V-Prt-OP ;i.e.0,6%ofresultsfoundintheBNC

220 relevantresultsoutof357for V-ON-Prt ;i.e.61,5%ofresultsfoundintheBNC

1427 relevantresultsoutof1773for V-OP-Prt ;i.e.80,5%ofresultsfoundintheBNC

For a better clarity and possibility to compare the individual structures, the resultsofverbalstructuresaresummarizedinthefollowingtable.Sincetheyareboth verydiverseandnumerous,onlytop(i.e.mostnumerous)resultshavebeenchosen:in thetabletherearetopfiveparticlesofeachstructure,aswellastopfivephrasalverbs, showingtopfiveparticlestheverbscombinewith.Theitems“totaltopfiveparticles” and “total top five phrasal verbs” shows the total number of occurrence of the mentionedparticlesandphrasalverbs,whereastheitems“totaldifferentparticle”and

“total different phrasal verbs” show the diversity of all relevant particles and phrasal verbsfoundintheBNC.

41 V Prt ON V Prt OP V ON Prt V OP Prt Total relevant results 2832 5 220 1427 Top five particles up 1238 out 2 back 52 up 349 out 806 in 1 up 43 out 270 down 210 off 1 down 34 back 213 off 148 up 1 out 33 down 185 on 141 in 19 off 152 Total top five particles 2543 5 181 1169 Total different particles 13 4 12 14 Top five verbs with set up 362 fill out 1 get back 13 take back 50 their top five particles 427 1 42 186 out 54 in 6 out 30 down 5 up 6 in 23 off 5 out 5 up 23 back 1 on 4 down 21 take on 90 find out 1 bring down 11 put down 31 255 1 28 126 over 63 back 8 off 20 up 62 in 4 back 17 out 32 over 4 on 16 down5 out 1 up 15 carry out 206 give in 1 take back 9 get back 41 223 1 28 98 on 14 out 8 out 11 off 3 off 3 down 10 up 3 in 10 down 1 off 8 bring about 52 stick up 1 put back 6 bring back 40 155 1 18 92 in 42 in 4 up 15 out 22 on 4 down 14 down 15 up 3 out 10 up 15 down 1 in 8 make up 84 take off 1 keep down 5 send out 11 96 1 11 34 out 12 up 3 off 8 in 1 back 7 on 1 down 2 out 1 in 2 Total top five phrasal 1156 5 127 536 verbs Total different phrasal 396 5 109 378 verbs 42 3.3.2. “POSS VERB-ing DP”

Intuitivelywe can suppose that nominalizations formed fromphrasalverbsarerather rare,incomparisonwithphrasalverbsdescribedintheprevioussubchapter.Togetas much positive results as possible, the queries for nominalizations should not be restrictedasmuchasthequeriesforphrasalverbswere.

1) “POSS VERB-ing DP” without particle shift and nominal object – ing -Prt-ON:

As mentioned above, the “POSS VERB ing DP” type of nominalization requires element which “possesses” the nominalization; it can be either a possessive pronoun

(DPS)oraSaxongenitive(POS;canbeeitherapropernounoracommonnoun).This should be followed by a word ending in –ing (.*ing), followed tightly byAVP 14 . In ordertogetasatisfactorynumberofresults,theNPisnotrestricted(canbeanynoun, includingpluralnouns).

Hits:200

Querylc,[tag="POS|DPS"][word=".*ing"][tag="AVP"][]{0,3}[tag="NN."]

2) “POSS VERB-ing DP” without particle shift and pronominal object – ing -Prt-

OP:

Similarlytothefirstnominalizedstructure,thisconstructionmustalsocontainthesame elements, differing in object which is pronominal in this case. Hence, PNP follows tightlyAVP:

14 Thefactthat“POSSVERBing DP”typeofnominalizationcanbemodifiedwithanadverbwastaken intoconsideration.Thispossibilitywasprovedbutitbroughtzeroresults.Therefore,asimplerquerywas submittedintheBNCinordertofindasmuchrelevantresultsaspossible. 43 Hits:7

Querylc,[tag="POS|DPS"][word=".*ing"][tag="AVP"][tag="PNP"]

3) “POSS VERB-ing DP” with particle shift and nominal object – ing-ON-Prt:

Inthisquerywearelookingfornominalizationwithparticleshiftandnominalobject.

Therefore,anNPfollows –ing andprecedesAVP.

Hits:26

Querylc,[tag="POS|DPS"][]{0,1}[word=".*ing"][]{0,2}[tag="NN."][tag="AVP"]

4) “POSS VERB-ing DP” with particle shift and pronominal object – ing -OP-Prt:

Thisstructureissimilarasthatinthepreviousquerybutapronominalobjectafter–ing nominalizationisdemanded,followedbyAVP.

Hits:68

Querylc,[tag="POS|DPS"][]{0,1}[word=".*ing"][]{0,1}[tag="PNP"][tag="AVP"]

The anticipation of the rareness of nominalizations with particles was correct.

Therefore,thereareofferedallconcreteresultsofthesearchintheBNC:

1) ing -Prt-ON

− relevantresults:36

− listofallrelevantphrasalverbs(numbershowsoccurrence):

actout,bringin,butterup,buyout,carryon 2,churnout,digup,fitout,flagdown,

44 giveup 4,handin,hangover,hushup,mopup,pickup,phaseout,putdown,put

on,putup,setout,setup 2,sumup 3,takeover 2,takeup 3,tearup,turnup

− themostfrequentparticles: up 20 , out 6,i.e. out and up 72 % ofallrelevantresults

− themostfrequentverb: take (+ over 2,up 3)

− themostfrequentphrasalverb: giveup 4, sumup 3, takeup 3

2) ing -Prt-OP

− relevantresults:0

3) ing-ON-Prt

− relevantresults:0

− aninterestingthingthatIhavetomentionisthefact,thatthequeryfoundastructure

his letting theside down

This phrase was checked up in www.thefreedictionary.com and it was found out that this is an idiomatic phrase where there is no possibility of object movement and therefore,cannotbetakenasarelevantresult.

4) ing-OP-Prt

− relevantresults: 11

− listofallrelevantphrasalverbs(numbershowsoccurrence):

buyback,handin,h andout,lookup 2,plugin,putup,shutout,takein,throwout,

urgeon

45 − themostfrequentparticles: in 3,out 3,up 3

− themostfrequentverb: hand(+in,+out ), lookup 2

− themostfrequentphrasalverb:lookup 2

3.3.3. ADDITIONAL RESEARCHES

3.3.3.1. “LONG” PRONOUNS

After an interesting and valuable discussion with professor Petr Karlík (Masaryk

University,Brno)andprofessorNorbertNübler(UniversityinKiel,Germany),Ihave decidedtodoanadditionalresearchconcerningindefiniteandrather“long”pronouns

(PNI; e.g. somebody , anything , none etc.) as object. This leads to an interesting observation,thattheuseof“long”pronounsisverycommoninstructureswithphrasal verbs (the BNC found over 900 results for phrasal verbs without particle shift and approximately 1,500 results for phrasal verbs with particle shift). On the other hand, therearenorelevantresultsfor“POSSVERBing DP”withparticle(neitherwithout particleshift,norwiththeshift).

3.3.3.2. ADDITIONAL RESEARCH: “POSS VERB-ing DP” WITH PRT SHIFT

The results of “POSS VERBing DP” nominalizations with particle shift did not confirmtheassumptionthatsuchtypeofnominalizationcanbeformedfromthemost frequentphrasalverbs.Thereasonforthatisprobablytherestrictionoftheresearchof phrasalverbsintheBNC(theyhadtoberestrictedbecauseofthehighnumberoffound results which would be impossible to check one by one). Therefore, this additional

46 research of phrasal verbs which formed “POSSVERBing DP” nominalizations with particleshiftisadded,inordertoprovethattheseverbsarealsofrequent.Itshouldbe pointed out, that only pronominal object was taken into consideration.The verbs are orderedfromthemostfrequenttotheleastfrequent:

Take in lc,[lemma="take"][tag="PNP"][word="in"]

Relevantresults: 614

Put up lc,[lemma="put"][tag="PNP"][word="up"] results: 255

Look up lc,[lemma="look"][tag="PNP"][word="up"] results : 206

Throw out lc,[lemma="throw"][tag="PNP"][word="out"] results : 180

Hand in lc,[lemma="hand"][tag="PNP"][word="in"] results : 68

Plug in lc,[lemma="plug"][tag="PNP"][word="in"] 47 results : 59

Shut out lc,[lemma="shut"][tag="PNP"][word="out"] results : 46

Buy back lc,[lemma="buy"][tag="PNP"][word="back"] results: 39

Hand out lc,[lemma="hand"][tag="PNP"][word="out"] results:33

Urge on lc,[lemma="urge"][tag="PNP"][word="on"] results: 23

3.4. CONCLUSION

Inthepreviouschapterwehavelearntthatphrasalverbsaremorefrequentin

English language than “POSS VERBing DP” formed from phrasal verbs. The frequencyofphrasalverbswasoftensthousands,whereastherewereonlytens“POSS

VERBing DP”formedfromphrasalverbs.Forabetterlucidity,thereareagainshown theeightgoalsoftheresearchwithaddedresultsfromtheBNC:

48 phrasalverbs withoutPrtshift object=noun 2,832

object=pronoun 5

withPrtshift object=noun 220

object=pronoun 1,427

“POSSVERBing DP” withoutPrtshift object=noun 36

object=pronoun 0

withPrtshift object=noun 0

object=pronoun 11

Asforphrasalverbs,theBNCfoundmoreverbswithoutparticleshift(witha nominalobject 2,832 results whichwereonlyarandomsampleoftensthousandsof found results, compared with 5 results with pronominal object) than verbs without particle shift where the most frequent object was pronominal with 1,427 results , comparedwithverbswithnominalobjectand 220 results .Asfarasthefrequencyof nominalized structures is concerned, there exist only structures without particle shift accompaniedbyanominalobject( 36 results )andstructureswithparticleshiftanda pronominalobject( 11 results ).TheBNCdidnotextract any “POSSVERBing DP” withoutparticleshiftandwithpronominalobject,nordiditfind any “POSSVERBing

DP”withparticleshiftandnominalobject.

As regards to the individual elements of the above mentioned structures, the mostfrequentverbsofphrasalverbsdifferfromthemostfrequentnominalizedverbs.

Nevertheless,theadditionalresearchcouldshowthateventhemostnumerousphrasal verbs forming “POSSVERBing DP” appear in the BNC in a high number (e.g. the

49 occurrenceof takein asaverbis 614 ).Bothphrasalverbswith/withoutparticleshift and“POSSVERBing DP”formedfromphrasalverbswith/withouttheshiftinclinedto beaccompaniedbyrathershortparticles:i.e. up (occurrencewithphrasalverbs: 1,631 out of 4484 – i.e. 36 % – of total number of particles; “POSS VERBing DP”: 23 resultsoutof47totalresults,i.e. 49 %), and out (1,111 outof4484–25 %forverbs;

“POSSVERBing DP”: 9outof47,i.e. 19 %).Asforthechoiceofobject,anominal object is preferred to pronominal object in verbal and nominalized structures without particleshift,whereasapronominalobjectismorelikelytoaccompanystructureswith the shift of particle. On the other hand, the choice of the “length”, as well as the concreteness of a pronoun as an object is also important – longer and indefinite pronounsaccompanyphrasalverbsfrequently,whereastherewerenosuchpronounsas objectfor“POSSVERBing DP”withparticles.

Onthebasisofempiricallycontrollableprocessesrealizedabove,aswellasfrom thesimilaritiesbetweenphrasalverbsand“POSSVERBing DP”nominalizations(i.e. choice of particles and object) we can conclude that “POSS VERBing DP” nominalizationsinheritsomeofthecharacteristicsofverbsandtherefore,thisapproves theassertionoftheabovementionedlinguists,that“POSSVERBing DP”istheresult ofinflection,inlieuofderivation.

50 4. WORKS USED AND CITED

BritishNationalCorpus .

Chomsky, N. (1970). Remarks on Nominalization. In R. Jacobs and P. Rosenbaum,

ReadingsinEnglishTransformationalGrammar (pp.184221) .Waltham(MA):

Ginn.

Dušková, L. (1994). Mluvnice současné angličtiny: na pozadí češtiny . Praha :

Academia.

Harley,H.&NoyerR.(1998).Mixednominalizations,shortverbmovementandobject

shiftinEnglish. ProceedingsofNELS,28.

Huddleston, R. and Pullum G. K. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English

Language .Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Lees,R.(1968). TheGrammarofEnglishNominalisations. TheHague:Mouton.

Marantz,A. (1997). No escape from syntax: Don’t try morphological analysis in the

privacyofyourownlexicon. PennWorkingPapersinLinguistics,4.2 ,201225.

McArthur, T. (Ed.). (1992). The Oxford Companion to the English Language . New

York:OxfordUniversityPress.

Quirk,Randolph,etal.(1985). AComprehensiveGrammaroftheEnglishLanguage .

London:Longman.

Sinclair, J., Hanks P.& Moon R. et al (Eds.). (1990). Collins Cobuild Dictionary of

PhrasalVerbs. Glasgow:Collins.

51