FREQUENCY EFFECTS on ESL COMPOSITIONAL MULTI-WORD SEQUENCE PROCESSING by Sarut Supasiraprapa a DISSERTATION Submitted to Michiga

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

FREQUENCY EFFECTS on ESL COMPOSITIONAL MULTI-WORD SEQUENCE PROCESSING by Sarut Supasiraprapa a DISSERTATION Submitted to Michiga FREQUENCY EFFECTS ON ESL COMPOSITIONAL MULTI-WORD SEQUENCE PROCESSING By Sarut Supasiraprapa A DISSERTATION Submitted to Michigan State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Second Language Studies – Doctor of Philosophy 2017 ABSTRACT FREQUENCY EFFECTS ON ESL COMPOSITIONAL MULTI-WORD SEQUENCE PROCESSING By Sarut Supasiraprapa The current study investigated whether adult native English speakers and English- as-a-second-language (ESL) learners exhibit sensitivity to compositional English multi- word sequences, which have a meaning derivable from word parts (e.g., don’t have to worry as opposed to sequences like He left the US for good, where for good cannot be taken apart to derive its meaning). In the current study, a multi-word sequence specifically referred to a word sequence beyond the bigram (two-word) level. The investigation was motivated by usage-based approaches to language acquisition, which predict that first (L1) and second (L2) speakers should process more frequent compositional phrases faster than less frequent ones (e.g., Bybee, 2010; Ellis, 2002; Gries & Ellis, 2015). This prediction differs from the prediction in the mainstream generative linguistics theory, according to which frequency effects should be observed from the processing of items stored in the mental lexicon (i.e., bound morphemes, single words, and idioms), but not from compositional phrases (e.g., Prasada & Pinker, 1993; Prasada, Pinker, & Snyder, 1990). The present study constituted the first attempt to investigate frequency effects on multi-word sequences in both language comprehension and production in the same L1 and L2 speakers. The study consisted of two experiments. In the first, participants completed a timed phrasal-decision task, in which they decided whether four-word target phrases were possible English word sequences. This task measured how fast participants receptively process a phrase, with their reaction time being the outcome measure. In the second experiment, the same participants completed an oral elicitation task, in which they saw and orally produced target phrases. The outcome measure was the production durations of the first three words (e.g., don’t have to worry) in the same target phrases used in the first experiment. Participants were a sample of native English speakers (N=51) and ESL learners (N=52) who can be characterized as being proficient enough to study in an English academic environment (mean internet-based TOEFL scores = 95.52, SD = 6.63) and who had lived in the US for 2-3 years (mean = 2.61 years, SD = 0.56). The results from the first experiment suggested phrase frequency effects in both participant groups and countered the proposal that L2 learners cannot retain information about L2 word occurrences in their memory (Wray, 2002). These results support the prediction from usage-based approaches and further corroborate previous proposals that frequency data from large native English corpora should be representative of the regularities of English input that speakers in general are exposed to (Hoey, 2005; Wolter & Gyllstad, 2013). Moreover, the results entail a need for future L1 and L2 psycholinguistics model to accommodate phrase frequency effects. On the other hand, in the second experiment, both participant groups did not exhibit phrase frequency effects. In light of previous similar compositional multi-word sequence production studies (e.g., Bannard & Matthew, 2008; Ellis, Simpson–Vlach, & Maynard, 2008), which had yielded mixed results, and the results from the first experiment, the absence of the effects in the second experiment could have stemmed from cross-study methodological differences, including the type of experimental tasks used to investigate multi-word sequence frequency effects. Copyright by SARUT SUPASIRARPAPA 2017 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The completion of this dissertation was possible due to the support and help from many. First, my gratitude goes to my dissertation committee members. I am thankful to Charlene Polio for her guidance and for always having strong faith in me since I first entered this academic field in 2008. I am also grateful to Aline Godfroid for her detailed and insightful suggestions and for encouraging me to continue pursuing research in this area. My thanks also go to Shawn Loewen, who provided useful advice on the design of my study and statistical analyses. In addition, I thank Susan Gass for her suggestions and for her leadership of the Second Language Studies program, which has brought about several forms of support and opportunities for students, including a summer fellowship which allowed me to carry out this dissertation project in the summer of 2016. Several other professors and friends also made significant contributions to this dissertation. Without Karthik Durvasula, with whom I took a phonology class as an MATESOL student, and Qian Luo from the linguistics department, the speech production experiment in this dissertation would have been a serious practical challenge. My thanks also go to Jens Schmidtke, who was always willing to provide suggestions about the R program no matter where he was, be it the US, Mozambique, Germany, or Jordan. Moreover, Nicole Jess at the MSU Center for Statistical Training and Consulting gave me useful advice about mixed effects regression modeling. I also thank Attakrit Leckcivilize for his statistical insight and the friendship we have formed since high school, as well as Suzanne Wagner and Suzanne Johnston for their respective suggestions on the Linguistics Data Consortium and Superlab. In addition, Stella He and several of my Thai v friends at MSU, including Pui, Kwan, Sorrachai, and Supawadee, helped me with participant recruitment. I would also like to thank professors with whom I took classes during my time in the MATESOL and the SLS programs at MSU, including Cristina Schmitt, Debra Hardison, Patti Spinner, and Dianne Larsen–Freeman. The knowledge I gained from them has broadened my academic perspectives, increased my interest in linguistics and language acquisition, and influenced this dissertation, directly or indirectly, in various ways. In addition, I am grateful to many good friends in Thailand for their encouragement and support during the past four years, including Torsak, Gaew, Kwang, Win, Seth, Sa, and Mon. Many people I befriended with at MSU also made my time in the US more fun and meaningful, particularly Ben, Se Hoon, Prare, Big, Yui, Kung, Tim, Pom, Tink, and Aoi. I also thank Sakol Suethanapornkul for his encouragement and inspiring academic determination. On some occasions, it was words of wisdom from these people that gave me the energy to go on. Finally, I thank my parents, Saner Supasiraprapa and Weena Supasiraprapa, and my sister, Sukumal Supasiraprapa, for their unending love, patience, and support. I am also grateful to my uncle, Vithul Liwgasemsarn, for helping my mom and my sister in many ways when my dad passed away in 2013 and I had to be away from home to pursue this doctoral degree. This dissertation was financially supported by the College of Arts and Letters with a Dissertation Completion Fellowship. vi TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES ix LIST OF FIGURES x INTRODUCTION 1 CHAPTER 1: REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 4 1.1 L1 acquisition from a usage-based perspective 4 1.2 Previous work on frequency effects on compositional word sequence 11 processing in native English speakers 1.3 Frequency in L2 acquisition 18 1.4 Previous work on frequency effects on ESL word sequence comprehension 28 1.5 Previous work on the role of frequency in ESL word sequence production 38 1.6 Research questions in the current study 42 CHAPTER 2: EXPERIMENT I 44 2.1 Research question and prediction 44 2.2 Method 45 2.2.1 Participants 45 2.2.2 Materials 48 2.2.2.1 Target phrases 48 2.2.2.2 Fillers 56 2.2.2.3 Background questionnaires 56 2.2.3 Procedure 57 2.3 Analysis 62 2.4 Results 70 2.5 Discussion 75 CHAPTER 3: EXPERIMENT II 84 3.1 Research question and prediction 84 3.2 Method 85 3.2.1 Participants 85 3.2.2 Materials 85 3.2.3 Procedure 86 3.3 Analysis 90 3.4 Results 101 3.5 Discussion 105 CHAPTER 4: GENERAL DISCUSSION 115 CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSION 123 5.1 Summary of findings 123 vii 5.2 Implications 125 5.3 Limitations and future research 128 APPENDICES 136 APPENDIX A: The 28 target pairs 137 APPENDIX B: Frequency (F) of the target phrases and their sub-parts 139 APPENDIX C: Fillers in Experiment I 141 APPENDIX D: Background questionnaire for native English speakers 142 APPENDIX E: Background questionnaire for ESL learners 145 APPENDIX F: Transformation of regression coefficients for result interpretations 148 APPENDIX G: Fillers in Experiment II 150 REFERENCES 151 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 1. Examples constructions at varying levels of complexity and abstraction 7 (Goldberg, 2003, 2013) Table 2. Previous studies investigating frequency effects on ESL compositional 30 phrase comprehension Table 3. ESL learners’ background (N=52) 46 Table 4. Examples of the 28 target pairs 51 Table 5. Explanatory variables in the first experiment 64 Table 6. Average reaction times in milliseconds from the phrasal acceptability 71 judgment task (SD in parentheses) Table 7. Mixed effects model results for the high cut-off bin in the phrasal 73 acceptability task Table 8. Mixed effects model results for the low cut-off bin in the phrasal 75 acceptability task Table 9. Explanatory variables in the second experiment 97 Table 10. Average production durations of the target segments in milliseconds 102 from the elicited production task (SD in parentheses) Table 11. Mixed effects model results for the high cut-off bin in the elicited 103 production task Table 12. Mixed effects model results for the low cut-off bin in the elicited 105 production task Table 13. Target pairs in the high cut-off bin 137 Table 14.
Recommended publications
  • The Field of Phonetics Has Experienced Two
    The field of phonetics has experienced two revolutions in the last century: the advent of the sound spectrograph in the 1950s and the application of computers beginning in the 1970s. Today, advances in digital multimedia, networking and mass storage are promising a third revolution: a movement from the study of small, individual datasets to the analysis of published corpora that are thousands of times larger. These new bodies of data are badly needed, to enable the field of phonetics to develop and test hypotheses across languages and across the many types of individual, social and contextual variation. Allied fields such as sociolinguistics and psycholinguistics ought to benefit even more. However, in contrast to speech technology research, speech science has so far taken relatively little advantage of this opportunity, because access to these resources for phonetics research requires tools and methods that are now incomplete, untested, and inaccessible to most researchers. Our research aims to fill this gap by integrating, adapting and improving techniques developed in speech technology research and database research. The intellectual merit: The most important innovation is robust forced alignment of digital audio with phonetic representations derived from orthographic transcripts, using HMM methods developed for speech recognition technology. Existing forced-alignment techniques must be improved and validated for robust application to phonetics research. There are three basic challenges to be met: orthographic ambiguity; pronunciation variation; and imperfect transcripts (especially the omission of disfluencies). Reliable confidence measures must be developed, so as to allow regions of bad alignment to be identified and eliminated or fixed. Researchers need an easy way to get a believable picture of the distribution of transcription and measurement errors, so as to estimate confidence intervals, and also to determine the extent of any bias that may be introduced.
    [Show full text]
  • (Or, the Raising of Baby Mondegreen) Dissertation
    PRESERVING SUBSEGMENTAL VARIATION IN MODELING WORD SEGMENTATION (OR, THE RAISING OF BABY MONDEGREEN) DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Christopher Anton Rytting, B.A. ***** The Ohio State University 2007 Dissertation Committee: Approved by Dr. Christopher H. Brew, Co-Advisor Dr. Eric Fosler-Lussier, Co-Advisor Co-Advisor Dr. Mary Beckman Dr. Brian Joseph Co-Advisor Graduate Program in Linguistics ABSTRACT Many computational models have been developed to show how infants break apart utterances into words prior to building a vocabulary—the “word segmenta- tion task.” Most models assume that infants, upon hearing an utterance, represent this input as a string of segments. One type of model uses statistical cues calcu- lated from the distribution of segments within the child-directed speech to locate those points most likely to contain word boundaries. However, these models have been tested in relatively few languages, with little attention paid to how different phonological structures may affect the relative effectiveness of particular statistical heuristics. This dissertation addresses this is- sue by comparing the performance of two classes of distribution-based statistical cues on a corpus of Modern Greek, a language with a phonotactic structure signif- icantly different from that of English, and shows how these differences change the relative effectiveness of these cues. Another fundamental issue critically examined in this dissertation is the practice of representing input as a string of segments. Such a representation im- plicitly assumes complete certainty as to the phonemic identity of each segment.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetic Analysis of Speech Corpora
    The Phonetic Analysis of Speech Corpora Jonathan Harrington Institute of Phonetics and Speech Processing Ludwig-Maximilians University of Munich Germany email: [email protected] Wiley-Blackwell 2 Contents Relationship between International and Machine Readable Phonetic Alphabet (Australian English) Relationship between International and Machine Readable Phonetic Alphabet (German) Downloadable speech databases used in this book Preface Notes of downloading software Chapter 1 Using speech corpora in phonetics research 1.0 The place of corpora in the phonetic analysis of speech 1.1 Existing speech corpora for phonetic analysis 1.2 Designing your own corpus 1.2.1 Speakers 1.2.2 Materials 1.2.3 Some further issues in experimental design 1.2.4 Speaking style 1.2.5 Recording setup 1.2.6 Annotation 1.2.7 Some conventions for naming files 1.3 Summary and structure of the book Chapter 2 Some tools for building and querying labelling speech databases 2.0 Overview 2.1 Getting started with existing speech databases 2.2 Interface between Praat and Emu 2.3 Interface to R 2.4 Creating a new speech database: from Praat to Emu to R 2.5 A first look at the template file 2.6 Summary 2.7 Questions Chapter 3 Applying routines for speech signal processing 3.0 Introduction 3.1 Calculating, displaying, and correcting formants 3.2 Reading the formants into R 3.3 Summary 3.4 Questions 3.5 Answers Chapter 4 Querying annotation structures 4.1 The Emu Query Tool, segment tiers and event tiers 4.2 Extending the range of queries: annotations from the same
    [Show full text]
  • Pdf Field Stands on a Separate Tier
    ComputEL-2 Proceedings of the 2nd Workshop on the Use of Computational Methods in the Study of Endangered Languages March 6–7, 2017 Honolulu, Hawai‘i, USA Graphics Standards Support: THE SIGNATURE What is the signature? The signature is a graphic element comprised of two parts—a name- plate (typographic rendition) of the university/campus name and an underscore, accompanied by the UH seal. The UH M ¯anoa signature is shown here as an example. Sys- tem and campus signatures follow. Both vertical and horizontal formats of the signature are provided. The signature may also be used without the seal on communications where the seal cannot be clearly repro- duced, space is limited or there is another compelling reason to omit the seal. How is color incorporated? When using the University of Hawai‘i signature system, only the underscore and seal in its entirety should be used in the specifed two-color scheme. When using the signature alone, only the under- score should appear in its specifed color. Are there restrictions in using the signature? Yes. Do not • alter the signature artwork, colors or font. c 2017 The Association for Computational Linguistics • alter the placement or propor- tion of the seal respective to the nameplate. • stretch, distort or rotate the signature. • box or frame the signature or use over a complexOrder background. copies of this and other ACL proceedings from: Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL) 5 209 N. Eighth Street Stroudsburg, PA 18360 USA Tel: +1-570-476-8006 Fax: +1-570-476-0860 [email protected] ii Preface These proceedings contain the papers presented at the 2nd Workshop on the Use of Computational Methods in the Study of Endangered languages held in Honolulu, March 6–7, 2017.
    [Show full text]
  • Research Methods in Linguistics Edited by Robert J
    Cambridge University Press 978-1-107-01433-6 - Research Methods in Linguistics Edited by Robert J. Podesva and Devyani Sharma Index More information Index 1 Advancement Exclusiveness Law, 416 bit, 171 absolute amplitude, 379 block design, 119 acceptability judgment (also see grammaticality box plot, 298, 299, 330, 331 judgment), 6, 27–47, 68, 102–3, 126, 257, British National Corpus (BNC), 267, 272 307, 308, 320 broker, 201–2, 203 access, 19, 24, 65, 69, 76, 80, 99, 181, 193, 196, Brown Corpus, 264, 265, 266, 269, 270, 295 198, 199–200, 201–4, 224, 229, 239, 480, 500 Buckeye Corpus, 88, 273 acoustic analysis, 3, 5, 6, 58, 171, 289, 396 fricatives, 171, 302, 380, 389, 391, 392 CallHome American English Speech Corpus, 262 laterals, 394 carrier phrase, 88, 175 nasals, 389, 394 carryover effects, 120 place of articulation, 391–2 categorical context, 444 stops, 379–80, 389, 391, 392 categorical variable, 290, 307, 322, 324, 325, trills, 380–1, 389, 391, 392, 393 339, 431 vowels, 171, 376–9, 389, 390–1, 394 categorization, 398, 404, 405–6, 408, 410, 419 aerodynamic measures, 187–8 central tendency (also see mean, median, mode), 45, African American Vernacular English, 103, 446, 288, 295–8, 310, 328–35 471, 475, 476 children, 3, 17, 20–1, 89–90, 131, 394, 505 age, 17, 68, 75, 78, 79, 81, 89, 90, 98, 99, 104, 118, chi-squared test, 311–13, 327 121, 122, 123, 260, 261, 263, 271, 288, 349, CLAN, 249, 250 452, 496, 498, 499, 501, 502, 503, 507, 511 classification, 338, 361–7 age grading, 66, 502, 503, 505, 507, 508, 509 clipping, 172, 175 agent-based
    [Show full text]
  • Research Methods in Linguistics
    Research Methods in Linguistics A comprehensive guide to conducting research projects in linguistics, this book provides a complete training in state-of-the-art data collection, processing, and analysis techniques. The book follows the structure of a research project, guiding the reader through the steps involved in collecting and processing data, and providing a solid foundation for linguistic analysis. All major research methods are covered, each by a leading expert. Rather than focusing on narrow specializations, the text fosters inter- disciplinarity, with many chapters focusing on shared methods such as sampling, experimental design, transcription, and constructing an argu- ment. Highly practical, the book offers helpful tips on how and where to get started, depending on the nature of the research question. The only book that covers the full range of methods used across the field, this student- friendly text is also a helpful reference source for the more experienced researcher and current practitioner. robert j. podesva is an Assistant Professor in the Department of Linguistics at Stanford University. devyani sharma is a Senior Lecturer in Linguistics at Queen Mary University of London. Research Methods in Linguistics edited by ROBERT J. PODESVA Stanford University and DEVYANI SHARMA Queen Mary University of London University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York Cambridge University Press is part of the University of Cambridge. It furthers the University’s mission by disseminating knowledge in the pursuit of education, learning, and research at the highest international levels of excellence. www.cambridge.org Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781107696358 © Cambridge University Press 2013 This publication is in copyright.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Book
    Adrian-Horia Dediu Carlos Martín-Vide Klára Vicsi (Eds.) Statistical Language LNAI 9449 and Speech Processing Third International Conference, SLSP 2015 Budapest, Hungary, November 24–26, 2015 Proceedings 123 Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence 9449 Subseries of Lecture Notes in Computer Science LNAI Series Editors Randy Goebel University of Alberta, Edmonton, Canada Yuzuru Tanaka Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan Wolfgang Wahlster DFKI and Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany LNAI Founding Series Editor Joerg Siekmann DFKI and Saarland University, Saarbrücken, Germany More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/1244 Adrian-Horia Dediu • Carlos Martín-Vide Klára Vicsi (Eds.) Statistical Language and Speech Processing Third International Conference, SLSP 2015 Budapest, Hungary, November 24–26, 2015 Proceedings 123 Editors Adrian-Horia Dediu Klára Vicsi Research Group on Mathematical Department of Telecommunications Linguistics and Media Informatics Rovira i Virgili University Budapest University of Technology Tarragona and Economics Spain Budapest Hungary Carlos Martín-Vide Research Group on Mathematical Linguistics Rovira i Virgili University Tarragona Spain ISSN 0302-9743 ISSN 1611-3349 (electronic) Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence ISBN 978-3-319-25788-4 ISBN 978-3-319-25789-1 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-25789-1 Library of Congress Control Number: 2015952770 LNCS Sublibrary: SL7 – Artificial Intelligence Springer Cham Heidelberg New York Dordrecht London © Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015 This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
    [Show full text]
  • Mining a Year of Speech”1
    Mining Years and Years of Speech Final report of the Digging into Data project “Mining a Year of Speech”1 John Coleman Mark Liberman Greg Kochanski Jiahong Yuan Sergio Grau Chris Cieri Ladan Baghai-Ravary Lou Burnard University of Oxford University of Pennsylvania Phonetics Laboratory Linguistic Data Consortium 1. Introduction In the “Mining a Year of Speech” project we assessed the challenges of working with very large digital audio collections of spoken language: an aggregation of large corpora of US and UK English held in the Linguistic Data Consortium (LDC), University of Pennsylvania, and the British Library Sound Archive. The UK English material is entirely from the audio recordings of the Spoken British National Corpus (BNC), which Oxford and the British Library have recently digitized. This is a 7½ million-word2 broad sample of mostly unscripted vernacular speech from many contexts, including meetings, radio phone-ins, oral history recordings, and a substantial portion of naturally-occurring everyday conversations captured by volunteers (Crowdy 1993). The US English material is drawn together from a variety of corpora held by the Linguistic Data Consortium, including conversational and telephone speech, audio books, news broadcasts, US Supreme Court oral arguments, political speeches/statements, and sociolinguistic interviews. Further details are given in section 2, below. Any one of these corpora by itself presents two interconnected challenges: (a) How does a researcher find audio segments containing material of interest? (b) How do providers or curators of spoken audio collections mark them up in order to facilitate searching and browsing? The scale of the corpora we are bringing together presents a third substantial problem: it is not cost-effective or practical to follow the standard model of access to data (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • 39. Jahrestagung Der Deutschen Gesellschaft Für Sprachwissenschaft ---- 08.–10
    0 1 0 1 0 0 1 0 1 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 0 0 1 1 0 1 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 0 A 0 1 0 0 1 1 1 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 1 1 N 1 1 U 0 1 R 0 1 1 0 1 1 1 0 1 0 0 1 0 D 1 0 1 1 1 0 1 F 0 0 1 1 R U 0 O 0 0 A I 1 1 N 1 1 1 R 0 0 1 D 1 0 L 0 1 0 1 R T 1 U 1 R 0 N 1 0 1 F E 0 1 O F O A D 1 N 1 H N L R O 0 U 1 R 0 N H A 0 R T 1 N S 1 F E C M H U H 1 O F O A I D K N F E D K N 1 C M R O 0 H U N L R 0 D A U I H 1 1 0 R T O F O A I 1 N U I H C O F O A I I F E C M N L R G N D K N I D A O R T N N S H R O I O I A I I F E N N N S L R G T H R I H C O N D K N D A O U S C M N P P N U O I E N I F N D K O U R G O I A I H N S C T H R M N D A N S D I E R U N G O R M A I C H A C H L N S P H U N D N F O K O I T & K O D I E R U N G I N F O R M A T I O N L I C H S P R A C H K O D I E R U N G I N F O R M A T I O N S P R A C H L I C H E & & I N F O R M A T I O N K O D I E R U N G S P R A C H L I C H E I N F O R M A T I O N & S P R A C H L I C H E K O D I E R U N G 39.
    [Show full text]
  • Using the Audio Features of Bncweb
    ICAME Journal, Volume 45, 2021, DOI: 10.2478/icame-2021-0004 Better data for more researchers – using the audio features of BNCweb Sebastian Hoffmann and Sabine Arndt-Lappe, Trier University Abstract In spite of the wide agreement among linguists as to the significance of spoken language data, actual speech data have not formed the basis of empirical work on English as much as one would think. The present paper is intended to con- tribute to changing this situation, on a theoretical and on a practical level. On a theoretical level, we discuss different research traditions within (English) lin- guistics. Whereas speech data have become increasingly important in various linguistic disciplines, major corpora of English developed within the corpus-lin- guistic community, carefully sampled to be representative of language usage, are usually restricted to orthographic transcriptions of spoken language. As a result, phonological phenomena have remained conspicuously understudied within traditional corpus linguistics. At the same time, work with current speech corpora often requires a considerable level of specialist knowledge and tailor- made solutions. On a practical level, we present a new feature of BNCweb (Hoffmann et al. 2008), a user-friendly interface to the British National Corpus, which gives users access to audio and phonemic transcriptions of more than five million words of spontaneous speech. With the help of a pilot study on the vari- ability of intrusive r we illustrate the scope of the new possibilities. 1 Introduction The aim of this paper is threefold: First, on a rather basic level, the paper is intended to provide an overview of the functionality of a new feature of BNCweb (Hoffmann et al.
    [Show full text]