Alignment Typology in Diachronic Perspective

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Alignment Typology in Diachronic Perspective Alignment Typology in Diachronic Perspective Eystein Dahl, UiT – The Arctic University of Norway This workshop sets out to explore the diachronic dimension of alignment typology. Alignment typology is understood in a broad sense, as to include both the basic alignment pattern expressing core arguments of intransitive and transitive predicates and various types of valency- affecting constructions, e.g. causatives, passives, anticausatives/middles, antipassives, impersonals/transimpersonals and A- or P-lability. These two types of phenomena have different diachronic properties. While the basic alignment pattern or, in the case of split alignment, patterns of a given language generally tends to be diachronically stable, the inventory of valency-affecting constructions appear to be somewhat more prone to undergo change. Given that there appears to be a clear diachronic relation between certain types of valency-affecting constructions and certain types of alignment constructions, e.g. passives and ergatives (cf. e.g. Gildea 1997, Dahl 2016), it clearly makes sense to explore basic alignment and valency-affecting constructions together, as they may be regarded as two dimensions of the same area of grammar. Alignment typology has been a central focus of linguistic research at least since the publication of Dixon (1972). A number of studies have been concerned with the emergence of ergative alignment (cf. e.g. Garrett 1990, Gildea 1997, Butt 2001, Dahl 2016) or semantic alignment (cf. e.g. Aldai 2008, Holton 2008). The development of other alignment types, e.g. accusative, neutral or tripartite alignment, have received less attention and, consequently, their synchronic and, especially, their diachronic properties are less well understood. Similar observations pertain to the emergence and development of valency-affecting constructions. Synchronically, there appears to be systematic correlation between certain types of basic alignment and certain types of valency-affecting construction types. For example, it has long been noted that antipassive constructions tend to show up in languages with predominantly ergative alignment (cf. Silverstein 1972), and that languages showing semantic alignment tend not to have passive constructions (cf. e.g. Wichmann 2007). Observations along such lines suggest a strong diachronic correlation between certain basic alignment types and certain types of valency-changing constructions. However, closer examination reveals that such correlations appear to be statistical tendencies at best (cf. e.g. Polinsky 2005, Janic 2016 on the existence of antipassive constructions in accusative languages), a fact suggesting that historical contingency plays an important role in the structuring of alignment systems. Thus the topic of the present workshop bears directly on the relationship between universal and language-specific factors in language change. A systematic study of the synchronic and diachronic dimensions of interactions between basic alignment types and valency-changing constructions is not only highly motivated but may be expected to contribute significant new insights into this area of grammar as well as into diachronic syntax more generally. A central aim of the present workshop is to bring together scholars working on alignment typology and change in different languages and from different theoretical perspectives in order to address research questions including but not necessarily limited to the following: • To what extent are changes in alignment typology unidirectional? • To what extent are particular correlations between certain types of basic alignment constructions and certain kinds of valency-affecting constructions diachronically persistent? • To what extent are some basic alignment types diachronically more stable than others? • To what extent do basic alignment systems with a split between different alignment construction types show different diachronic behavior from typologically consistent basic alignment systems? References: Aldai, Gontzal. 2008. ‘From ergative case marking to semantic case marking: the case of historical Basque’ In Donohue and Wichmann (Eds.). 2008, 197-218. Butt, Miriam, 2001. ‘A reexamination of the accusative to ergative shift in Indo-Aryan.’ In Miriam Butt and Tracy H. King (Eds.) Time Over Matter: Diachronic Perspectives on Morphosyntax . Stanford: CSLI. Publications, 105–141. Dahl, Eystein 2016. ‘The origin and development of the Old Indo-Aryan predicated -tá construction.’ In Eystein Dahl and Krzysztof Stro ński (Eds.) 2016, 63-110. Dahl, Eystein and Krzysztof Stro ński (Eds.). 2016a. Indo-Aryan Ergativity in Typological and Diachronic Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Dahl, Eystein and Krzysztof Stro ński. 2016b. Ergativity in Indo-Aryan and beyond . In Dahl & Stro ński (Eds.), 1-37. Dixon, Robert M.W. 1972. The Dyirbal language of North Queensland . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Donohue, Mark and Søren Wichmann (Eds.). 2008. The Typology of Semantic Alignment . Oxford: Oxford University Press. Garrett, Andrew. 1990. ‘The Origin of NP Split Ergativity’ in Language Vol. 66, No. 2 (Jun., 1990), pp. 261-296. Gildea, Spike. 1997. ‘Evolution of grammatical relations in Cariban: How functional motivation precedes syntactic change.’ In Talmy Givón (Ed.) Grammatical Relations: A Functionalist Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Press, 155-198. Holton, Gary. 2008. ‘The rise and fall of semantic alignment in North Halmahera, Indonesia.’ In Donohue and Wichmann (Eds.) 2008, 252-276. Janic, Katarzyna M. 2016. L’antipassif dans les langues accusatives . Frankfurt a.M.: Peter Lang Verlag. Polinsky, Maria. 2005. ‘Antipassive Constructions.’ In Haspelmath, Martin et al. (Eds.) The World Atlas of Language Structures . Oxford: OUP, 438-439. Silverstein, Michael. 1972. ‘Chinook Jargon: Language Contact and the Problem of Multilevel Generative Systems, part 1.’ Language 48(2):378-406. Wichmann, Søren. 2007. Valency-reduction in event-oriented languages. In Zarina Estrada Fernández, et al. (Eds.), Studies in voice and transitivity (Estudios de voz y transitividad) . München: LINCOM Europa, 33-51. .
Recommended publications
  • SPLIT-ERGATIVITY in HITTITE Petra Goedegebuure (University of Chicago)
    Published in: Zeitschrift für Assyriologie und vorderasiatische Archäologie. Volume 102, Issue 2, Pages 270–303, ISSN (Online) 1613-1150, ISSN (Print) 0084-5299, DOI: 10.1515/za- 2012-0015, January 2013 1 SPLIT-ERGATIVITY IN HITTITE Petra Goedegebuure (University of Chicago) “it is possible that all languages show ergativity on some level” (McGregor 2009, 482) 1. Introduction2 As a highly heterogeneous phenomenon ergativity remains a conundrum for linguistic theory. The ergative case has been treated as a structural case, an inherent/lexical case, or rather as a mix (Butt 2006). Split-ergativity is thought to arise as an epiphenomenon, as ‘collateral damage’ of diachronic change after reinterpretation of passive constructions with instrumentals (Dixon 1994) or through reanalysis of transitive null-subject clauses with inanimate instrumentals (Garrett 1990b). Alternatively, case assignment and therefore also split-ergativity ultimately depends on synchronic structural properties of the clause (Merchant 2006). It has been claimed that only 25% of the world’s languages shows ergativity (Van de Visser 2006), or that “all languages show ergativity on some level” (McGregor 2009, 482). Irrespective of the correct ratio, split-ergativity seems to be the norm among languages that show ergativity. When the ergative split is based on semantic features of noun phrases, it is generally assumed that animacy plays a major role. Silverstein (1976) has shown that pronouns and nouns can be hierarchically arranged based on semantic features such as person, number, or grammatical gender. The strength of this hierarchy is that if agent marking is attested for the first time at a certain point in the hierarchy, all nominals lower in the hierarchy will carry agent marking as well.
    [Show full text]
  • 08-Siewierska 339..370
    C:/3d/J/Trps/Trps 101-2/08-siewierska.3d ± 7/7/3 ± 15:58 ± disk/sr Transactions of the Philological Society Volume 101:2 (2003) 339±370 PERSON AGREEMENT AND THE DETERMINATION OF ALIGNMENT By Anna Siewierska Lancaster University Abstract The paper explores two issues arisingfrom the extension of the notion of alignment from the domain of monotransitive to ditransitive clauses. The ®rst is, To what extent should other than purely formal patterns of identi®cation be taken into account in determiningthe ditransitive counterparts of the respective monotransitive alignments, i.e., accusative, erga- tive, active, etc.? The second issue is, How should con¯icts between the formal criteria used in the determination of the alignment of person agreement be resolved? It is argued that only under a purely formal interpretation of alignment is it possible to discern ditransitive counterparts of all the major monotransitive alignments, and that once the notion of neutral alignment is distinguished from absence of agreement, form/order con¯icts involvingagreement markers can be consistently resolved in favour of phonological form. 1. Introduction The term `alignment' when used in regard to grammatical functions denotes how core grammatical functions are organised relative to each other. Traditionally the major patterns of alignment have been de®ned exclusively in relation to the arguments of intransitive and monotransitive verbs, i.e., in relation to the S (sole argument of intransitive verb), A (agentive argument of transitive verb) and P (patient-like argument of transitive verb), as characterised by Dixon (1972) and Comrie (1978). They are the familiar, neutral, accusative, # The Philological Society 2003.
    [Show full text]
  • Morphosyntactic Alignment, Pattern Replication, and the Classical Armenian Periphrastic Perfect
    1 Morphosyntactic Alignment, Pattern Replication, * 2 and the Classical Armenian Periphrastic Perfect 3 ROBIN MEYER 4 University of Oxford 5 1. Introduction 6 The Classical Armenian periphrastic perfect and its unusual construction have 7 posed difficulties for historical linguists since their first detailed discussion in the 8 1903 edition of Meillet’s Esquisse d’une grammaire comparée de l’arménien 9 classique (see the revised edition, Meillet 1936). While the communis opinio still 10 favors an explanation proffered by Benveniste (1952), which likens the Armenian 11 pattern to the Old Persian taya manā kr̥ tam construction, this model cannot ac- 12 count for several problems in the Armenian data; a new model, informed by the 13 shortcomings of its predecessors and taking into account typological and compar- 14 ative data, is therefore necessary. 15 In this paper it will be argued that the most consistent analysis of the Arme- 16 nian perfect construction must interpret it as a synchronically tripartite alignment 17 pattern, resulting from extensive language contact with the Middle Iranian lan- 18 guages; typological irregularities and synchronically deviant patterns are manifes- 19 tations of alignment change under pressure from the non-perfect tenses. 20 After a brief exposition of the Armenian data and the issues associated with it 21 (§2), previous attempts at an explanation of the phenomenon will be summarized 22 and their weaknesses discussed (§3). The Armenian situation will then be com- 23 pared to that in Middle Iranian, especially Parthian, and their differences and 24 commonalities will be set out (§4). FinallY, a new model will detail why a tripar- 25 tite analysis of the periphrastic perfect is synchronically and typologically appro- 26 priate (§5), how far Iranian influence is the likely origin of this pattern, and what 27 other expressions of such influence can be found in Classical Armenian (§6).
    [Show full text]
  • John Benjamins Publishing Company
    John Benjamins Publishing Company This is a contribution from Studies in Language 41:3 © 2017. John Benjamins Publishing Company This electronic file may not be altered in any way. The author(s) of this article is/are permitted to use this PDF file to generate printed copies to be used by way of offprints, for their personal use only. Permission is granted by the publishers to post this file on a closed server which is accessible only to members (students and faculty) of the author's/s' institute. It is not permitted to post this PDF on the internet, or to share it on sites such as Mendeley, ResearchGate, Academia.edu. Please see our rights policy on https://benjamins.com/content/customers/rights For any other use of this material prior written permission should be obtained from the publishers or through the Copyright Clearance Center (for USA: www.copyright.com). Please contact [email protected] or consult our website: www.benjamins.com Multiple ergatives From allomorphy to differential agent marking Peter Arkadiev Institute of Slavic Studies RAS/RSUH, Moscow This paper presents a cross-linguistic survey of case systems with several non-phonologically distributed markers of ergative case, based on a convenience sample of more than 70 languages from all over the world. It is shown that in most languages the distribution of different ergative markers splits along the lines predicted by the referential hierarchy (local pronouns > non-local pronouns > proper names > kinship terms > humans > nonhumans > inanimates), thus com- plementing the much better known ergativity splits. Other types of conditioning of “multiple ergatives” include gender, such nominal morphosyntactic features as number and (in)definiteness, as well as clausal morphosyntactic features like tense/aspect, polarity and person of co-arguments.
    [Show full text]
  • The Morphology and Syntax of Ergativity
    THE MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX OF ERGATIVITY: A TYPOLOGICAL APPROACH by Justin Rill A dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the University of Delaware in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics Winter 2017 © 2017 Justin Rill All Rights Reserved ProQuest Number:10257594 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. ProQuest 10257594 Published by ProQuest LLC ( 2017). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 THE MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX OF ERGATIVITY: A TYPOLOGICAL APPROACH by Justin Rill Approved: Benjamin Bruening, Ph.D. Chair of the Department of Linguistics and Cognitive Science Approved: George H. Watson, Ph.D. Dean of the College of Arts and Sciences Approved: Ann L. Ardis, Ph.D. Senior Vice Provost for Graduate and Professional Education I certify that I have read this dissertation and that in my opinion it meets the academic and professional standard required by the University as a dis- sertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. Signed: Gabriella Hermon, Ph.D. Professor in charge of dissertation I certify that I have read this dissertation and that in my opinion it meets the academic and professional standard required by the University as a dis- sertation for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Linguistic Typology
    An Introduction to Linguistic Typology An Introduction to Linguistic Typology Viveka Velupillai University of Giessen John Benjamins Publishing Company Amsterdam / Philadelphia TM The paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of 8 the American National Standard for Information Sciences – Permanence of Paper for Printed Library Materials, ansi z39.48-1984. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data An introduction to linguistic typology / Viveka Velupillai. â. p cm. â Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Typology (Linguistics) 2. Linguistic universals. I. Title. P204.V45 â 2012 415--dc23 2012020909 isbn 978 90 272 1198 9 (Hb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 1199 6 (Pb; alk. paper) isbn 978 90 272 7350 5 (Eb) © 2012 – John Benjamins B.V. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. John Benjamins Publishing Company • P.O. Box 36224 • 1020 me Amsterdam • The Netherlands John Benjamins North America • P.O. Box 27519 • Philadelphia PA 19118-0519 • USA V. Velupillai: Introduction to Typology NON-PUBLIC VERSION: PLEASE DO NOT CITE OR DISSEMINATE!! ForFor AlTô VelaVela anchoranchor and and inspiration inspiration 2 Table of contents Acknowledgements xv Abbreviations xvii Abbreviations for sign language names xx Database acronyms xxi Languages cited in chapter 1 xxii 1. Introduction 1 1.1 Fast forward from the past to the present 1 1.2 The purpose of this book 3 1.3 Conventions 5 1.3.1 Some remarks on the languages cited in this book 5 1.3.2 Some remarks on the examples in this book 8 1.4 The structure of this book 10 1.5 Keywords 12 1.6 Exercises 12 Languages cited in chapter 2 14 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Uncommon Patterns of Core Term Marking and Case Terminology
    This is the pre-final version of a paper published in Lingua 119 (2009) 445-459. Please don’t quote without checking the published version before. Uncommon patterns of core term marking and case terminology Denis Creissels Laboratoire Dynamique du Language, ISH, 14 avenue Berthelot 69363 Lyon cedex 07, France E-mail address: [email protected] Abstract Two majors patterns of core term marking are found cross-linguistically: an accusative pattern in which P is the only core syntactic role marked by a case form distinct from the quotation form of nouns or by an adposition, and an ergative pattern in which A is the only core syntactic role marked by a case form distinct from the quotation form of nouns or by an adposition. Current case terminology is adapted to the description of systems consistently following one of these two patterns, but is difficult to extend to core term marking systems characterized by alignment variations. Moreover, several minor patterns are attested, characterized by a wider use of marked case forms or adpositions, and current terminology fails to provide convenient labels for case forms or adpositions occurring in such patterns. It is argued that: (a) in languages in which nouns are inflected for case, the case form coinciding with the quotation form of nouns should be labeled in a uniform way, irrespective of the uses it may have in syntactic constructions; (b) new labels should be coined for marked case forms or adpositions used in S/A or S/P role. Keywords: Case marking; Alignment; Markedness; Nominative; Absolutive; Accusative; Ergative; Marked nominative Abbreviations: ACC, accusative; ANTIACC, antiaccusative (see Section 9.2); ANTIERG, antiergative (see Section 9.2); AOR, aorist; DEF, definite; DEM, demonstrative; DAT, dative; ERG, ergative; F, feminine; GEN, genitive; IPFV, imperfective; M, masculine; N, neuter; NEG, negation; OBL, oblique case; PFV, perfective; PL, plural; POSS, possessive; PRS, present; PST, past; SG, singular; TAM, tense-aspect-modality marker.
    [Show full text]
  • MORPHOLOGICAL SPLIT ERGATIVE ALIGNMENT and SYNTACTIC NOMINATIVE-ACCUSATIVE ALIGNMENT in PESH Claudine Chamoreau
    MORPHOLOGICAL SPLIT ERGATIVE ALIGNMENT AND SYNTACTIC NOMINATIVE-ACCUSATIVE ALIGNMENT IN PESH Claudine Chamoreau To cite this version: Claudine Chamoreau. MORPHOLOGICAL SPLIT ERGATIVE ALIGNMENT AND SYNTACTIC NOMINATIVE-ACCUSATIVE ALIGNMENT IN PESH. International Journal of American Linguis- tics, University of Chicago Press, inPress. halshs-03112558 HAL Id: halshs-03112558 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-03112558 Submitted on 16 Jan 2021 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Chamoreau, C. forthcoming. Morphological split ergative alignment and syntactic nominative–accusative alignment in Pesh. International Journal of American Linguistics. MORPHOLOGICAL SPLIT ERGATIVE ALIGNMENT AND SYNTACTIC NOMINATIVE–ACCUSATIVE ALIGNMENT IN PESH CLAUDINE CHAMOREAU CNRS (CEMCA/SEDYL) ABSTRACT Pesh (Chibchan, Honduras) has until now been described as having a morphological nominative–accusative alignment. This paper argues that Pesh displays a bi-level split ergative pattern for morphological alignment. On the first level of the system, Pesh features a split alignment that is conditioned by the way the arguments are expressed. It has a nominative– accusative alignment for the obligatory indexing of arguments on the verb, and three systems of alignment for flagging case. The distribution of these three systems shapes the second level where ergative–absolutive, tripartite, and nominative–accusative are seen depending on the types of clause and the varieties spoken.
    [Show full text]
  • Alignments Across Tsimshianic∗
    Alignments Across Tsimshianic∗ Tyler Peterson University of Arizona 1 Introduction Dixon (1994) claims that there is no language that manifests a purely ergative case or agreement system: all ergative languages have a split, which is typically conditioned by semantic factors such as animacy or an argument hierarchy, tense, aspect and modality, or syntactic factors such as matrix vs. subordinate environ- ments. Typically, what lies on the other side of this split is a nominative alignment. In this paper I argue that the Tsimshianic languages present us with a case of a language family where the individual languages are almost entirely morpholog- ically ergative in the agreement and pronominal systems. While there is a split in Tsimshianic, conditioned by both clause type and a person hierarchy, the other side of the split is not the expected nominative-accusative alignment. Rather, other ∗Special thanks to my Gitksan consultants Doreen Jensen, Fern Weget, Clara Weget, Gwen Simms, Barbara Sennott, and Louise Wilson. Thanks also to Will Oxford, Jason Brown, Henry Davis, and Bruce Rigsby for their helpful comments and corrections. This research was made possible with grants from the Endangered Language Fund, Jacobs Research Fund (Whatcom Mu- seum Society), and The Endangered Languages Documentation Program (SOAS), awarded to the author. All errors are my own. Examples not cited are from fieldwork. The Tsimshian practical orthography is used: k = [q]; hl and ë = [ì]; g = [G]; x = [X]; a = [@]; ü = [W]. See Rigsby (1986); Tarpent (1987); Stebbins (2003); Brown (2010) for additional background. 1 logical groupings of semantic roles are found. One of the splits groups the Object (O) and Agent (A), or what is called a transitive (or ‘horizontal’) alignment in the Tsimshianic literature.
    [Show full text]
  • Overview of the Alignment of Arawakan Languages Tom Durand
    Overview of the alignment of Arawakan languages Tom Durand To cite this version: Tom Durand. Overview of the alignment of Arawakan languages. Amerindia, Association d’Ethno- linguistique Amérindienne, 2018, 40, pp.135 - 167. hal-01864837 HAL Id: hal-01864837 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01864837 Submitted on 30 Aug 2018 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. AMERINDIA 40: 135-167, 2018 Overview of the alignment of Arawakan languages1 Tom DURAND INALCO/USPC, SeDyL (UMR 8202) Abstract: This article compares the alignments of the languages from the Arawakan family, one of the largest linguistic families of South America; in other words, how these languages encode the arguments of intransitive and transitive predicates. It had been shown that most of these languages are characterized by split intransitivity, more precisely by what will be called nominative-absolutive alignment, where an intransitive predicate takes nominative or absolutive agreement according to semantic, pragmatic or morphosyntactic factors. Two major subtypes are observed. In the first, one set of person markers is lexically assigned to a particular intransitive predicate, while in the second, depending on non-lexical factors, a predicate can accept one set or the other.
    [Show full text]
  • Ergativity and Markedness in Nepali Luke S
    Yale University EliScholar – A Digital Platform for Scholarly Publishing at Yale Linguistics Graduate Dissertations Department of Linguistics Spring 5-20-2019 A Jewel Inlaid: Ergativity and Markedness in Nepali Luke S. Lindemann Yale University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://elischolar.library.yale.edu/ling_graduate Part of the Discourse and Text Linguistics Commons, Language Description and Documentation Commons, Semantics and Pragmatics Commons, and the Syntax Commons Recommended Citation Lindemann, Luke S., "A Jewel Inlaid: Ergativity and Markedness in Nepali" (2019). Linguistics Graduate Dissertations. 4. https://elischolar.library.yale.edu/ling_graduate/4 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Linguistics at EliScholar – A Digital Platform for Scholarly Publishing at Yale. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Graduate Dissertations by an authorized administrator of EliScholar – A Digital Platform for Scholarly Publishing at Yale. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Abstract A Jewel Inlaid: Ergativity and Markedness in Nepali Luke Lindemann 2019 Nepali presents with a complex case marking pattern in which ergative case is obligatory in perfective transitive clauses, disallowed in unaccusative intransitive clauses and copular clauses, and varies with the nominative elsewhere. Where erga- tive marking is variable, its usage correlates with a variety of semantic and pragmatic factors. The purpose of this investigation is to precisely delineate the grammatical domains for which ergative marking is variable and to provide a unified analysis of the semantic and pragmatic factors that correlate with its expression. The study of pragmatic phenomena requires the implementation of multiple strate- gies for collecting language data.
    [Show full text]
  • Word Order Type and Alignment Type*
    Word order type and alignment type* 0. Introduction In her recent stimulating book NICHOLS (1992) suggests that there are no correlations between word order type and morphological alignment, where by alignment is meant the patterns of argument marking in mono-transitive and intransitive clauses, e.g. accusative, ergative, active, tripartite, hierarchical and neutral. Her findings pertain to the relationship between word order type and the dominant alignment type displayed by a language, dominant alignment being an abstraction over the alignments found with nouns, independent pronouns and verbal agreement. Since consistency in the alignment of the three categories is the exception rather than the norm (see p.8), the question arises of whether the lack of a correlation between word order and alignment established on the basis of dominant alignment also holds for the individual alignments of nouns, pronouns and agreement. The relationships between word order type and alignment that have been proposed in the literature include: a) a correlation between non-neutral alignment and SOV order; GREENBERG's (1963:96) universal 41: "if in a language the verb follows both the nominal subject and nominal object as the dominant order, the language almost always has a case system". b) a tendency for neutral alignment of nouns and agreement in SVO order (e.g. LEHMANN 1978; MALLINSON & BLAKE 1981:179)1; c) a preference for non-neutral alignment of nominals and/or agreement in free word order languages (e.g. MALLINSON & BLAKE 1981; STEELE 1978); d) an association between ergative alignment and non-SVO order (e.g. TRASK 1979, DIXON 1979, GARRET 1990); e) an association between ergative alignment and object-before- subject order (e.g.
    [Show full text]