Emotion Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescent Girls Following an Interpersonal Rejection

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emotion Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescent Girls Following an Interpersonal Rejection Emotion Reactivity and Regulation in Adolescent Girls Following an Interpersonal Rejection Adam Bryant Miller, Mitchell J. Prinstein, Emily Munier, Laura S. Machlin, and Margaret A. Sheridan Abstract ■ Failures in emotion regulation, especially as a result of inter- emotion reactivity and regulation before and after this rejection. personal stress, are implicated as transdiagnostic risk factors for Adolescent girls evidence greater reactivity via higher self- psychopathology. This study examines the effects of an experi- reported emotional intensity and greater amygdala activation mentally timed targeted interpersonal rejection on emotion reac- to negative stimuli immediately after (compared with before) tivity and regulation in typically developing adolescent girls. the rejection. Self-reported emotional intensity differences be- Girls (n = 33, ages 9–16 years, M = 12.47, SD = 2.20) under- fore and after rejection were not observed during regulation went fMRI involving a widely used emotion regulation task. The trials. However, on regulation trials, girls exhibited increased emotion task involves looking at negative stimuli and using cog- prefrontal activation in areas supporting emotion regulation nitive reappraisal strategies to decrease reactions to negative after compared with before the rejection. This study provides stimuli. Participants also engaged in a social evaluation task, evidence that a targeted rejection increases self-report and which leads participants to believe a preselected peer was neural markers of emotion reactivity and that girls increase watching and evaluating the participant. We subsequently told prefrontal activation to regulate emotions after a targeted participants they were rejected by this peer and examined rejection. ■ INTRODUCTION but not whether a targeted rejection affects subsequent Difficulties with emotion regulation are a transdiagnostic emotion regulation and associated neural recruitment in risk mechanism for psychopathology (Aldao, Gee, Reyes, areas underlying emotion reactivity to negative stimuli & Seager, 2016; Eisenberg et al., 2001; Gross & Muñoz, and emotion regulation via cognitive reappraisal. This ap- 1995), and the ability to regulate emotional responses proach, which we pursue here, is critical for understand- to interpersonal stressors is a fundamental skill that sup- ing the impact of targeted interpersonal rejection on ports positive mental health and well-being (Dahl, 2001). psychological health. For adolescents, it is not necessarily Peer relationships are central to adolescent socioemotional the immediate impact of targeted interpersonal rejection development, and interpersonal stress in peer relation- but the subsequent impact of dysregulation associated ships is expected but contributes risk for maladjustment with rejection which drives risk for psychopathology (Prinstein & Giletta, 2016). Lack of emotion regulation dur- (McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, & Hilt, 2009). Importantly, ing interpersonal interactions predicts peer rejection and participants were unaware that the social evaluation and subsequent psychopathology in adolescents (Laceulle, the emotion regulation tasks, described below, were Veenstra, Vollebergh, & Ormel, 2017). During the adoles- linked. We examined whether, after a targeted inter- cent transition, girls, compared with boys, experience more personal rejection, girls (1) became more reactive to internalizing reactions to interpersonal stress (Rose & negative stimuli and (2) evidenced altered emotion reg- Rudolph, 2006), especially targeted interpersonal rejection ulationinanunrelatedtask. (i.e., overt social rejection; Slavich & Irwin, 2014; Slavich, fMRI studies with adolescent samples consistently im- Tartter, Brennan, & Hammen, 2014). Thus, adolescent plicate the amygdala as a marker of emotional reactivity girls must skillfully apply emotion regulation following to negative stimuli (Silvers et al., 2015, 2017a), with female interpersonal stress. adolescents showing stronger reactivity than male ado- Previous research has investigated the neural corre- lescents (Stevens & Hamann, 2012). Cognitive reappraisal lates of emotion reactivity and regulation (Silvers, Shu, represents a form of emotion regulation that decreases Hubbard, Weber, & Ochsner, 2015; Buhle et al., 2014), negative emotions (Gross, 1998) and reduces biological reactivity to negative stimuli (Buhle et al., 2014; Ochsner et al., 2004; Gross, 1998). Cognitive reappraisal involves University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill reinterpreting the content of an evocative stimulus to © 2018 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 31:2, pp. 249–261 doi:10.1162/jocn_a_01351 reduce emotional intensity. The neural substrates of cog- little effect on neural recruitment during either reactivity nitive reappraisal are well characterized in both typical or regulation trials. As we review above, adolescent girls (Buhle et al., 2014; McRae et al., 2012; Silvers et al., have stronger emotion dysregulation reactions to inter- 2012) and atypical samples (Picó-Pérez, Radua, Steward, personal rejection. Thus, in the current study, we expect Menchón, & Soriano-Mas, 2017; McLaughlin, Peverill, different effects—both because the effects of a targeted re- Gold, Alves, & Sheridan, 2015). In brief, meta-analytic ev- jection may be stronger than general interpersonal stress idence suggests that cognitive reappraisal reliably activates and because we assess these processes in adolescents. cognitive control regions, including the dorsomedial PFC, Given previous work documenting increased amygdala dorsolateral PFC, ventrolateral PFC (vlPFC), and posterior activation in the context of negative emotion reactivity parietal lobe in the service of modulating subcortical acti- (Silvers et al., 2017a; Buhle et al., 2014), we hypothesized vation to negative stimuli (Buhle et al., 2014). The vlPFC in that girls would exhibit a temporary increase in amygdala particular plays an important role in cognitive reappraisal activation when passively viewing negative stimuli after a due to its facilitation of response selection and inhibition targeted rejection. Based on previous work demonstrat- (Braunstein, Gross, & Ochsner, 2017; Buhle et al., 2014; ing that typically developing children and adolescents are Wager, Davidson, Hughes, Lindquist, & Ochsner, 2008). able to effectively use cognitive reappraisal techniques to However, we have no information about how a targeted down-regulate emotion reactivity (Buhle et al., 2014), rejection affects adolescents’ ability to engage cognitive we hypothesized that these typically developing girls reappraisal to downregulate emotional reactivity. would effectively regulate their emotional responses to Research from the peer literature distinguishes tar- negative stimuli after targeted rejection. However, as in geted interpersonal rejections from social exclusion, Shermohammed and colleagues (2017), we expected suggesting that targeted rejections may have more this regulation to be accompanied by a temporary in- immediate effects on adolescents’ subsequent ability to crease in recruitment in frontal control regions implicated regulate emotions (Prinstein & Giletta, 2016). Previous in cognitive reappraisal during these attempts. Specif- research has investigated neural correlates of social ex- ically, we expected the vlPFC to be recruited to a greater clusion (Fowler, Miernicki, Rudolph, & Telzer, 2017; extent immediately after targeted rejection. The vlPFC is van der Meulen et al., 2017; Chester & DeWall, 2014; commonly observed during cognitive reappraisal, and Bolling et al., 2011; Masten et al., 2011; Sebastian et al., activation of the vlPFC and vlPFC/amygdala functional 2011; Somerville, Heatherton, & Kelley, 2006; Eisenberger, connectivity is associated with the degree of reduction in Lieberman, & Williams, 2003) and social evaluation self-reported emotion after reappraisal (Braunstein et al., (Somerville et al., 2013). Many of these studies use the 2017; Silvers et al., 2017a, 2017b; Buhle et al., 2014). Cyberball task (van der Meulen et al., 2017; Fowler et al., 2017; Chester & DeWall, 2014; Bolling et al., 2011; Sebastian et al., 2011; Eisenberger et al., 2003), chatroom tasks, or METHODS similar (Silk et al., 2013; Guyer et al., 2008) to elicit feelings Participants of social exclusion. Somerville and colleagues (2013) Participants included 33 girls (ages 9–16 years, M = developed a task where adolescents are lead to believe 12.47, SD = 2.20, 81% 9–14 years) who completed a base- that a potential peer is watching them to elicit feelings line assessment and participated in the scanning proce- of social evaluation. Broadly, these tasks have been shown dures described below. Participants identified as black to activate the vlPFC (Sebastian et al., 2011; Guyer et al., (n = 14, 42.4%), Asian (n =1,3%),white(n =15, 2008), the medial PFC (mPFC) (Somerville et al., 2013), 45.5%), and mixed race (n = 2, 6.1%). One participant and the bilateral amygdala (Silk et al., 2013; Guyer et al., preferred not to answer. Three individuals (9.7%) identi- 2008). Although these tasks elicit a temporary negative or fied as Hispanic or Latinx. Participants were recruited heightened emotional state, there is little information from a variety of sources, including Internet ads, flyers, about how this temporary emotional state affects near term listserv e-mails, and medical chart reviews. Exclusion cri- emotions and related neural activation to negative stimuli teria
Recommended publications
  • Dual-Process Theories in Social Cognitive Neuroscience
    This article was originally published in Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, published by Elsevier, and the attached copy is provided by Elsevier for the author's benefit and for the benefit of the author's institution, for non-commercial research and educational use including without limitation use in instruction at your institution, sending it to specific colleagues who you know, and providing a copy to your institution’s administrator. All other uses, reproduction and distribution, including without limitation commercial reprints, selling or licensing copies or access, or posting on open internet sites, your personal or institution’s website or repository, are prohibited. For exceptions, permission may be sought for such use through Elsevier's permissions site at: http://www.elsevier.com/locate/permissionusematerial Spunt R.P. (2015) Dual-Process Theories in Social Cognitive Neuroscience. In: Arthur W. Toga, editor. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, vol. 3, pp. 211-215. Academic Press: Elsevier. Author's personal copy Dual-Process Theories in Social Cognitive Neuroscience RP Spunt, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA ã 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Glossary Efficiency The extent to which a process can be executed Awareness The extent to which a subject has conscious quickly and in the absence of attention. access to the stimulus that initiates a process, the operation Intentionality The extent to the initiation of a process is of the process itself, and/or the output of the process. subject to voluntary. Controllability The extent to which the course of a process can be altered (i.e., modulated or terminated) after it has begun.
    [Show full text]
  • Do Conscious Thoughts Cause Behavior?
    Annual Review of Psychology, in press for 2011 Do Conscious Thoughts Cause Behavior? Roy F. Baumeister1, E. J. Masicampo1, and Kathleen D. Vohs2 1Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306; email: [email protected], [email protected] 2Marketing Department, Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota 55455; email: [email protected] K ey Words consciousness, action, control, automaticity, dual process Abstract Everyday intuitions suggest full conscious control of behavior, but evidence of unconscious causation and automaticity has sustained the contrary view that conscious thought has little or no impact on behavior. We review studies with random assignment to experimental manipulations of conscious thought and behavioral dependent measures. Topics include mental practice and simulation, anticipation, planning, reflection and rehearsal, reasoning, counterproductive effects, perspective taking, self- affirmation, framing, communication, and overriding automatic responses. The evidence for conscious causation of behavior is profound, extensive, adaptive, multifaceted, and empirically strong. However, conscious causation is often indirect and delayed, and it depends on interplay with unconscious processes. Consciousness seems especially useful for enabling behavior to be shaped by nonpresent factors and by social and cultural information, as well as for dealing with multiple competing options or impulses. It is plausible that almost every human behavior comes from a mixture of conscious and unconscious processing. www.annualreviews.org ± Conscious Thoughts and Behavior 1 Contents PRGHO RI EHKDYLRU WKH\ DVVLJQ ³Qo role for FRQVFLRXVQHVV´ S ). A similarly negative DEFINITIONS AND assessment led Bargh (1997a) to speculate that THEORETICAL ISSUES««««. 3 ³WKHUH XOWLPDWHO\ LV QR IXWXUH IRU FRQVFLRXV MENTAL SIMULATION, processing in accounts of the mind, in the sense of MENTAL 35$&7,&(««««««.
    [Show full text]
  • Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association. Inc. 1996, Vol. 71, No. 2, 230-244 0022-3514/96/$3.00 Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action John A. Bargh, Mark Chen, and Lara Burrows New York University Previous research has shown that trait concepts and stereotypes become active automatically in the presence of relevant behavior or stereotyped-group features. Through the use of the same priming procedures as in previous impression formation research, Experiment l showed that participants whose concept of rudeness was primed interrupted the experimenter more quickly and frequently than did participants primed with polite-related stimuli. In Experiment 2, participants for whom an elderly stereotype was primed walked more slowly down the hallway when leaving the experiment than did control participants, consistent with the content of that stereotype. In Experiment 3, par- ticipants for whom the African American stereotype was primed subliminally reacted with more hostility to a vexatious request of the experimenter. Implications of this automatic behavior priming effect for self-fulfillingprophecies are discussed, as is whether social behavior is necessarilymediated by conscious choice processes. For many years, social psychologists have studied the effects Automaticity in Attitudes and Social Cognition of priming on the individual's subsequent impressions of oth- ers. Priming refers to the incidental activation of knowledge The extent to which one's own thought and behavior are or structures, such as trait concepts and stereotypes, by the current are not under one's own intentional control is a fundamental situational context.
    [Show full text]
  • Procedural and Declarative Memory in Children with Developmental Disorders of Language and Literacy
    Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Medicine 917 Procedural and Declarative Memory in Children with Developmental Disorders of Language and Literacy MARTINA HEDENIUS ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS ISSN 1651-6206 ISBN 978-91-554-8707-2 UPPSALA urn:nbn:se:uu:diva-204245 2013 Dissertation presented at Uppsala University to be publicly examined in Gustavianum, Uppsala, Friday, September 13, 2013 at 13:15 for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Faculty of Medicine). The examination will be conducted in English. Abstract Hedenius, M. 2013. Procedural and Declarative Memory in Children with Developmental Disorders of Language and Literacy. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. Digital Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Medicine 917. 96 pp. Uppsala. ISBN 978-91-554-8707-2. The procedural deficit hypothesis (PDH) posits that a range of language, cognitive and motor impairments associated with specific language impairment (SLI) and developmental dyslexia (DD) may be explained by an underlying domain-general dysfunction of the procedural memory system. In contrast, declarative memory is hypothesized to remain intact and to play a compensatory role in the two disorders. The studies in the present thesis were designed to test this hypothesis. Study I examined non-language procedural memory, specifically implicit sequence learning, in children with SLI. It was shown that children with poor performance on tests of grammar were impaired at consolidation of procedural memory compared to children with normal grammar. These findings support the PDH and are line with previous studies suggesting a link between grammar processing and procedural memory. In Study II, the same implicit sequence learning paradigm was used to test procedural memory in children with DD.
    [Show full text]
  • Ullman-2015.Pdf
    8 THE DECLARATIVE/ PROCEDURAL MODEL A Neurobiologically Motivated Theory of First and Second Language 1 Michael T. Ullman In evolution and biology, previously existing structures and mechanisms are con- stantly being reused for new purposes. For example, fins evolved into limbs, which in turn became wings, while scales were modified into feathers. Reusing structures to solve new problems occurs not only evolutionarily, but also developmentally, as we grow up. For example, reading seems to depend on previously existing brain circuitry that is coopted for this function as we learn to read. It thus seems likely that language should depend at least partly, if not largely, on neurobiological systems that existed prior to language—whether or not those sys- tems have subsequently become further specialized for this domain, either through evolution or development. In this chapter, I focus on long-term memory systems, since most of language must be learned, whether or not aspects of this capacity are innately specified. Specifically, we are interested in whether and how two memory systems, declarative memory and procedural memory, play roles in language. These are arguably the two most important long-term memory systems in the brain in terms of the range of functions and domains that they subserve. The declarative/procedural (DP) model simply posits that these two memory systems play key roles in language in ways that are analogous to the functioning of these systems in other domains. Importantly, these memory systems have been well studied in both animals and humans, and thus are relatively well understood at many levels, including their behavioral, brain, and molecular correlates.
    [Show full text]
  • A Theory of Unconscious Thought Ap Dijksterhuis & Loran F. Nordgren
    UTT -1- A Theory of Unconscious Thought Ap Dijksterhuis & Loran F. Nordgren University of Amsterdam To appear in: Perspectives on Psychological Science Running head: UTT This research was supported by a grant from NWO (016.025.030). Address for correspondence: Ap Dijksterhuis, Social Psychology Program, University of Amsterdam, Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [email protected]. We would like to thank Tanya Chartrand, Pamela Smith, Dan Wegner and Tim Wilson for helpful comments on an earlier draft. UTT -2- Abstract We present a theory about human thought named the Unconscious Thought Theory (UTT). The theory is applicable to decision making, impression formation, attitude formation and change, problem solving, and creativity. It distinguishes between two modes of thought: unconscious and conscious. Unconscious thought and conscious thought have different characteristics and these different characteristics make each mode preferable under different circumstances. For instance, contrary to popular belief, decisions about simple issues can be better tackled by conscious thought, whereas decisions about complex matters can be better approached with unconscious thought. The relation between the theory and decision strategies, and between the theory and intuition, are discussed. We end by discussing caveats and future directions. UTT -3- “One might almost believe that half of our thinking takes place unconsciously…I have familiarized myself with the factual data of a theoretical and practical problem; I do not think about it again, yet often a few days later the answer to the problem will come into my mind entirely from its own accord; the operation which has produced it, however, remains as much a mystery to me as that of an adding-machine: what has occurred is, again, unconscious rumination.” Arthur Schopenhauer Imagine you are looking for a new house.
    [Show full text]
  • Automatic Emotion Regulation Iris B
    Social and Personality Psychology Compass 1 (2007): 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.x BOSPCS0©J1A2o0?2ulo xra2u05.?igctfg1co0r?iuika1nmrO0n-?sdwl1a07a at,a1l 0e tl 2 UTinC/0lAcj0ld. h0K o1Pr0E etmP7ium c5Aebploe1rluiistls-atio9hhtn0ioion a0rRnsgl4i e t©.Lyg2 tu0Pd2l0a0s7t0yio.c70nh 0Bo0llao0cg5ky.xw Ceollm Ppuabslishing Ltd Automatic Emotion Regulation Iris B. Mauss,1* Silvia A. Bunge2 and James J. Gross3 1 University of Denver, 2 University of California, Berkeley 3 Stanford University Abstract How do people effectively regulate their emotional reactions? Why are some people better at this than others? Most prior research has addressed these ques- tions by focusing on deliberate forms of emotion regulation. We argue that this focus has left out an important aspect of emotion regulation, namely, automatic emotion regulation (AER). Our review of the behavioral literature suggests that AER is pervasive in everyday life, and has far-reaching consequences for individuals’ emotions. However, the behavioral literature has yet to address the mechanisms underlying the observed effects. Because it is difficult to directly measure the processes involved in AER, evidence from neuroscientific studies is particularly helpful in addressing these questions. Our review of the neuroscientific literature suggests distinct neural bases for different types of AER, which provides important clues about the cognitive and behavioral processes that might be involved in AER. Remember the last time you were in a hurry but had to wait for the driver in front of you to clumsily parallel park his car? Chances are that in this situation (or others like it), you may have felt like honking or yelling out of the car window.
    [Show full text]
  • Lost in Dissociation: the Main Paradigms in Unconscious Cognition
    1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.04.004 Lost in dissociation: The main paradigms in unconscious cognition. Luis M. Augusto Independent scientist / University of Barcelona (visiting researcher) [at completion of paper] Spain Published in Consciousness and Cognition, 42 (2016), 293-310 Abstract: Contemporary studies in unconscious cognition are essentially founded on dissociation, i.e., on how it dissociates with respect to conscious mental processes and representations. This is claimed to be in so many and diverse ways that one is often lost in dissociation. In order to reduce this state of confusion we here carry out two major tasks: based on the central distinction between cognitive processes and representations, we identify and isolate the main dissociation paradigms; we then critically analyze their key tenets and reported findings. Keywords: Unconscious cognition; the dissociation logic; exclusiveness assumption; exhaustiveness assumption; cognitive formats or levels; representation paradigms; pro- cessing paradigms Luis M. Augusto - Lost in dissociation: The main paradigms in unconscious cognition 2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.04.004 1. Introduction We have long suspected that there is (far) more to our mental life than we are conscious of (see, e.g., Ellenberger, 1970). Evidence abounds that points in this direction: for ex- ample, we often reach conclusions and find solutions to problems without actually being aware of the reasoning processes, and we frequently cannot tell what knowledge basis we draw from when making important decisions and judgments, such as judging faces or investing our hard-won money. Today, we have the scientific means to approach this hypothesis, and investigation into this field now permeates the whole of psychology and cognitive science.
    [Show full text]
  • Automaticity in Social-Cognitive Processes
    Review Feature Review Automaticity in social-cognitive processes John A. Bargh, Kay L. Schwader, Sarah E. Hailey, Rebecca L. Dyer, and Erica J. Boothby Yale University, Department of Psychology, 2 Hillhouse Avenue, New Haven, CT 06520, USA Over the past several years, the concept of automaticity and prejudice in adults (see [2]); instead we devote of higher cognitive processes has permeated nearly all attention to the new emerging research on attitudes and domains of psychological research. In this review, we prejudice in very young children (see the section on devel- highlight insights arising from studies in decision-mak- opment). ing, moral judgments, close relationships, emotional The second major trend in automaticity research has processes, face perception and social judgment, motiva- been the growing recognition that not all higher-level tion and goal pursuit, conformity and behavioral conta- automatic processes are put in place via a process of skill gion, embodied cognition, and the emergence of higher- acquisition (e.g., [3]), in which a mental process starts out level automatic processes in early childhood. Taken as conscious and effortful and only with frequent and together, recent work in these domains demonstrates consistent practice and experience becomes efficient and that automaticity does not result exclusively from a automatic. Early childhood studies and research on em- process of skill acquisition (in which a process always bodied influences have shown how innate processes and begins as a conscious and deliberate one, becoming those acquired in very early childhood (such as concepts capable of automatic operation only with frequent about the physical world and physical experiences) can use) – there are evolved substrates and early childhood exert an automatic, nonconscious influence on the higher learning mechanisms involved as well.
    [Show full text]
  • The Motivational Unconscious
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Previously Published Works Title The motivational unconscious Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/7sq7379r Journal Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 13(5) ISSN 1751-9004 Author Kihlstrom, JF Publication Date 2019-05-01 DOI 10.1111/spc3.12466 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California DOI: 10.1111/spc3.12466 ARTICLE The motivational unconscious John F. Kihlstrom University of California Abstract Correspondence Motives may be said to be unconscious in a variety of ways. John F. Kihlstrom, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, MC 1650, They may be automatically and unconsciously elicited by 2121 Berkeley Way, Berkeley, CA 94720‐ consciously perceptible situational cues; they may be insti- 1650. Email: [email protected] gated by cues that are themselves excluded from conscious awareness, as expressions of implicit perception or memory; Funding information Richard and Rhoda Goldman Fund or the person may be consciously unaware of his or her actual motivational state. The paper reviews the evidence pertaining to all three aspects of unconscious motivation, with emphasis on conceptual and methodological questions that arise in the study of motives which are not accessible to phenomenal awareness or voluntary control but none- theless influence the individual's experience, thought, and action. 1 | THE MOTIVATIONAL UNCONSCIOUS Motives may be defined as internal states which drive, direct, and select behavior which approaches rewards and incentives and avoids threats and punishments (McClelland, 1985; for more complete coverage of motivation, see Ryan, 2012; Shah & Gardner, 2013). Usually we think of motives as conscious mental states, and motivated behavior as consciously directed toward some goal which will satisfy some desire.
    [Show full text]
  • Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action
    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1996. Vol. 71, No. 2. 230-244 0022-3514/96/S3.00 Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action John A. Bargh, Mark Chen, and Lara Burrows New York University Previous research has shown that trait concepts and stereotypes become active automatically in the presence of relevant behavior or stereotyped-group features. Through the use of the same priming procedures as in previous impression formation research, Experiment 1 showed that participants whose concept of rudeness was primed interrupted the experimenter more quickly and frequently than did participants primed with polite-related stimuli. In Experiment 2, participants for whom an elderly stereotype was primed walked more slowly down the hallway when leaving the experiment than did control participants, consistent with the content of that stereotype. In Experiment 3, par- ticipants for whom the African American stereotype was primed subliminally reacted with more hostility to a vexatious request of the experimenter. Implications of this automatic behavior priming effect for self-fulfilling prophecies are discussed, as is whether social behavior is necessarily mediated by conscious choice processes. For many years, social psychologists have studied the effects Automaticity in Attitudes and Social Cognition of priming on the individual's subsequent impressions of oth- ers. Priming refers to the incidental activation of knowledge The extent to which one's own thought and behavior are or structures, such as trait concepts and stereotypes, by the current are not under one's own intentional control is a fundamental situational context.
    [Show full text]
  • Approach to Automaticity
    .!. .'.. 1 ~ A parallel distributed processing i ' approach to automaticity JONATHAN D., COHEN, DAVID SERVAN-SCHREIBER Camegie Mellon University and University of Pittsburgh JAMES L. McCLELLAND Carnegie Mellon University We consider how a particular set of information processing principles, de- veloped within the parallel distributed processing (PDP) framework, can address issues concerning automaticity. These principles include graded, activation-based processing that is subject to attentional modulation; incre- mental, connection-based learning; and interactivity and competition in pro- ~ 0 cessing. We show how simulation models, based on these principles, can account for the major phenomena associated with automaticity, as well as many of those that have been troublesome for more traditional theories. In particular, we show how the PDP framework provides an alternative to the usual, diChotomy between automatic and controlled processing and can ex- plain the relative nature of automaticity as, well as the fact that seemingly automatic processes can be influenced by attention. We also discuss how this framework can provide insight into the role that bidirectional influences play in processing: that is, how attention can influence processing at the same time that processing influences attention. Simulation models of. the Stroop color-word task. and the Eriksen response-competition task are de- scribed that help illustrate the application of the principles to performance in specific behavioral tasks. This special issue surveys current thinking about the concept of au- tomaticity. In this-: article , we consider this issue within the context a set of principles of information processing formulated in the. broad framework of parallel distributed processing (PDP). We will show how these principles make it possible to construct models that capture the major phenomena of automaticity, as well as many findings that have been seen as problematic for the usual dichotomy between automatic and controlled processes.
    [Show full text]