Determinants of Reproductive Status and Mate Choice in Captive Colonies of the Naked Mole-Rat, Heterocephalus Glaber

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Determinants of Reproductive Status and Mate Choice in Captive Colonies of the Naked Mole-Rat, Heterocephalus Glaber DETERMINANTS OF REPRODUCTIVE STATUS AND MATE CHOICE IN CAPTIVE COLONIES OF THE NAKED MOLE-RAT, HETEROCEPHALUS GLABER FRANK M. CLARKE A thesis submitted in fulfillment of the requirements of University of London for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy OCTOBER 1998 ProQuest Number: 10609357 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a com plete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. uest ProQuest 10609357 Published by ProQuest LLC(2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States C ode Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106- 1346 I declare that I have conducted the work in this thesis, that I have composed this thesis, that all the quotations and sources of information have been acknowledged and that this work has not previously been accepted in an application for a degree. Frank Clarke Abstract Naked mole-rats are small, fossorial, cooperatively breeding rodents with a high reproductive skew. Wild colonies contain around 80 individuals and reproduction is monopolised by a single female, the 'queen', and one to three males. This study investigates the hormonal, behavioural, and genetic correlates of dominance and breeding status in captive colonies. I examine the relationship between dominance rank, reproductive status, and urinary testosterone and cortisol levels, and try to determine whether physiological and behavioural parameters can be used as predictors of succession by experimentally removing breeders. Additionally, Y-maze choice tests were used to investigate kin recognition and female mate choice. Colony social structure is characterised by a linear dominance hierarchy with male and female rank correlated with body weight, age, and testosterone levels. Rank appears the most important determinant of reproductive status. Queens are generally the highest ranking colony member and breeding males the highest ranking males. Both are succeeded by the next highest ranking individuals. Queen succession results in an increase in body weight of colony members, the reproductive activation of one or more females, and often intense dominance-related aggression by females. In contrast, male-male competition over breeding rights is low. This result is interpreted in terms of 'skew theory' and female mate choice. No evidence for a simple relationship between social status and the 'stress' hormone cortisol was found. Furthermore, cortisol does not appear to be causally implicated in suppression of subordinate reproduction. Females discriminate between conspecifics on the basis of familiarity, not through detection of genetic similarity, and discrimination is dependant on their reproductive status. Whereas queens prefer unfamiliar males as mates, reproductively inactive females fail to discriminate. Preferences are interpreted as inbreeding avoidance. Thus, the genetic relationship of males to queens is an important determinant of reproductive status in males. DEDICATION To my mum, Grace, who encouraged my love of natural history from a very young age, and made all this possible. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This work was supported by a Biotechnology and Biological Research Council studentship reference no. 94316743. The research was carried out at the Institute of Zoology, part of the Zoological Society of London. The B.B.S.R.C. also contributed towards the costs of visiting South Africa, carrying out collaborative research at the University of Cape Town, and of speaking at die Symposium for Southern African Zoology Pretoria 1996. This study was covered by Home Office project license PPL/3897. A very special thanks to Chris for being incredibly patient and understanding supervisor. He needed to be! Also, thanks Chris for inspiring me - that one can have a life and be a scientist! Thanks also to my sub-supervisors, Mike Bruford and Linda Partridge. Mike, your unsympathetic approach to student problems stopped us dwelling on our worries - we know as a student you had it 100 times worse. Thanks Linda for your (very) critical analysis of my first year report, which gave me a kick up the ass and put me on the right track. Not forgetting the mole-rats themselves. I have gained an deep insight into their lives which has proved more fascinating than watching the most outrageous soap opera, and with the same high drama - sex, infidelity, incest, fighting and murder. Many thanks to all my friends, family, and workmates who made life as a Ph.D student tolerable - even enjoyable at times. A special mention goes to the following people. Kofi, A1 and Ulrich for their friendship and supports, and for the endless philosophical discussions about the meaning of life. Tom Kyffin for his friendship and excellent mole-rat illustrations. Fellow workfriends Dada, Rob, Kath Jeffries, and fellow suffering Ph.D. students - Michelle, Tamsin, Trevor and Steve. All the London Techno Soundsystems and free party people who provided a much needed distraction from work. I would also like to thank the following people who for their support and help. Roger Downie and Felicity Huntingford (University of Glasgow), for the use of Zoology departmental facilities. Mandy Donaldson and technicians at medical endocrinology unit, Hammersmith Hospital, for providing me with radioactive label. Jennifer Jarvis (UCT) and Nigel Bennett (Pretoria University) for making my stay in South Africa so enjoyable and productive. Daphne Green for patiently answering many questions on assays (with patently obvious answers) and by risking life and limb by sharing the same laboratory with me. Elizabeth for help with The Observer and SPSS. Mark Beaumont and Stuart Bearshop for statistical advice and all those who read earlier drafts of this thesis. On behalf of the mole-rats I would like to thank the following people. Selwyn Mundy for keeping a roof over their heads and providing them with a burrow system any self respecting rodent would give their incisors for. Mike Lovett and the other animal technicians at the Institute of Zoology for meals on wheels and their expert care and attention. Tony Sainsbury and the other veterinary staff at the Zoological Society of London for excellent medical care. CONTENTS 1. INTRODUCTION 1.1 PHYLOGENY 1 1.2 MORPHOLOGY 1 1.3 ECOLOGY & DEMOGRAPHY 2 1.4 SOCIAL ORGANISATION 3 1.4.1 The eusociality continuum 3 1.4.2 Reproductive division of labour 5 1.4.3 Reproductive suppression 6 1.4.4 Overlap of generations 7 1.4.5 Cooperative care of young 7 1.4.6 Division of labour among helpers 8 1.4.7 Inbreeding and inbreeding avoidance 9 1.4.8 Evolution of eusociality & cooperative breeding in mole-rats 12 1.4.9 Reproductive skew 14 1.5 DETERMINANTS OF REPRODUCTIVE STATUS 18 1.5.1 Dominance rank, size and age 18 1.5.2 Dominance hierarchies 19 1.5.3 Dominance, aggression and androgens 20 1.5.4 Benefits of being dominant 22 1.5.5 Dominance, social stress and reproduction 23 1.6 KIN RECOGNITION 25 1.6.1 Recognition cues 26 1.6.2 Recognition mechanisms 27 1.6.3 MHC and kin recognition 29 1.7 MATE CHOICE 30 1.7.1 Choice for genetic benefits 30 1.7.2 Dissassortative mating in mammals 31 1.8 RESEARCH AIMS 33 2. GENERAL METHODS 2.1 HOUSING AND HUSBANDRY 38 2.2 BEHAVIOURAL RECORDING 40 2.2.1 Sampling method 40 2.2.2 Ethogram 41 2.2.3 Recording method 43 2.3 COLLECTION OF BODY FLUIDS 44 2.3.1 Urine sampling 44 2.3.2 Blood sampling 44 2.4 HORMONE DETERMINATION 45 2.4.1 Creatinine determination 45 2.4.2 Testosterone assay 47 2.4.3 Progesterone assay 50 2.4.4 Cortisol assay 53 2.4.5 Luteinising hormone assay 56 3. HORMONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL CORRELATES OF FEMALE DOMINANCE AND REPRODUCTIVE STATUS 3.1 INTRODUCTION 60 3.2 METHODS AND MATERIALS 62 3.3 RESULTS 65 3.3.1 Pre-queen removal 65 3.3.2 Post-queen removal 68 3.4 DISCUSSION 73 4. HORMONAL AND BEHAVIOURAL CORRELATES OF MALE DOMINANCE AND REPRODUCTIVE STATUS 4.1 INTRODUCTION 77 4.2 METHODS AND MATERIALS 79 4.3 RESULTS 82 4.3.1 Before breeding male removal 82 4.3.2 Breeding male removal (1) 82 4.3.3 Breeding male removal (2) 88 4.4 DISCUSSION 91 5. FUNCTION OF SHOVING 5.1 INTRODUCTION 94 5.2 METHODS AND MATERIALS 96 5.3 RESULTS 98 5.3.1 Before breeder removal 98 5.3.2 After queen removal 106 5.3.3 After breeding male removal 109 5.3.4 Urinary hormone levels 110 5.4 DISCUSSION 111 6. KIN RECOGNITION AND FEMALE MATE CHOICE 6.1 INTRODUCTION 117 6.2 METHODS AND MATERIALS 119 6.3 RESULTS 124 6.3.1 Female reproductive status 124 6.3.2 Odour preference for familiarity 125 6.3.3 Odour preference for relatedness 126 6.3.4 Social preference for familiarity 127 6.3.5 Social preference for relatedness 127 6.3.6 Mate choice 128 6.4 DISCUSSION 131 7. DISPERSAL 7.1 INTRODUCTION 137 7.2 METHODS AND MATERIALS 139 7.3 RESULTS 142 7.3.1 Identification of dispersers 142 7.3.2 Transfer experiment 144 7.4 DISCUSSION 145 8. GENERAL DISCUSSION 147 REFERENCES 157 vii LIST OF FIGURES Typical layout of laboratory burrow systems. 39 Mean body weight of males and females as a function of dominance rank in colony Nl, colony O, and colony NN before queen removal. 66 Female urinary testosterone levels (mean ± s.d.) as a function of dominance rank in colony Nl, colony O, and colony NN before queen removal.
Recommended publications
  • But Should They Be? and What Is a Dominance Hierarchy Anyways?
    Myrmecological News 24 71-81 Online Earlier, for print 2017 Dominance hierarchies are a dominant paradigm in ant ecology (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), but should they be? And what is a dominance hierarchy anyways? Katharine L. STUBLE, Ivan JURIĆ, Xim CERDÁ & Nathan J. SANDERS Abstract There is a long tradition of community ecologists using interspecific dominance hierarchies as a way to explain species coexistence and community structure. However, there is considerable variation in the methods used to construct these hierarchies, how they are quantified, and how they are interpreted. In the study of ant communities, hierarchies are typ- ically based on the outcome of aggressive encounters between species or on bait monopolization. These parameters are converted to rankings using a variety of methods ranging from calculating the proportion of fights won or baits monopolized to minimizing hierarchical reversals. However, we rarely stop to explore how dominance hierarchies relate to the spatial and temporal structure of ant communities, nor do we ask how different ranking methods quantitatively relate to one another. Here, through a review of the literature and new analyses of both published and unpublished data, we highlight some limitations of the use of dominance hierarchies, both in how they are constructed and how they are interpreted. We show that the most commonly used ranking methods can generate variation among hierarchies given the same data and that the results depend on sample size. Moreover, these ranks are not related to resource acquisition, suggest- ing limited ecological implications for dominance hierarchies. These limitations in the construction, analysis, and interpre- tation of dominance hierarchies lead us to suggest it may be time for ant ecologists to move on from dominance hierarchies.
    [Show full text]
  • Dominance and Prestige: Dual Strategies for Navigating Social Hierarchies
    CHAPTER THREE Dominance and Prestige: Dual Strategies for Navigating Social Hierarchies J.K. Maner*,1, C.R. Case*,† *Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, United States †Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States 1Corresponding author: e-mail address: [email protected] Contents 1. Dominance and Prestige as Evolved Strategies for Navigating Social Hierarchies 132 1.1 Social Hierarchies in Evolutionary Perspective 132 1.2 The Motivational Psychology of Social Rank 133 1.3 Dominance 134 1.4 Prestige 137 1.5 Summary 139 2. When Leaders Selfishly Sacrifice Group Goals 141 2.1 Primary Hypotheses 143 2.2 Tactics Dominant Leaders Use to Protect Their Social Rank 145 2.3 From Me vs You to Us vs Them 161 2.4 Summary 162 3. Dual-Strategies Theory: Future Directions and Implications for the Social Psychology of Hierarchy 164 3.1 Identifying Additional Facets of Dominance and Prestige 164 3.2 Additional Moderating Variables 165 3.3 The Pitfalls of Prestige 167 3.4 Rising Through the Ranks 169 3.5 The Psychology of Followership 170 3.6 Sex Differences 172 3.7 Intersections Between Dominance and Prestige and the Broader Social Psychological Literature on Hierarchy 173 4. Conclusion 174 References 175 Abstract The presence of hierarchy is a ubiquitous feature of human social groups. An evolution- ary perspective provides novel insight into the nature of hierarchy, including its causes and consequences. When integrated with theory and data from social psychology, an evolutionary approach provides a conceptual framework for understanding the # Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Volume 54 2016 Elsevier Inc.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Dominance in Childhood and Its Evolutionary Underpinnings: Why It Matters and What We Can Do
    SUPPLEMENT ARTICLE Social Dominance in Childhood and Its Evolutionary Underpinnings: Why It Matters and What We Can Do AUTHOR: Patricia H. Hawley, PhD Bullying is a common and familiar manifestation of power differentials Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas and social hierarchy. Much has been written lately about bullying in KEY WORDS schools, in the workplace, and even in the National Football League. Such aggression, bullying, evolution, social dominance hierarchies are pervasive in nature. They can be subtly, almost imper- Dr Hawley conceptualized and designed the article, drafted and ceptibly, managed (by glances, gestures, or implicit cultural expectations), revised the manuscript, and approved the manuscript as brutally enforced (authoritarian rule, vicious attacks, or explicit edicts), or submitted. anything in between. These power differentials affect our daily behaviorand www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2014-3549D thought processes, are a large source of our psychosocial stress, and doi:10.1542/peds.2014-3549D influence our health and well-being. Accepted for publication Dec 19, 2014 As an evolutionary developmental psychologist focusing on aggression and Address correspondence to Patricia H. Hawley, PhD, 3008 18th St, Box 41071, Lubbock, TX 79409-41071. E-mail: patricia.hawley@ttu. peer relationships in childhood, I present for this article an evolutionary view edu to children’s social functioning as it relates to power differentials. First, 3 PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). common errors in thinking about dominance are dispelled. The discussion Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics next focuses on social dominance in childhood, including how humans FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The author has indicated she has no appear to be prepared to think about and navigate these relationships, how financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Dominance and Reproductive Differentiation Mediated By
    © 2015. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | The Journal of Experimental Biology (2015) 218, 1091-1098 doi:10.1242/jeb.118414 RESEARCH ARTICLE Social dominance and reproductive differentiation mediated by dopaminergic signaling in a queenless ant Yasukazu Okada1,2,*, Ken Sasaki3, Satoshi Miyazaki2,4, Hiroyuki Shimoji2, Kazuki Tsuji5 and Toru Miura2 ABSTRACT (i.e. the unequal sharing of reproductive opportunity) is widespread In social Hymenoptera with no morphological caste, a dominant female in various animal taxa (Sherman et al., 1995; birds, Emlen and becomes an egg layer, whereas subordinates become sterile helpers. Wrege, 1992; mammals, Jarvis, 1981; Keane et al., 1994; Nievergelt The physiological mechanism that links dominance rank and fecundity et al., 2000). In extreme cases, it can result in the reproductive is an essential part of the emergence of sterile females, which reflects division of labor, such as in social insects and naked mole rats the primitive phase of eusociality. Recent studies suggest that (Wilson, 1971; Sherman et al., 1995; Reeve and Keller, 2001). brain biogenic amines are correlated with the ranks in dominance In highly eusocial insects (honeybees, most ants and termites), hierarchy. However, the actual causality between aminergic systems developmental differentiation of morphological caste is the basis of and phenotype (i.e. fecundity and aggressiveness) is largely unknown social organization (Wilson, 1971). By contrast, there are due to the pleiotropic functions of amines (e.g. age-dependent morphologically casteless social insects (some wasps, bumblebees polyethism) and the scarcity of manipulation experiments. To clarify and queenless ants) in which the dominance hierarchy plays a central the causality among dominance ranks, amine levels and phenotypes, role in division of labor.
    [Show full text]
  • Why We Live in Hierarchies: a Quantitative Treatise
    Why we live in hierarchies: a quantitative treatise by Anna Zafeiris1 and Tamás Vicsek1, 2 1Statistical and Biological Physics Research Group of HAS 2Department of Biological Physics, Eötvös University Preprint version Final version to be published by Springer as part of SpringerBriefs in Physics series. July 2017 1 Contents 1 Introduction ........................................................................................................................... 4 1.1 General considerations ................................................................................................... 4 1.2 Motivation ...................................................................................................................... 8 1.3 Hierarchical structures in space and in networks .......................................................... 9 Reference list ..................................................................................................................... 10 2 Definitions and Basic Concepts .......................................................................................... 12 2.1 Describing hierarchical structures ............................................................................... 17 2.1.1 Graphs and networks ............................................................................................ 17 2.1.2 Measuring the level of hierarchy .......................................................................... 19 2.1.3 Classification of hierarchical networks ...............................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Hierarchical Classification by Rank and Kinship in Baboons
    R EPORTS the study period into one “premove” and two “post- 23. T. E. Seeman, B. H. Singer, C. D. Ryff, G. D. Love, L. 34. STATA 8.0, Stata, College Station, TX (2003). move” periods. For Hook’s Group, which was ob- Levy-Storms, Psychosomatic Med. 64, 395 (2002). 35. We thank the Office of the President of Kenya and the served for 9 years before the move and 7 years after 24. W. L. Gardner, S. Gabriel, A. B. Diekman, in Handbook Kenya Wildlife Service for permission to work in Am- the move, we divided the study period into two of Psychophysiology, J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, boseli; the staffs of the National Museums of Kenya, the premove and one postmove periods. We calculated G. G. Berntson, Eds. (Cambridge Univ. Press, New Kenya Wildlife Service, and Amboseli National Park for the value of the composite sociality index for each York, 2000), pp. 643–664. cooperation and assistance; the members of the pasto- female during each time period using the median 25. J. T. Cacioppo et al., Int. J. Psychophysiol. 35, 143 (2000). ralist communities of Amboseli and Longido and the values of the three behavioral measures derived from 26. N. L. Collins, C. Dunkel-Schetter, M. Lobel, S. C. Institute for Primate Research in Nairobi for assistance each time period for each group. We computed the Scrimshaw, J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 1243 (1993). and local sponsorship; R. Mututua, S. Sayialel, P. Mu- adjusted value of relative infant survival as the dif- 27. T. E. Seeman, B.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin-Biased Social Behaviour in Wild Adult Female White-Faced Capuchins, Cebus Capucinus
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2008, 76, 187e199 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.020 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Kin-biased social behaviour in wild adult female white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus SUSAN PERRY*†,JOSEPHH.MANSON*†,LAURAMUNIZ†, JULIE GROS-LOUIS‡ &LINDAVIGILANT† *Department of Anthropology and Center for Behavior, Evolution and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles yMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology zDepartment of Psychology, Indiana University (Received 16 October 2007; initial acceptance 30 October 2007; final acceptance 4 January 2008; published online 27 May 2008; MS. number: A10889R) Studies of kin bias in the distribution of social behaviour in group-living matrifocal species generally underline the importance of bonds among female kin. However, few studies examine either how kin bias may be affected by variation in the availability of kin or the relevance of paternal kin. In this study, we used genetic and behavioural data to analyse correlates of coalition formation, proximity, grooming and dominance relations among female white-faced capuchins over a 10-year period during which the number of adult females in the group varied from 6 to 10. Females sided with the most closely related of two opponents when joining coalitions. Both dominance rank and kinship influenced proximity and grooming patterns. In particular, when group size was small, mean relatedness high and interdyadic var- iation in relatedness low, rank distance was a better predictor of proximity and grooming than was kinship distance. However, when group size was large, mean relatedness lower and interdyadic variation in relat- edness higher, females significantly biased their grooming and spatial proximity towards kin.
    [Show full text]
  • Lord of the Flies: an Ethological Study of Dominance Rankorderings Bed
    0 -DOCUMENT RESUME ED 114 751 CG 010:212 AUTHOR . Williams, Richard C."' TITLE Lord of the Flies: An Ethological Study of Dominance Ordering in a Group .of Human .Adolescents. PUB. :DATTI 1pr'75, . NOTE tr17p.; Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society-for Research in Child Development (Denver, Colorado, April 10-1'3, 1975) - .EDRS, PRICE MF-$0.76 HC -$1.58 Plus Postage DESCRIPTORS '-*AdoleScents;.*Behavior Patterns; *Group Structure; Organization ;Peer Relationship; *Power Structure; Research Methodology; *Sociometric Techniques; Summer, Programs; Teenagers . IDEN'TIFIERS Dominance; *Ethology, ABSTRACT A stable, ordered dominance hierarchy was found via observational.and sociometr,ic methods for a group of 13-:year-old boyS during a five-week summer camp. This group ,structure was formed early in camp and was stable across settings, time, and types ofdominance interactions. The hierarchy correlated significantly with the rankorderings bed position and hiking position and highly, but not significantly, with athletic ability,physicalfitnessi chronologiCal ge*andlate popularity. Group characteristics and individual differenCeSThre-noted, especially in.regard .to the alpha and omega individuals. The dominifde-tierarchy appears to serve in the reductibll of antagonistic behaviors and, on an individual level, to provide knowledge of. where ones place is among one peers. (Author) ft *****************************4:***************m*******4;********:45****** * Documents acquired by ERIC include many .informalunpublished.' * *.thaterials not available from other sources. ERIC makes everyeffort * * to obtain the best copy available Nevertheless, items o marginal * * reproducibility are often encountered andthis affects 4.1te quality * * of the microfiche and hardcopyreproductions:. ERIC makes availble * via the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EDFS) .EDRS is not * responsible for the quality of theoriginal document.
    [Show full text]
  • Tropical Arboreal Ants Form Dominance Hierarchies Over Nesting Resources in Agroecosystems
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/442632; this version posted October 13, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 1 Tropical arboreal ants form dominance hierarchies over nesting resources in 2 agroecosystems. 3 4 Senay Yibarek1§ 5 Stacy M. Philpott2 6 7 1Department of Integrative Biology 8 University of California, Berkeley 9 VLSB 5017, Berkeley, California 94720 10 11 2Environmental Studies Department 12 University of California, Santa Cruz 13 1156 High Street 14 Santa Cruz, CA 95064 15 16 17 18 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/442632; this version posted October 13, 2018. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. 19 Abstract 20 Interspecific dominance hierarchies have been widely reported across animal systems. While 21 some dominant individuals (winners) get to monopolize resources, during dyadic interactions, 22 they can increase their relative fitness as compared to subdominant individuals (losers). In some 23 ant species, dominance hierarchies have been used to explain species coexistence and 24 community structure. However, it remains unclear whether or in what contexts dominance 25 hierarchies occur in tropical ant communities. Furthermore, it can be challenging to infer and 26 quantify reliable dominance hierarchies from observed interactions.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Dominance Hierarchy: Toward a Genetic and Evolutionary Understanding Bruce T
    www.nature.com/cr www.cell-research.com RESEARCH HIGHLIGHT Social dominance hierarchy: toward a genetic and evolutionary understanding Bruce T. Lahn1 Cell Research (2020) 30:560–561; https://doi.org/10.1038/s41422-020-0347-0 In social animals, the formation of dominance hierarchy is behavior.3 Many brain regions have been associated with social essential for maintaining the stability and efficacy of social decision-making including dominance behavior such as the groups. A study by Wang and colleagues employ a combination prefrontal cortex and the amygdala. Several hormones and of comparative genomic and functional approaches to shed neurotransmitters such as testosterone, dopamine and serotonin new light on both the genetic mechanisms and the evolutionary have also been implicated. By contrast, not much is known about histories of dominance behavior. the genetic basis of dominance behavior and how it evolved. Many animals display social behavior of one sort or another, A recent paper in Cell Research by Wang et al. offers a nice ranging from the relatively simple (e.g., food sharing in wolf packs) inroad into the genetics and evolution of dominance behavior.5 to the extremely complex (e.g., the formation of human societies). This study took a somewhat unusual approach. The authors While social behavior is widespread up and down the animal began by performing a whole-genome sequence comparison kingdom, there is a clear evolutionary trend wherein the more across 16 amniotes (12 placental mammals, one marsupial, two 1234567890();,: derived species on the phylogenetic tree such as placental birds and one reptile) to screen for genomic regions exhibiting mammals tend to display more sophistication in their social accelerated evolution in the common ancestral lineage leading behavior.1 This trend reflects the fact that positive Darwinian to placentals.
    [Show full text]
  • DOMINANCE in HUMANS Tian Chen Zeng Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA Tianchen [email protected] Orcid: 0000-0001-6697-8691
    DOMINANCE IN HUMANS Tian Chen Zeng Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA [email protected] ORCid: 0000-0001-6697-8691 Joey T. Cheng Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada [email protected] Joseph Henrich Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA [email protected] ORCid: 0000-0002-5012-0065 ABSTRACT Dominance is the aspect of social hierarchy that arises from agonistic interactions involving actual aggression or threats and intimidation. Accumulating evidence points to its importance in humans and its separation from prestige--an alternate mechanism in which status arises from competence or benefit-generation ability. In this review, we first provide an overview of the theoretical underpinnings of dominance as a concept, as well as some complications regarding the application of this concept to humans, which often shade into arguments that minimise its importance as a determinant of social influence in our species. We then review empirical evidence for its continued importance in human groups, including the effects of dominance rank on measurable outcomes such as social influence and reproductive fitness (independently of prestige), evidence for a specialized dominance psychology, and evidence for gender-specific effects. Finally, because human-specific factors such as norms and coalitions may place bounds on purely coercive status-attainment strategies, we end by considering key situations and contexts that increase the likelihood for dominance status to coexist alongside prestige status within the same individual, including how: 1) institutional power and authority tend to elicit dominance; 2) dominance-enhancing traits can at times generate benefits for others (prestige), and 3) certain dominance cues and ethology may lead to mis-attributions of prestige.
    [Show full text]
  • Food Competition and Linear Dominance Hierarchy Among Female Chimpanzees of the Taï
    P1: IZO International Journal of Primatology [ijop] pp895-ijop-467726 June 27, 2003 20:7 Style file version Nov. 18th, 2002 International Journal of Primatology, Vol. 24, No. 4, August 2003 (C 2003) Food Competition and Linear Dominance Hierarchy among Female Chimpanzees of the Taı¨ National Park Roman M. Wittig1 and Christophe Boesch1 Received April 24, 2002; revision October 15, 2002; accepted January 20, 2003 Dominance rank in female chimpanzees correlates positively with reproduc- tive success. Although a high rank obviously has an advantage for females, clear (linear) hierarchies in female chimpanzees have not been detected. Fol- lowing the predictions of the socio-ecological model, the type of food competi- tion should affect the dominance relationships among females. We investigated food competition and relationships among 11 adult female chimpanzees in the Ta¨ı National Park, Coteˆ d’Ivoire (West Africa). We detected a formal linear dominance hierarchy among the females based on greeting behaviour directed from the subordinate to the dominant female. Females faced contest compe- tition over food, and it increased when either the food was monopolizable or the number of competitors increased. Winning contests over food, but not age, was related to the dominance rank. Affiliative relationships among the females did not help to explain the absence of greetings in some dyads. How- ever comparison post hoc among chimpanzee study sites made differences in the dominance relationships apparent. We discuss them based on social relationships among females, contest competition and predation. The cross- site comparison indicates that the differences in female dominance hierarchies among the chimpanzee study sites are affected by food competition, predation risk and observation time.
    [Show full text]