Laboratory Primate Newsletter
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
LABORATORY PRIMATE NEWSLETTER Vol. 50, No. 2 April 2011 JUDITH E. SCHRIER, EDITOR JAMES S. HARPER, GORDON J. HANKINSON AND LARRY HULSEBOS, ASSOCIATE EDITORS MORRIS L. POVAR AND JASON MACHAN, CONSULTING EDITORS ELVA MATHIESEN, ASSISTANT EDITOR ALLAN M. SCHRIER, FOUNDING EDITOR, 1962-1987 Published Quarterly by the Schrier Research Laboratory Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island ISSN 0023-6861 POLICY STATEMENT The Laboratory Primate Newsletter provides a central source of information about nonhuman primates and related matters to scientists who use these animals in their research and those whose work supports such research. The Newsletter (1) provides information on care and breeding of nonhuman primates for laboratory research, (2) disseminates general information and news about the world of primate research (such as announcements of meetings, research projects, sources of information, nomenclature changes), (3) helps meet the special research needs of individual investigators by publishing requests for research material or for information related to specific research problems, and (4) serves the cause of conservation of nonhuman primates by publishing information on that topic. As a rule, research articles or summaries accepted for the Newsletter have some practical implications or provide general information likely to be of interest to investigators in a variety of areas of primate research. However, special consideration will be given to articles containing data on primates not conveniently publishable elsewhere. General descriptions of current research projects on primates will also be welcome. The Newsletter appears quarterly and is intended primarily for persons doing research with nonhuman primates. Back issues may be purchased for $10.00 each. We are no longer printing paper issues, except those we will send to subscribers who have paid in advance. Readers with access to electronic mail may receive a notice when a new issue is put on the Website by sending the message subscribe LPN-WARN your-own-name to [email protected]. (Send the message subscribe LPN-PDF to receive PDF files by e-mail; or the message subscribe LPN-L to receive the nongraphic contents of each issue.) Current and back issues of the Newsletter are available on the World Wide Web at <http://www.brown.edu/primate>. Persons who have absolutely no access to the Web, or to the electronic mailing, may ask to have paper copies sent to them. The publication lag is typically no longer than the three months between issues and can be as short as a few weeks. The deadline for inclusion of a note or article in any given issue of the Newsletter has in practice been somewhat flexible, but is technically the tenth of December, March, June, or September, depending on which issue is scheduled to appear next. Reprints will not be supplied under any circumstances, but authors may reproduce their own articles in any quantity. PREPARATION OF ARTICLES FOR THE NEWSLETTER. – Articles, notes, and announcements may be submitted by mail, e-mail, or computer disk, but a printed copy of manuscripts of any length or complexity should also be sent by regular mail. Articles in the References section should be referred to in the text by author(s) and date of publication, e.g., Smith (1960) or (Smith & Jones, 1962). Names of journals should be spelled out completely in the References section. Latin names of primates should be indicated at least once in each note and article. In general, to avoid inconsistencies within the Newsletter, the Latin names used will be those in Mammal Species of The World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference, 2nd Ed. D. E. Wilson & D. M. Reeder (Eds.). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, 1993. For an introduction to and review of primate nomenclature see The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates, by N. Rowe, Pogonias Press, 1996. All correspondence concerning the Newsletter should be addressed to: Judith E. Schrier, Department of Cognitive, Linguistic, and Psychological Sciences, Box 1853, Brown University Providence, Rhode Island 02912 [401-863-2511; FAX: 401-863-1300] e-mail address: [email protected] Current and back issues of the Newsletter are available on the World Wide Web at http://brown.edu/primate ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The Newsletter is supported by Brown University. Cover photograph of a bonobo (Pan paniscus), three year old Vic, visiting with his friend, a three year old boy in red. Taken at the Cincinnati Zoo and Botanical Gardens by Marian Brickner, June 2, 2006. Copyright © 2011 by Brown University Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 2011, 50[2] Male Monkey Murder During Intergroup Interaction in Free-Ranging Hanuman Langurs, Semnopithecus entellus, Around Jodhpur (Rajasthan) Goutam Sharma, Chena Ram, Prateek Vijay, and L. S. Rajpurohit Animal Behaviour Unit, Department of Zoology, Jai Narain Vyas University, Jodhpur Introduction thy & Mohnot, 1988; Rajpurohit et al., 2003; Sharma et al., 2010), Mount Abu (Hrdy, 1974), Kanha (Newton, The Hanuman langur (Semnopithecus entellus) is the 1986), Tirunelveli (Ross, 1993), and Nepal (Borries, best-studied and most adaptable South Asian colobine. 1997). But in the present paper we describe an unsuc- They live in a wide range of habitats, from deciduous cessful attempted takeover or male replacement. One forest to scrub and open rocky areas (such as Jodhpur), male of an all-male band was killed by focal troop B-1’s and in villages, towns, and on cultivated land (Roonwal & resident male. Mohnot, 1977; Wolfheim, 1983). The two basic types of social groups are bisexual troops and all-male bands. Resident males of bisexual troops show great aggres- Troops are matrilineal groups of adult females and off- siveness, which is an important behavior in this species spring with either one adult male (unimale bisexual troop) during intertroop interactions. Sometimes the result may or more than one adult male (multi-male troops). Around be a great social change, periodically seen in the unimale Jodhpur 99% of the bisexual troops are unimale (Mohnot bisexual troops: a resident may lose his residentship. In- et. al., 1987; Rajpurohit, 1987). teractions between all-male bands are mostly mild and peaceful. When two groups meet, they either sit close or The langur groups usually have foraging and feeding separate without any engagement. The present paper de- grounds, provisioning places, water holes, resting sites, scribes the interaction between a bisexual troop and sev- night lodgings, etc. These places are often shared by two eral members of an all-male band. or more neighboring groups because of overlapping home ranges (Mohnot, 1977). All-male bands have been ob- Interactions between bisexual troops and all-male served to interact with several unimale bisexual groups bands are usually not peaceful: there is much excitement within their extensive nomadic range. The langurs have and aggressive interactions occur. The resident male of a also been observed interacting (grooming and playing) bisexual group starts “whooping”, either at the sight of an with cows, goats, and dogs. The interactions with all- all-male group or when males of the latter give “whoop” male bands may increase conflict and aggression, leading calls from a distance of 1 to 3 kilometers. Then the resi- to temporary changes such as inter-group infant kidnap- dent male leaps, jumps, and goes around the “preferred ping (Mohnot, 1980) or permanent social changes (Moh- area” of his home range, goes from branch to branch, and not, 1971, Makwana, 1979). thumps and shakes horizontal tree branches in excitement, interrupting with vocalizations such as “whoops”, canine Intensity of interactions between bisexual troops and grinding, barking, air-biting, and coughing. The attention all-male bands differs from location to location in this of females is diverted toward these acts of their leader. At habitat. In some locations, such as the Mandore Garden, that time young male infants continuously produce Daijar, Bhadershawer, and Bhimbharak, groups come in screams which develop into a “chorus” when the resident contact with one another daily because of the overlapping male utters “whoops”; the older male infants run towards of considerable parts of their home ranges. In other loca- the intruding leader and may hit or bite him. Estrous fe- tions, like Sursagar, Kailana, and Bijolai, contact is less males often present to the intruder males, which may lead frequent because of less overlapping. During artificial to severe fights with the resident male, and even end in feeding two bisexual troops may feed together peacefully, group fission. In the initial stages of intrusion, the in- but when a bisexual troop and an all-male band interact at truder males follow the females and, if excited, threaten such times, the interaction is more agonistic. them and try to chase them toward their own band. Intertroop interaction has been observed in many Subjects and Methods mammals including several primate species. Male re- placement and infant killings in Hanuman langurs have Hanuman langurs have been well studied in and been reported at several study sites in India and in Nepal: around the city of Jodhpur during the past 40 years (e.g., Dharwar (Sugiyama, 1965), Jodhpur (Mohnot, 1971; Mohnot, 1971; Makwana, 1979; Rajpurohit, 1987). A Makwana, 1979; Sommer & Mohnot, 1985; Agoramoor- demographic study of those langurs has been in progress for the last seven years. Jodhpur lies at the eastern