<<

| DPP16~1! 445 1021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Development and Psychopathology 16 ~2004!, 157–177 Copyright © 2004 Cambridge University Press Printed in the United States of America DOI: 10.10170S0954579404044451

The expression and understanding of jealousy in children with autism

NIRIT BAUMINGER Bar-Ilan University

Abstract We investigated the expression and understanding of jealousy in 16 high-functioning children with autism and 17 typically developing children matched for IQ, chronological age, gender, and maternal education. We examined the expression of jealousy via children’s behaviors, verbalizations, and affects demonstrated during two jealousy-provoking triadic scenarios ~drawing and playing! enacted among the child in the experimental group ~autism or typical!, that child’s main caregiver ~mostly !, and a familiar peer or . The two scenarios corresponded with the two types of jealousy described in past studies: social-comparison jealousy ~drawing scenario! and social-relational jealousy ~playing scenario!. To tap children’s understanding, we asked them to identify jealousy from a picture, to provide examples of times they felt jealous, and to offer suggestions for coping with jealousy. The main results revealed that children with autism expressed jealousy in situations similar to their typical age mates but manifested it in different behaviors. Moreover, children with autism revealed a less coherent understanding of the . We discuss the meaning of the gap between demonstrating and understanding jealousy in light of the two central theoretical views conceptualizing the core emotional deficit in children with autism.

Jealousy is a complex, unpleasant feeling that ship with a ~Hansen, 1991; is highly dependent on social context and the Parrott, 1991!. ability to make spontaneous comparisons Although researchers have often examined ~Izard, 1991; Miller, Volling, & McElwain, jealousy ~mainly romantic jealousy! in , 2000!. By definition, an individual experi- much less work has focused on childhood jeal- ences jealousy when a potential threat exists ousy. One issue that complicates the study of that a valued relationship will be lost to a jealousy ~like other complex such as rival ~Izard, 1991; Parrott, 1991; Salovey & or ! concerns the vague- Rothman, 1991!. Thus, jealousy primarily ap- ness of the facial indices related to jealousy, pears in triadic contexts involving the jealous which precludes identification of jealousy individual, the rival, and the valued relation- based solely on facial expression. Researchers tend to agree that the affective expression of jealousy comprises a composite of several emo- The Internal Grants Program of Bar-Ilan University sup- tions ~e.g., , , , !; ported this research. The author extends special thanks to however, agreement is lacking about the spe- the children and who were willing to take part in this study and expresses her appreciation to Dee B. Ankon- cific emotional components that constitute jeal- ina for her editorial contribution and to Dov Har-Even for ousy ~Arnold, 1960; Hupka, 1984; Izrad, 1991; his statistical assistance. The author would also like to Parrott, 1991!. Because of this unreliability of thank Galit Halevy–Tendler for her inspiring remarks, Cory facial indices associated with jealousy, re- Shulman for her help in data collection, and the four anon- searchers believe its manifestation is orga- ymous reviewers for their thought-provoking comments. Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Nirit nized in a script-based pattern rather than as a Bauminger, School of Education, Bar-Ilan University, simple blend of basic level emotions. There- Ramat-Gan 52900, Israel; E-mail: [email protected]. fore, studies have focused on the identifica-

157 | DPP16~1! 445 2021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

158 N. Bauminger

tion of situations that provoke jealousy in child’s superiority or equality ~Bers & Rodin, children and on the detection of behaviors and 1984; Masciuch & Kienapple, 1993; Miller action components that indicate jealousy in et al., 2000!. these situations ~Masciuch & Kienapple, 1993!. Distinct behaviors and actions have been When considering situations that provoke identified as indices of jealousy among chil- jealousy, another issue to be taken into ac- dren of different ages. In infancy through count constitutes the obscurity of the distinc- preschool, children in both types of jealousy- tion between jealousy and ~Parrott, 1991!. provoking situations ~social comparison and Jealousy always involves a triadic situation in social relations! evidence behaviors such as which the child’s own loss equals the rival’s gazing directly at their main caregiver ~moth- gain, and it involves complex projections about ers and0or ! and0or the other child, the self in regard to others. In contrast, envy discontinuing work and focusing on may involve only two-person situations, and the triad, frowning, making attempts to inter- this feeling comprises the wish to have an- fere with or enter into the rival interaction other person’s possession or success and0or using attention-provoking behaviors, taking the wish that the other person did not possess the other child’s objects, hugging or climbing this desired characteristic or object ~Parrott, on the main caregiver, answering questions 1991!. According to these definitions, the ma- that were addressed to the other child, attempt- jor differentiation between the two ing to correct the other child, or trying to consists of jealousy’s necessary loss of a rela- change the situation by complaining ~Masci- tionship within a triadic situation, whereas envy uch & Kienapple, 1993; Miller et al., 2000!. does not require this loss. Furthermore, Par- As children reach school age, other behaviors rott and Smith ~1993! have suggested that in also emerge, such as comparing oneself to envy one’s own appraisal leads to dissatisfac- the other child, expressing a for the tion with oneself whereas in jealousy the re- object or relations, attempting to protect one- flected appraisal or attention of another leads self from being demeaned, behaving nega- to a lack of security and . However, tively or making negative comments toward complicating the differentiation is that in jeal- another person or about oneself, and attempt- ousy the perceived threat may not necessarily ing to do at least as well as ~equalization! or involve the loss of , and the child may better than ~! the rival ~Bers & experience jealousy related to the significant Rodin, 1984!. It should be noted that preado- other’s appreciation of the rival’s higher suc- lescents and adolescents, in particular, are so- cess ~Parrott, 1991!. Moreover, both jealousy cialized to show their anger, distress, fear, or and envy are concerned with losses of self- more indirectly through more subtle esteem stemming from social comparison, behaviors rather than direct actions or ex- demonstrate similar behavioral manifesta- plicit facial expressions ~Blumberg & Izard, tions, and may co-occur in the same situations 1991; Harris, 1989!. Thus, children’s capac- ~Bers & Rodin, 1984; Parrott, 1991; Salovey ity for spontaneous comparisons ~which al- & Rodin, 1984; Silver & Sabini, 1978!. lows for jealousy! increases with age, whereas In an attempt to better differentiate jeal- their overall explicit negative associ- ousy from envy in children, investigators de- ated with jealousy diminishes with age ~Bers vised two different situations within a social & Rodin, 1984!. triad to distinctly elicit jealousy. One focuses Wide consensus exists that emotional dif- on the child’s loss of love and0or attention, ficulties comprise one of the chief character- which is social-relations jealousy. The other istics of the autism syndrome, manifesting focuses on the child’s loss of be- themselves in both the expression and under- cause of another child’s higher success, which standing of . However, the affective is social-comparison jealousy. Situations of versus cognitive nature of these difficulties re- social-relations jealousy challenge one’s ex- mains ambiguous ~Happé, 1994; Hobson, clusivity in a relationship, whereas situations 1993a; Travis & Sigman, 1998!. Although an of social-comparison jealousy challenge the extensive body of research has investigated | DPP16~1! 445 3021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 159

simple emotions such as , sadness, more, conceptualization of a rival relation- fear, and anger ~see review in Dissanayake & ship seems to call for an understanding or Sigman, 2001!, few studies have focused on beliefs regarding the quality of the interper- the understanding of complex, self-conscious sonal relations between the jealous individ- emotions such as pride, embarrassment, and ual and a significant other ~i.e., !,as ~e.g., Capps, Yirmiya, & Sigman, 1992; well as the understanding of the interpersonal Kasari, Chamberlain, & Bauminger, 2001; relationship between the significant other and Yirmiya, Sigman, Kasari, & Mundy, 1992! or a rival ~i.e., peer or sibling!. Thus, jealousy on the behavioral manifestations and expres- appears to require inferences regarding a net- sions of such emotions ~e.g., Dawson & Mc- work of interpersonal relationships involving Kissick, 1984; Kasari, Sigman, Baumgartner, the self and others ~Volling, McElwain, & & Stipek, 1993; Spiker & Ricks, 1984!.In Miller, 2002!. Moreover, the experience of self-conscious emotions, the child expresses jealousy involves the loss of “formative atten- awareness or concern for others’ evaluations. tion,” which influences the particular aspects These emotions hold particular importance for of the self that are intrinsically interpersonal studying the nature of the emotional deficit in ~Neu, 1980; Tov–Ruach, 1980!. Thus, one may autism, in that they touch upon one of these consider the child’s “interpersonal self” to be children’s major difficulties: the metarepresen- important for jealousy to occur ~i.e., the par- tation of one’s own and others’ mental states ticular aspect of the self that concerns the ~Baron–Cohen & Swettenham, 1997; Hob- self as distinct from others, as socially effec- son, 1993a!. Complex emotions involve pro- tive, and as an object of others’ regard; Neisser, jection of one’s own mental state vis-à-vis 1988!. Hobson ~1990! and Lee and Hobson others. For example, a child may be hurt, sad, ~1998! suggested that the development of the or even angry after slipping and falling but self in relation to the physical world ~i.e., the will feel embarrassed if an audience observed “ecological self” according to Neisser, 1988! the situation. In Kasari, Sigman, Baumgart- is intact in children with autism, whereas their ner, and Stipek’s ~1993! study on pride, young interpersonal self fails to develop normally, children with autism could express resulting in difficulties in self-conscious emo- from their success in completing a puzzle as tions such as jealousy. often as typically developing children, but they failed to share their success or to look for praise from another and even looked away when they The Present Study were given praise ~which is different than typ- ical controls!. Thus, Kasari and colleagues con- The social complexity and cognitive and af- cluded that autism may involve a specific fective prerequisites of jealousy enable its affective deficit that is related to emotions and use as a means to shed light upon several situations that contain some social interactive debated issues in the affective deficit of chil- component. dren with autism. In order to attain a thor- Like pride, jealousy comprises a self- ough understanding of both the expressions reflective, socially mediated, complex emo- and understanding of jealousy, the present tion that is highly dependent on the individual study combined observations of children’s be- interpretation of social reactions. Jealousy oc- havioral reactions to two in vivo triadic situ- curs when one believes that a significant other ations with an actual rival, which were known prefers a rival; thus, like in pride, one’s own to engender jealousy in typically developing mental state is reflected vis-à-vis the reflec- children, with the examination of children’s tion of another person’s mental state. How- social cognitive processing of jealousy such ever, jealousy seems to require multiple as its recognition and the experience of and inferences on the part of the child. It appears coping with jealousy. that the child needs to infer someone else’s The first issue of debate relates to the nature mental perspective ~significant other! toward of the affective deficit in autism. Inasmuch as two individuals ~rival and oneself!. Further- jealousy involves both social–affective capa- | DPP16~1! 445 4021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

160 N. Bauminger

bilities ~e.g., the ability to form an interper- aimed to challenge the child’s sense of self- sonal relationship with a significant other, the evaluation vis-à-vis the reflection of another ability to perceive the evolving rival relation- person’s mental state ~e.g., “I believe she ship as interpersonal! and social–cognitive ca- thinks0feels my drawing is not as good.”!. pabilities ~e.g., self-evaluation vis-à-vis others, Thus, if theory of mind best characterizes the sense of competition, possessiveness!, it is very nature of the affective deficit, children would likely that both theories concerning the under- be less likely to experience jealousy in this standing of autism ~affective and cognitive! situation. The different foci of the two situa- would predict difficulties in the expression and tions ~interpersonal vs. cognitive processes! understanding of jealousy. The affective view aimed to help elucidate the nature of the emo- ~Hobson, 1993a, 1993b; Rogers & Penning- tional deficit in autism. ton, 1991! highlights the child’s disturbance in We also posed the second question of devi- intersubjective personal with oth- ant versus delayed expression and understand- ers, which is the lack of intersubjective sharing ing of jealousy. It is already well documented in autism, which seriously disrupts the child’s that high-functioning children with autism pos- ability to experience or understand interper- sess higher social–emotional capabilities com- sonal relationships as such. Thus, this approach pared with their low-functioning peers ~e.g., would emphasize the child’s inability to grasp higher prosocial abilities and emotional respon- the relations between the mother and a rival as siveness; Bacon, Fein, Morris, Waterhouse, & interpersonal and as relations that could poten- Allen, 1998!. Furthermore, in the understand- tially jeopardize the child’s interpersonal rela- ing of such as and em- tions with the mother. The cognitive view, that barrassment, children with autism compensate is, the theory of mind explanation to the affec- for their emotional deficit by utilizing their tive deficit in autism, would emphasize the higher cognitive capabilities, which is the cog- child’s inability to take another person’s views nitive compensation hypothesis ~Capps et al., into account, leading to difficulties in attribut- 1992; Kasari et al., 2001; Yirmiya et al., 1992!. ing mental states to others and to oneself in re- Finally, certain capabilities develop later in gard to others ~e.g., Tager–Flusberg, 2001!. these children, for example, theory of mind Thus, such difficulties would necessarily im- capabilities ~Happé, 1995!. Considering all pede the manifestation or understanding of self- these previous studies together, we hypoth- reflective emotions such as jealousy. esized that the ability to express a self-reflective In line with research in typical develop- emotion such as jealousy may already be ment, the present study implemented two dif- present among older ~preadolescent and ado- ferent jealousy-provoking social situations to lescent! high-functioning children with au- test these two theories in children with au- tism, even if a similar self-reflective emotion tism. The social-relations scenario comprised such as pride was not shown to exist yet in an affectively laden situation designed to evoke younger, low-functioning children with autism. jealousy as the child’s reaction toward an in- A third issue of particular was the terpersonal interaction ~shared play! between examination of differences between children another child ~a rival! and the mother, who with autism and children with typical devel- ignored her own child. If the affective ap- opment in regard to their expressions and0or proach best characterizes the affective deficit understanding of jealousy. According to Lewis in autism, then the child would be expected to ~1993!, children may sustain an emotional state act as a “behaviorist” ~standing outside rela- but not necessarily experience conscious tionships and only watching behaviors; Hob- awareness of that state. For example, chil- son, 1993b!; therefore, jealousy would be dren as young as 2–3 years can express pride experienced less in this situation. In the “social- in the presence of others, but it is not until 7 comparison scenario,” the mother praised an- or 8 years that they recognize the role that other child’s drawing ~but did not actually others play in the evaluation of their own socially interact with that rival! while ignor- accomplishments. In addition, the majority of ing her own child’s drawing. This scenario school-aged children’s descriptions of pride- | DPP16~1! 445 5021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 161

evoking events do refer to an audience ~Kasari, behaviors, which may include bizarre re- Sigman, Yirmiya, & Mundy, 1993; Seidner, sponses to various aspects of the environ- Stipek, & Fesbach, 1988!. Thus, the investi- ment, such as resistance to change. The author gation of children’s expressions and under- administered the ADI-R to the of the standing of jealousy can help clarify whether children to verify diagnosis and to provide ad- a gap exists between a more automatic behav- ditional information about the children’s de- ioral process of affective expression and a velopmental histories. The ADI-R focuses on higher level, conscious-awareness process that meeting criteria for autism in three main areas: encompasses children’s ability to understand reciprocal social interaction; communication the factors eliciting this emotion, including and language; and repetitive, restrictive, and the role of others. stereotyped behaviors. The child also needs to The present study sought to examine the show evidence of developmental delay or de- expression and understanding of jealousy in viance prior to the age of 36 months. All 16 high-functioning children with autism. More children met the criteria for autism on all four specifically, the study posed three objectives: ADI-R criteria. to describe the manifestations of jealousy in The mean age was 11.14 years ~SD ϭ 3.01! children with autism compared with typically for the children with autism and 11.51 years developing age-matched children, to explore ~SD ϭ 2.62! for the typically developing the differences in understanding jealousy be- children. Mean full-scale IQ scores, as mea- tween the two samples, and to examine how sured on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for understanding and exhibiting jealousy is re- Children—Revised ~Wechsler, 1974!, were lated to children’s mental age. 92.81 ~SD ϭ 14.15! for the children with autism and 98.35 ~SD ϭ 7.19! for the typi- cally developing children. We matched the Method group of typically developing children to the children with autism on chronological age, Participants mental age, all IQ scales ~full, verbal, and A sample of 33 preadolescents and adoles- performance!, gender, and maternal educa- cents participated in the study, including 16 ~2 tion. As can be seen in Table 1, student t tests female! high-functioning individuals with au- revealed no significant differences between tism and 17 ~2 female! typically developing groups regarding any demographic variables. individuals. All participants in the autism sam- We recruited the children with autism through ple met the criteria for autism on the Autism the Special Education Department in the Is- Diagnostic Interview—Revised ~ADI-R; Lord, raeli Ministry of Education. We recruited typ- Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994!. In addition, all ical children from local public schools. children but one had the diagnosis of autism based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Man- ual of Mental Disorders ~DSM-IV; American Measures Psychiatric Association, 1994! prior to their participation in the study as determined by li- To examine children’s expressions of jealousy censed unassociated with the cur- in the two different jealousy-provoking situa- rent study. One child was diagnosed with tions, we manipulated and videorecorded two Asperger syndrome prior to his participation experimental triadic scenarios. Assessment of in the study, but he met the ADI-R criteria for children’s jealousy-provoked behaviors, ver- autism. The DSM-IV criteria included ~a! on- balizations, and affects utilized three coding set prior to 36 months of age; ~b! qualitative scales: explicitness of jealousy manifesta- impairment in social interaction; ~c! qualita- tions, quantity of different jealousy behaviors, tive impairment in communication ~e.g., def- and response time. To investigate the under- icits or abnormalities in language development standing of jealousy, we asked children to rec- or deficit in play, particularly symbolic play!; ognize jealousy from a picture, to generate and ~d! restricted and repetitive stereotyped examples of times they experienced jealous | DPP16~1! 445 6021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

162 N. Bauminger

Table 1. Sample characteristics

Autism Typical ~n ϭ 16! ~n ϭ 17!

MSDMSDSignificance

Chronological age ~months! 133.75 36.17 138.18 31.53 ns Mental age ~months! 121.93 28.37 135.33 29.54 ns Full-scale IQ 92.81 14.15 98.35 7.19 ns Verbal IQ 91.75 15.36 95.06 5.38 ns Performance IQ 95.69 14.74 102.53 11.50 ns Male0female ratio 14021502ns Mother’s education 4.50 1.36 3.70 1.16 ns

Note: IQ and mental age scores are based on the WISC-R. Mother’s education was calculated on a 1–6 scale ~1 ϭ less than 8 years of study,2ϭhigh school without matriculation,3ϭhigh school matriculation,4ϭspecial professional training after high school,5ϭbachelor’s degree,6ϭsecond degree and above!.

feelings, and to suggest ways for coping with ceived prior written instructions ~a! to sit in such feelings. close proximity to the two children, ~b! to complete a demographic questionnaire while Jealousy-provoking experimental scenarios. ignoring the two children, and ~c! to praise Based on Masciuch and Kienapple ~1993!,we the rival child’s picture while ignoring his or manipulated two experimental scenarios in the her own child upon the children’s completion current study to provoke jealousy in the chil- of their drawings. Videotaping began when dren. The drawing scenario corresponded with the started to praise the rival child and social-comparison jealousy, and the playing lasted 5 min. scenario corresponded with social-relations In the playing scenario ~i.e., social-relations jealousy. Each scenario included a triad con- jealousy!, adapted from Masciuch and Kienap- sisting of the child in the experimental group ple’s ~1993! reading scenario, we told chil- ~autism or typical!, his or her main caregiver dren that they were going to play separately ~all mothers except for two families having a with some games. We instructed children to child with autism, where the , who was choose a game from a box that included seven the main caregiver, participated!, and another games: a falling tower construction game, an familiar child ~the rival child! who was either optical viewing device with a choice of differ- the child’s friend or sibling. The interchange- ent 3-dimensional slides of animated movie able participation of and peers in the scenes, two different assembly toys, domi- scenarios, in accordance with parents’ prefer- noes, one magnetic construction game, and one ences and accessibility considerations, was jus- magnetic mosaic game where the child needed tifiable because of the procedure’s similar to copy a shape from a picture. Prior to the ability to provoke jealousy in children with beginning of the scenario, parents received both a sibling and a peer ~Masciuch & Kienap- written instructions ~a! to sit in close proxim- ple, 1993; Miller et al., 2000!. Ten parents in ity to the children, ~b! to ignore both children each sample preferred to implement the sce- while completing a questionnaire for 2 min, narios with a sibling. and ~c! after 2 min to join the rival child in In the drawing scenario ~i.e., social- lively affectionate play for 5 min while ignor- comparison jealousy!, based on Masciuch and ing his or her own child. Videotaping began Kienapple ~1993!, we gave each child a box when the parent joined the rival child and lasted with colored markers and a blank sheet of 5 min. paper and instructed the child to complete a We adapted the playing scenario from Ma- drawing of his or her choice. Parents re- sciuch and Kienapple’s ~1993! reading sce- | DPP16~1! 445 7021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 163

nario in order to suit the ages of the children the other child’s hand; a score of 5 indicated in the current sample. The original study on direct behaviors or verbalizations that focused preschoolers instructed the mother to place the parent’s attention on the drawing or playing of rival child on her lap while reading him or her the child in the experimental group, such as a story, which would have been inappropriate putting one’s picture in front of the parent’s for preadolescents and adolescents. Prior to eyes after the parent finished praising the other the beginning of the present study, we imple- child’s picture, with or without asking the par- mented a pilot study of the current playing ent to look at the picture or game; and a score scenario with children unrelated to the present of 6 indicated a direct declaration of compar- sample ~three typically developing children and ison and lack of equality, with or without frus- three high-functioning children with autism!. tration, such as “Mom, why don’t you also The pilot study confirmed that the playing sce- play with me?” and0or when a child expressed nario provoked behaviors and verbalizations negative affects such as frustration, anger, similar to the ones reported in the reading sce- crying, sadness, or a depressed facial expres- nario by Masciuch and Kienapple. sion as a reaction to the mother’s behavior. It should be noted that behaviors and verbaliza- Coding of videotaped jealousy expressions. As tions were rated as 5 for only one child, but mentioned above, we assessed children’s both coders gave him a final score of 6 based jealousy-provoked behaviors, verbalizations, on his negative affective reaction ~intensely and affects using three coding scales: explic- frustrated tone of voice and facial expres- itness, quantity of different jealousy behav- sion!. In all other cases, children’s negative iors, and response time. affective reactions to the mother coincided with a full, explicit behavioral and0or verbal ex- Explicitness: Hierarchical jealousy scale. pression of jealousy. We developed a hierarchical jealousy scale for On this scale, separately for each of the the purpose of this study to measure the ex- two scenarios ~drawing and playing!, the child plicitness of the jealousy manifestations evi- was assigned the highest score evidenced over denced by the child during each of the two the 5-min scenario. A score of 4 and above scenarios. The scale derived from the behav- indicated explicit behaviors, verbalizations, and iors, verbalizations, and affects identified as affects that reflected jealousy whereas a score jealousy indices by previous research ~e.g., below 4 indicated only eye gaze in different Bers & Rodin, 1984; Hupka, 1984; Masciuch degrees. Appendix A presents a more system- & Kienapple, 1993; Miller et al., 2000!. This atic description of children’s jealousy re- scale included six scores describing the behav- sponses on the hierarchical scale. iors, verbalizations, and affective expressions Two different trained coders rated all of the of jealousy in hierarchical order from an ab- children’s responses separately for the draw- sence of explicit jealousy indices to the most ing and the playing scenarios. Pearson corre- explicit indices of jealousy. Scoring was as lations between the two coders were .82 for follows: a score of 1 indicated that the child the drawing scenario and .81 for the playing did not seem to pay attention to any of the scenario. ongoing scenario; a score of 2 indicated one brief eye gaze at the parent, rival child, or Quantity of different jealousy manifesta- dyadic interaction; a score of 3 indicated one tions: Behavioral coding category scale. We long gaze or a number of several short eye utilized the behavioral coding category scale gazes directed at the parent, rival child, or ~Masciuch & Kienapple, 1993! to assess the dyadic interaction, with or without stopping quantity of jealousy behaviors and vocaliza- his0her own activity; a score of 4 indicated tions of different categories that we observed behaviors or verbal comments that indirectly in each of the two 5-min jealousy scenarios intervened into the interaction between the par- ~drawing and playing!. The scale included 10 ent and the rival child, such as asking the par- indices of jealousy comprising three main cat- ent different questions or grabbing objects from egories: the child’s gaze direction, verbaliza- | DPP16~1! 445 8021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

164 N. Bauminger

tions, and actions. We separately counted and behaviors, verbalizations, and actions! using summed up the total number of jealousy indi- the six pilot study videotapes ~three autism ces observed for each of the three categories and three typical development!, until an inter- during the 5 min of observation for the draw- observer agreement level of 85% or higher ing and playing scenarios; thus, a higher score was obtained on each of the scale’s three cat- in a particular category indicated a higher quan- egories for both of the scenarios ~drawing and tity of that category of jealousy manifesta- playing!. Then, these two observers indepen- tions. The child’s gaze direction category dently rated a randomly selected 50% of chil- included two main gaze behaviors: the eyes of dren’s responses across participants and the child in the experimental group directed at scenarios. The intraclass correlation coeffi- parent and0or directed at the peer0sibling. The cients for the drawing scenario were .99 for child’s verbalizations category included five eye gaze and .98 for verbalizations and for components: ~a! : the child actions. The intraclass correlation coeffi- makes verbal attempts to draw the parent’s at- cients for the playing scenario were 1.00 for tention to self or to the child’s own drawing0 eye gaze, .98 for verbalizations, and .90 for playing, such as “My drawing is prettier,” actions. “Mom, we used to play this together”; ~b! self- deprecatory: the child makes comments that Response time. We measured the response refer negatively to self or the child’s own time to examine whether children with au- drawing0playing, such as “My picture is ugly”; tism, because of their well-documented emo- ~c! prosocial comments: the child makes pos- tional difficulties in linking emotions with itive comments about the rival child’s drawing0 social situations ~Dennis, Lockyer, & La- playing, such as “That’s such a nice game”; zenby, 2000!, would need a longer duration to ~d! interactive comments: the child makes com- respond in both scenarios compared to their ments that enter into the ongoing actions and typical age mates. We coded children’s initial conversations between the parent and the rival jealousy response on the jealousy behavioral child, such as “Yes, I remember that coding scale in seconds and separately for each trip to Eilat, do you remember diving with the of the two scenarios ~drawing and playing!. dolphins?”; and ~e! negative comments: the child makes comments that signal Assessing the understanding of jealousy. We or personal disregard for the rival child or the assessed the understanding of jealousy through parent, such as “Your assembly game is very three tasks measuring the child’s ability to rec- easy compared to mine.” The category for ognize jealousy in a picture, elicit examples child’s actions included three components: ~a! of different situations that provoke jealousy, desisting0modifying activity: the child stops and provide strategies for coping with jeal- playing when the parent joins the interaction ousy. In the first task, in order to assess the with the rival child, or the child tries to im- recognition of jealousy, we showed children prove his0her own drawing after the parent a color drawing depicting a typical social- praises the other child’s ~i.e., resumes draw- relations triadic scenario ~similar to Miller ing after having stopped earlier!; ~b! attention et al., 2000!, in which a mother is hugging her seeking: the child takes actions to draw the new baby while an older sibling is watching. parent’s attention to self or to his0her own Fifteen typically developing preadolescents and drawing0playing, such as placing his0her pic- adolescents ~unrelated to the present study! ture in front of the parent’s eyes or caressing who viewed the picture in a prior pilot study the parent’s hair; and ~c! involvement behav- could easily recognize that the older sibling iors: the child attempts to physically inter- was jealous and wanted to obtain the mother’s vene in the interaction between the parent and attention as well. After looking at the picture, the rival child, such as initiating play with that we asked the children to identify the child’s dyad. ~older sibling’s! feelings in the picture. A score Two observers underwent training in cod- of 0 indicated erroneous identification of the ing the three categories ~children’s eye gaze child’s feeling ~e.g., very happy, joyful!, a score | DPP16~1! 445 9021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 165

of 1 indicated a basic feeling sharing the same possibility that verbal output rendered con- hedonic tone ~e.g., sad, angry!, a score of 2 founding effects on the dependent variable of indicated a complex feeling from the same he- types of jealousy. To obtain this score, we cal- donic tone ~e.g., lonely, neglected!, and a score culated the total number of words stated by of 3 indicated the recognition of jealousy in the child in the description of the experience the picture. of jealousy. The t-test analysis revealed that In the second task, the understanding of the no significant differences emerged between the different situations that elicit jealousy, we asked autism group ~sum ϭ 470, range ϭ 3–85, M ϭ children to provide examples of situations in 29.37, SD ϭ 25.04! and the typical group which they or other people would feel jealous. ~sum ϭ 508, range ϭ 6–70, M ϭ 29.88, We coded children’s generated examples of SD ϭ 17.73! with regard to productivity, jealousy first in line with the two theorized t ~31! ϭ .07, p . .05. types of jealousy ~social relations and social The intraclass correlation coefficients cal- cognitive! and second in line with the per- culated between two raters who indepen- sonal immediacy of the example given ~a per- dently coded all of the children’s responses sonal example vs. an example about other were 1.00 for social–cognitive jealousy and persons!. For the first analysis, we coded af- .96 for affective jealousy. Raters discussed all fective jealousy ~corresponding to social- disagreements on the affective scale until they relations jealousy! when participants’examples attained agreement. indicated that jealousy involved negative feel- We conducted another analysis for the sec- ings associated with a child’s responses to a ond task with regard to the personal immedi- social triangle in which the parent, another fa- acy level demonstrated in children’s examples miliar ~e.g., teacher, grandmother, or of the experience of jealousy. This examina- grandfather!, or a peer paid exclusive atten- tion compared children’s generation of per- tion to another peer or a sibling ~Masciuch & sonal examples of jealousy, which was scored Kienapple, 1993!. Affective jealousy reflects 1 ~e.g., “I’m jealous of my friend because she’s situations in which the child’s exclusivity in a prettier than me.”! versus children’s genera- relationship is threatened. tion of examples regarding other persons, We coded social–cognitive jealousy ~corre- which was scored 0 ~e.g., “When everyone gets sponding with social-comparison jealousy! the credit, but he does not”!. when participants’ examples indicated that To further the assessment of jealousy un- jealousy arose when one child enjoyed more derstanding, the third task sought to assess the success or possessions compared with an- quality and quantity of children’s repertoire of other child, challenging the first child’s supe- strategies for coping with jealousy. We coded riority or equality ~Bers & Rodin, 1984!. coping strategies to deal with jealousy along Children obtained a score of 2 if they pro- two dimensions: the number of suggestions vided both types of examples ~affective and offered by the child and the various content social–cognitive jealousy!, a score of 1 if only areas suggested by the child. Because of the one type was specified, and a score of 0 paucity of research specifically investigating if neither type was specified. Appendix B children’s coping strategies for jealousy, the presents examples of children’s suggestions for present content analysis utilized Harris’ ~1989! their experiences of jealousy according to the reported coping strategies implemented by chil- jealousy type. dren when dealing with similar unpleasant, In addition, to determine whether the par- painful feelings ~e.g., sadness, disappoint- ticipants who included both types of jealousy ment, distress!. Harris identified two main strat- in their examples did so because they were egies for coping with an unpleasant feeling. more verbally productive, we computed a pro- First, children attempt to change a situation ductivity score for each participant to mea- by moving to or creating a situation or activity sure verbal output regarding their descriptions that is more enjoyable. Usually this will in- of jealousy experiences. Similar productivity volve partial or total restoration of the loss. rates in the two groups would eliminate the “When confronting sad feelings, children may | DPP16~1! 445 10021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

166 N. Bauminger

try to modify this feeling by deliberately en- ducted the research at the home of the child gaging in an activity that is normally associ- in the experimental group in a quiet room ated with feeling happy” ~Harris, 1989, p. 156!. during one home visit. In half of the cases Second, children may attempt to reduce an un- the children completed the three-part jeal- pleasant feeling by implementing cognitive– ousy understanding task prior to enactment mentalistic techniques such as trying not to of the two experimental jealousy scenarios, think about the unpleasant event, forgetting it, and the sequence was reversed for the other or occupying the mind with other things. By half. No significant order effect or interaction offering the first solution, children acknowl- of Group ϫ Order emerged for any of the edge that a later positive event can mitigate expression or understanding variables. To an earlier negative emotion; by suggesting the counteract possible negative effects of the second solution, children demonstrate aware- jealousy-provoking situations, we instructed ness of the connection between cognition and parents to compliment the drawing produced emotion. According to Harris, the types of so- by the child with autism or to join in the lutions offered by children change over devel- child’s play immediately after we turned off opment: younger children ~at around age 6 the videocamera. years! are more likely to offer the first solu- tion whereby unpleasant feelings are modi- Results fied by carrying out some activity; and by the age of 10 years or over, children are more likely Expressions of jealousy to suggest cognitive solutions as well. Two coders examined the adaptability of The first set of analyses examined the differ- Harris’ ~1989! categories to the present sam- ences in the explicitness, quantity, and re- ple’s solutions for coping with jealousy. All of sponse time of jealousy expressions ~using the the children’s suggestions but four could be hierarchical scale, behavioral coding scale, and coded in line with one of the two criteria, and response time measure, respectively! between there was 92% agreement between coders. The children with autism and typically developing four exceptions all comprised aggressive so- children in the drawing and playing scenarios. lutions such as hitting the other child ~sug- As can be seen in Table 2, which de- gested by a child with autism! or destroying scribes the distribution of the two groups on the other child’s beautiful car ~suggested by a the jealousy hierarchical scale, the majority typically developing child!. Thus, we con- of children in both groups expressed explicit sidered aggressive solutions as a separate behaviors, verbalizations, and affects that in- content category; however, because of their dicated jealousy ~i.e., a score of 4–6!.On low frequency, they were not included in the this scale, a score of 4 and above indicated analysis. Appendix C presents examples of explicit behaviors, verbalizations, and affects children’s suggestions for each of the two that reflected jealousy whereas a score below categories. 4 indicated only eye gaze in different de- grees. A jealousy index of 4 or above for the drawing and the playing scenarios emerged Procedure for 88 and 75% of the children with autism We contacted the parents of the children in and 67 and 73% of the typically developing both the special and regular education set- children, respectively. Indeed, a 2 ϫ 2 Group- tings through their school principal and0or ing ~autism0typical! ϫ Scenario ~drawing0 through the child’s teacher. After obtaining playing! analysis of variance ~ANOVA! with written parental consent for participation, we repeated measures on scenario, which we ex- arranged home visits by telephone with par- ecuted to examine group differences for the ents. We advised parents in advance about explicit expression of jealousy on the hierar- the nature of the research and the need to chical scale, was not significant for group ef- arrange for another child to be present in the fect, scenario effect, or the interaction of group home during the experimenter’s visit. We con- and scenario. Children in both groups ~au- | DPP16 ~ 1 ! 4 11 445 0 102 21 0 13 0 41:0a EIE PROOF REVISED am 11:40 04

Table 2. Distribution of the two groups according to explicitness of jealousy scores on the hierarchical jealousy scale for the drawing and playing scenarios

Drawing Playing

Autism Typical Autism Typical

Level of Explicitness of Jealousy n % n % n % n % 167

Level 1: no particular indication of jealousy 1 6.25 0 0.00 1 6.25 1 6.66 Level 2: one brief eye gaze 1 6.25 4 26.66 0 0.00 0 0.00 Level 3: long gaze or number of several short eye gazes 0 0.00 1 6.66 3 18.75 3 20.00 Level 4: behaviors or verbalizations that indirectly intervene into the parent—rival 5 31.25 4 26.66 4 25.00 5 33.33 child interaction Level 5: direct behaviors0verbalizations aimed at focusing parent’s attention to the 5 31.25 4 26.66 4 25.00 4 26.66 experimental child’s drawing0playing Level 6: direct declaration of comparison and lack of equality, or negative affect 4 25.00 2 13.33 4 25.00 2 13.33 | | DPP16~1! 445 12021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

168 N. Bauminger

Table 3. Means (M), standard deviations (SD), and F values for differences between children with autism and children with typical development on the behavioral coding category scale

Autism Typical F ~1, 29! F ~1, 29! F ~1, 29! Draw Play Draw Play Group Scenario Interaction

Gaze M 3.87 4.75 5.53 7.60 4.10* 2.81 .46 SD 2.57 2.93 3.60 5.92 Verbalization M 2.43 2.81 1.87 3.80 .07 2.45 1.12 SD 2.36 2.80 2.23 4.29 Action M 1.81 2.10 .40 .73 14.87** 1.22 .02 SD 1.47 1.65 .63 .79

Note: Group, autism0typical; scenario, drawing0playing. In the verbalization and action categories, several SDs were higher than their means; therefore, an additional nonparametric Mann–Whitney test for independent samples was performed for these cases, which mirrored the ANOVA results. *p , .05. **p , .001.

tism and typical! displayed jealousy at a sim- toward them, regardless of the scenario ~draw- ilar level of explicitness on the two scenarios ing or playing!. ~M ϭ 4.46, SD ϭ 1.34 for drawing and M ϭ Further, to examine the differences among 4.31, SD ϭ 1.36 for playing in the autism the three behaviors of the jealousy coding scale sample, and M ϭ 3.80, SD ϭ 1.37 for draw- ~eye gaze, verbalizations, and actions!,we ing and M ϭ 4.16, SD ϭ 1.33 for playing in performed a 2 ϫ 2 ϫ 3 ANOVA ~Group ϫ the control group!. Scenario ϫ Type of Jealousy Behaviors! with Next, we conducted a 2 ϫ 2 ~Group ϫ Sce- repeated measures on the type of scenarios and nario! multivariate ANOVA ~MANOVA! with behaviors. The results of the ANOVA yielded repeated measures on scenario to investigate a significant behavior effect, F ~2, 58! ϭ group differences on the quantity of the jeal- 32.08, p , .001, and interaction effect ousy behaviors observed for each of the three ~Group ϫ Behavior!, F ~2, 58! ϭ 5.87, p , category types ~gaze, verbalization, and ac- .01. The interactions for Type ϫ Scenario tion!. The results of the MANOVA revealed a and for Group ϫ Type ϫ Scenario were not significant main effect of group, F ~Wilk’s cri- significant. To determine the source of the terion!~3, 27! ϭ 7.20, p , .001. Neither the significant interaction, simple effect tests main effect of the scenario nor the interaction examined the differences between the three effect of Group ϫ Scenario was significant. jealousy behaviors ~eye gaze, verbalizations, Table 3 presents the means and standard devi- and actions! within each group. A signifi- ations for the three jealousy categories’ scores cant F value emerged for each group, but on the behavioral coding category scale evi- the difference between the behaviors in the denced by the children with autism and chil- typically developing group, F ~2, 28! ϭ dren with typical development. As can be seen 22.18, p . .001, was higher compared with in the table, univariate ANOVAs revealed sig- the difference within the group of children nificant group differences on gaze and action. with autism, F ~2, 30! ϭ 9.11, p , .001. Compared to typically developing children, Indeed, a further set of paired comparison children with autism were significantly less tests according to Newman–Keuls ~ p , .05! likely to look at the parent and0or the rival within each group revealed significant differ- child but were significantly more likely to act ences between all three jealousy behaviors for | DPP16~1! 445 13021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 169

the typical control group and only between tween the child’s mental age and action on the eye gaze and the other two jealousy behaviors play scenario ~Fisher Z ϭ 2.00, p , .05; au- ~e.g., verbalizations and actions! for the au- tism, r ϭϪ.56; typical, r ϭ .06!. tism group. To examine the group differences on re- Understanding jealousy sponse time, we conducted a 2 ϫ 2 ~Group ϫ Scenario! ANOVA with repeated measures. The next set of analyses focused on children’s We calculated the response time using loga- understanding of jealousy along the three main rithm values in order to decrease the large stan- dimensions tapped by the tasks: recognition dard deviations. The results of the ANOVA of jealousy in a picture, providing examples yielded a significant main scenario type ef- of the experience of jealousy, and describing fect, F ~1, 29! ϭ 10.84, p , .01, and nonsig- ways to cope with jealousy. Regarding the first nificant effects for group and for the interaction task, which assessed children’s ability to rec- of scenario and group. Children in both groups ognize jealousy, the majority of children with ~autism and typical! responded faster in the typical development ~n ϭ 13; 76.5%! accu- drawing scenario compared with the playing rately recognized jealousy in the picture ver- scenario. sus only 4 children ~25.0%! in the autism Last, nonparametric Mann–Whitney tests group. However, among the remaining 12 chil- examined within-group differences between dren with autism who could not recognize jeal- those scenarios implemented with siblings ousy in the picture, 7 children ~comprising ~n ϭ 10 in each group! and those imple- 43.8% of the autism sample! were neverthe- mented with peers regarding all three scales less able to identify basic and complex emo- for the expression of jealousy ~hierarchical tions with an accurate hedonic tone ~e.g., sad!, scale, behavioral coding scale, and response as were 3 out of the 4 remaining typically de- time!. No significant differences emerged on veloping children who had failed in the recog- any of the scales for either the drawing or the nition scale ~17.6% of the typical sample!.An playing scenarios. ANOVA with group as the independent vari- able and the child’s score on the recognition of jealousy as the dependent variable yielded Correlations between mental age and a significant group difference. Children with expressions of jealousy: Within-group autism were less likely to recognize jealousy examination in the picture compared with typically devel- We computed the correlations between the chil- oping children, F ~1, 31! ϭ 12.39, p , .001 dren’s mental age and the three measures of ~M ϭ 1.31, SD ϭ 1.19, for autism, and M ϭ jealousy manifestation ~hierarchical scale, jeal- 2.59, SD ϭ 0.87, for the typical group!. ousy behavioral coding scale, and response Regarding children’s ability to provide ex- time! in each group ~autism and typical! for amples of the experience of jealousy, a chi- the drawing and the playing scenarios. Few square analysis revealed that the two groups significant correlations emerged for either differed in the proportion of children who group. For children with autism, the child’s included both the affective and the social– mental age correlated negatively with the ac- cognitive jealousy types in the examples of tion scale for the playing situation ~r ϭϪ.56, jealousy experiences that they provided, x2 p , .05!. In the autism group, children with a ~2, 33! ϭ 8.11, p , .05. Only 2 children higher mental age were less likely to display with autism ~12.5%! provided both types, actions that expressed jealousy during the play versus 10 typically developing children scenario. In the typically developing group, ~58.8%!. An ANOVA with group as the inde- during the play scenario, children with a higher pendent variable and the total jealousy exam- mental age were less likely to display jealousy ple score as the dependent variable revealed explicitly ~r ϭϪ.46, p , .05!. A Fisher Z that, compared to typical children, children analysis revealed significant group differ- with autism were significantly less likely to ences only in regard to the correlation be- offer both types of examples ~affective and | DPP16~1! 445 14021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

170 N. Bauminger

social–cognitive jealousy!, F ~1, 31! ϭ 10.10, Correlations between mental age and p , .01 ~M ϭ 1.06, SD ϭ 0.44, for autism, understanding jealousy: Within-group and M ϭ 1.58, SD ϭ 0.50, for the typical examination group!. The next analyses separately exam- ined the proportions of each group that pro- We computed the correlations in each group vided examples of each type of jealousy. The ~autism and typical! between the children’s percentage of affective jealousy examples dif- mental age and the three tasks measuring the fered between the two groups, provided by understanding of jealousy: picture recogni- only 31.2% of the children with autism com- tion, the child’s examples of jealousy, and the pared to 70.5% in the control group, x2 ~1, child’s suggestions for coping with jealousy. 33! ϭ 5.10, p , .05. In contrast, no signifi- Few significant correlations emerged for ei- cant differences emerged in the percentages ther group. For children with autism, the abil- of children who provided examples related to ity to provide examples of social–cognitive social–cognitive jealousy ~75.0% in autism jealousy and the ability to provide both types vs. 88.2% in the typical group, Fisher exact of jealousy examples ~both affective and test, ns!. social–cognitive! each correlated positively ϭ A significant difference appeared in the per- with the children’s mental age ~r .52, p , ϭ centages of children who provided personal .05; r .60, p , .01, respectively!. Simi- examples of jealousy versus children who larly, the ability for children with autism to provided examples of jealousy relating to suggest cognitive–mentalistic solutions for other people. Only 56% percent of the chil- coping with jealousy correlated positively with ϭ dren with autism ~n ϭ 9! described per- children’s mental age ~r .56, p , .05!. For sonal examples versus 100% of the children typically developing children, child’s mental with typical development ~Fisher exact test, age correlated negatively with providing a p , .006!. situation-activity solution to cope with jeal- Regarding the third task, children’s strat- ousy and correlated positively with the abil- egies for coping with jealousy, we conducted ity to provide cognitive–mentalistic solutions ϭϪ ϭ analyses to examine the number of solutions ~r .42, p , .05; r .45, p , .05, respec- suggested by the child and the type of content tively!. The Fisher Z test revealed significant areas suggested. Children with autism sug- group differences for the following correla- gested a lower number of solutions for coping tions: mental age and children’s ability to pro- ϭ with jealousy compared to their typical age vide both types of examples of jealousy ~Z ϭ ϭϪ mates, F ~1, 31! ϭ 11.51, p , .01 ~M ϭ 1.06, 1.87, p , .05; r .60 for autism; r .03, SDϭ0.57; Mϭ2.11, SDϭ1.11, respectively!. for typical! and mental age and the ability to We computed an ANOVAto test for group dif- provide only the social-comparison examples ϭ ϭ ferences regarding the number of solutions sug- of jealousy ~Z 1.69, p , .05; r .52 for ϭϪ gested by the children in each of the two main autism; r .07 for typical!. coping categories ~situation activity and cognitive–mentalistic!. Children with autism Discussion suggested fewer situation-activity solutions compared with typically developing children, The present study explored the expression F ~1, 31!ϭ4.47, p , .05 ~Mϭ0.68, SDϭ0.47; and understanding of jealousy among high- M ϭ1.35, SD ϭ1.69, respectively!, and a sim- functioning children with autism. The main ilar number of cognitive–mentalistic solutions findings revealed that children with autism ~M ϭ 0.31, SD ϭ 0.47; M ϭ 0.59, SD ϭ 0.87, expressed jealousy in situations similar to respectively!. Because of the large standard de- those that provoked jealousy in their typi- viations found in comparison to the means for cally developing age mates ~the experimental the cognitive–mentalistic domain, we also per- drawing and playing scenarios!, yet, the man- formed a nonparametric Mann–Whitney test for ifestations of jealousy differed in autism ver- two independent samples, which mirrored the sus typical development. Typically developing ANOVAresults. children demonstrated more eye gaze behav- | DPP16~1! 445 15021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 171

iors toward the parent and0or toward the ri- opment. The considerably more explicit, ac- val child in each of the jealousy-provoking tive expressions of jealousy demonstrated situations ~drawing and playing!, whereas chil- by children with autism compared to their dren with autism displayed more actions to typical age mates in our study resembled the express their jealous feelings. Furthermore, less mature, more explicit jealousy-provoked children with autism exhibited a less coher- behaviors reported as characterizing younger, ent understanding of the feeling and of situa- typically developing children ~e.g., Bers & tions that provoke jealousy, compared to the Rodin, 1984; Masciuch & Kienapple, 1993!. typical control group. This discussion consid- For example, one child with autism took his ers the meaning of these differences between drawing and pushed it in front of his moth- autism and typical development and dis- er’s face after she had praised the rival child. cusses the implications of these gaps for un- He repeated this behavior seven times, in- derstanding the emotional deficit in autism. cluding one instance when he jumped over The finding that children with autism ex- the other child’s head in an attempt to reach pressed jealousy in both situations, which in- his mother’s face and show her his picture. volved the risk of losing formative attention The extent of active behaviors exhibited by ~play! and evaluation ~draw!, may suggest that the children with autism was unparalleled in these older, high-functioning children’s aware- the typical sample, whose more implicit ex- ness of themselves as objects for others’ eval- pressions of jealousy were generally limited uations and0or concerns ~“interpersonal self”! to gazing at the parent or rival child. In ad- is developed, even if expressed differently than dition, only the mental age of children with among typical controls. What was absent from autism negatively correlated with the action the capacity of 2-year-old children with au- jealousy scale during the play situations, in- tism and mental retardation, namely indica- dicating that children with a higher mental tions of pride, embarrassment, possessiveness, age exhibited fewer actions during this sce- or competition ~Hobson, 1990; Kasari et al., nario. Although the current outcome regard- 1993!, may already have evolved within older ing manifestations of jealousy in autism high-functioning children with autism. Fur- are possibly linked to deficits in emotional thermore, the current sample’s showing behav- understanding rather than to a qualitative iors ~e.g., “Mom, look!”! and direct and indirect deviance explanation, this study calls for fu- spontaneous attempts to share attention with ture research that directly compares children the caregiver regarding a third object ~e.g., the with autism to young typical children. Fur- child’s painting! call for further examination ther studies would also do well to investigate of the possibility of later development of sec- the expression of other self-reflective emo- ondary intersubjectivity and, by extrapola- tions ~pride, embarrassment! in older high- tion, joint attention ~the shared attention of functioning children with autism. two persons regarding a third event or object! What implications arise from such findings in these children. Indeed, the DSM-IV-TR in terms of the affective versus cognitive in- ~American Psychiatric Association, 2000! con- terpretations of the emotional deficit in au- siders the lack of a spontaneous search for tism? In contrast with expectations, each of shared experience to be a cardinal symptom the two scenarios ~drawing and playing! ren- of autism, but less is known about this behav- dered the same jealousy-provoking effect ior in older high-functioning children with au- on children with autism. Albeit there were dif- tism ~Rogers & Bennetto, 2001!. ferent roots for the two jealousy experiences The different expressions of jealousy dem- manipulated in the present study ~playing: af- onstrated by the children with autism in the fective and interpersonal; drawing: social– present study as compared to their typical cognitive and comparison processes!, both age mates may stem from a deficit in under- comprised classic jealousy situations in which standing socially accepted rules for emo- the child lost something ~either attention or tional display, which children gain through evaluation! from a valued person ~caregiver! the socialization process during typical devel- to a rival; hence, the loss experienced by the | DPP16~1! 445 16021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

172 N. Bauminger

child in the experimental group equaled the The children with autism in the present study rival’s gain. Thus, the children with autism in were less accurate in identifying jealousy from the present study did seem to experience a self- a social-relations picture compared with typ- reflective, socially mediated emotion such as ically developing children, and they provided jealousy. Although not enough is known about examples of social-relations jealousy less of- the demands of the current jealousy tasks to ten ~fewer than one-third of the autism group draw clear-cut conclusions, expressing jeal- succeeded vs. more than two-thirds in the typ- ousy in the play scenario could possibly imply ical group!. Such intergroup differences did children’s sense of the intersubjectivity of the not emerge for social-comparison jealousy. network of relations within the triadic sce- Thus, although children with autism ex- nario ~caregiver–rival–child in the experimen- pressed affective jealousy ~in the playing sce- tal group!. Expressing jealousy in the drawing nario! as often as typical controls, they were scenario may possibly imply children’s capa- less capable of recognizing the emotion in bilities to attribute their caregiver’s implicit such a situation and less competent at identi- mental states toward their own picture. How- fying the conditions that reflect this type of ever, the lack of superiority for one theoretical jealousy. explanation ~affective vs. cognitive! over the Similarly to these findings, the children with other calls for an integrative developmental autism in Bauminger and Kasari’s ~2000! study theoretical model that allows for the evolve- reported greater compared with typ- ment of higher social–emotional capabilities ical controls, but they failed to include the more in high-functioning children with autism and affective dimension of loneliness ~being left also accepts the existence of the expression of out of intimate close relationships! in their def- self-reflective emotions and sharing capabili- initions of the emotion. Is it possible that when ties. In addition, this model needs to take into dealing with the more affective root of an emo- account the gap between these children’s abil- tion that is linked to social relatedness ~e.g., ity to experience an emotion that is socially emotional loneliness, affective-relational jeal- mediated and rooted in social interaction in ousy!, high-functioning children with autism comparison to their seriously lagging ability are able to experience the emotion but cannot to understand this emotion. describe this experience because of their af- Indeed, the present study demonstrated a fective deficit? Interestingly, Lee and Hobson more severe deficit in the ability to con- ~1998! reported that high-functioning individ- sciously describe the experience and to under- uals with autism were deficient in their ability stand jealousy than in the expression of the to describe issues related to their interper- emotion. According to Saarni ~1999!, the abil- sonal self; none of the participants in their study ity to describe emotional experience requires provided social self-statements that referred the development of a network of concepts, to friends or to being a member of a social which are scripts for representing children’s group. However, that study did not examine own emotional responses within a multidimen- children’s actual social-interpersonal rela- sional matrix of causes, goals, values, social tions, and it is possible that these children were relations, and beliefs about emotion manage- involved in interpersonal relationships with- ment. Typically developing children of about out the ability to reflect on them. Other recent age 6–8 have well-defined scripts that reveal studies have reported in these high- such a multidimensional matrix. In the case functioning older children with autism ~Baum- of autism, these scripts seem to fail to de- inger & Kasari, 2000; Bauminger & Shulman, velop in the normative way. Not all types of 2003!. experiences are difficult for these children to The social-comparison examples were rel- reflect upon. It appears that when the emo- atively intact in children with autism. These tional experience requires a projection about examples are based on the child’s sense of com- the self vis-à-vis the representation of social petition and0or feeling of lack of equality, and relations, like in social-relations jealousy, the they are rooted in a social–cognitive rather child with autism is less likely to succeed. than affective process of comparison—the | DPP16~1! 445 17021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 173

child is projecting about the self through the affective elements that may be difficult for chil- accomplishments or possessions of others. Fur- dren with autism to reflect upon. thermore, in contrast with the experimental sce- In terms of coping with jealousy, the chil- nario that elicited jealousy, in the examples of dren’s ability to provide cognitive–mentalistic social-comparison jealousy it is hard to dis- suggestions was positively linked with mental count the possibility that children provided ex- age for both samples; however, overall, chil- amples of envy ~“I want for myself what the dren with autism provided a lower number of other child has.”!. Inasmuch as jealousy in- solutions for coping with jealousy in compar- volves complex projections about the self vis- ison to their typical age mates. This finding à-vis others whereas envy does not necessarily, emphasizes the difficulties in emotional un- the children with autism in the present study derstanding in the autism sample; they have could more easily provide less mature exam- less knowledge of how to deal with an unpleas- ples of jealousy ~or envy!, because of their ant feeling such as jealousy. difficulties in performing the more complex The following study limitations should be projections about the self required for the re- noted. First, because of the fact that only high- flection of jealousy. They could more easily functioning older children participated in the furnish examples stemming from their own study, questions still remain regarding the chro- self-needs or their own appraisal that led to nology of the development of jealousy in au- dissatisfaction, rather than examples rooted in tism and its universality to the disorder. Future interpersonal relationships and dealing with studies may examine whether low-functioning the fear of losing these relationships. children with autism experience and under- Overall, what seems to remain distorted stand jealousy in a similar manner and may even at older ages is the ability to reflect about attempt to identify the onset of jealousy among emotional experience with others, a reflection low- and high-functioning children with au- that requires the consideration of interper- tism. Along these lines, future research may sonal relationships. One common explanation do well to include participants with a nar- for the superior social–emotional functioning rower IQ range and to utilize a one on one of high versus low-functioning children with matching procedure. Second, the peer0sibling autism comprises the cognitive compensation participation factor should be mentioned. Be- or logicoaffective hypothesis ~Capps et al., cause of the fact that the manipulation of jeal- 1992; Hermelin & O’Connor, 1985; Kasari ousy scenarios largely depended on the parent’s et al., 2001; Yirmiya et al., 1992!. This hypoth- ability to perform the scenarios accurately, it esis suggests that children with autism learn was especially important that parents felt com- strategies to recognize emotions that “come fortable. Therefore, parents were given the op- naturally” to individuals with typical develop- tion of performing the scenario with a peer or ment. The major dilemma of this hypothesis with a sibling, in light of past studies’ reports concerns the boundaries of this strategy in fully that both siblings and peers provoke jealousy compensating for the affective deficit in au- in similar situations ~Masciuch & Kienapple, tism ~Kasari et al., 2001!. In the present study, 1993; Miller et al., 2000!. Indeed, a similar only for the autism sample, children’s mental percentage of peers and siblings was selected ages correlated positively with the ability to by parents in the two groups ~autism and typ- provide social-comparison examples of jeal- ical!, and nonsignificant differences emerged ousy but not with the ability to provide social- between scenarios that were implemented with relations examples of jealousy. Thus, cognitive siblings versus those with peers. However, a capabilities seemed less helpful in producing possibility remains that because the study was examples that are rooted in interpersonal rela- not limited to only siblings or to only familiar tionships ~e.g., social-relations jealousy!. More- peers for all of the scenarios, the results may over, future studies utilizing social–cognitive have been influenced in some way. analogue tasks such as recognition of affec- Third, another limitation regards the exper- tive jealousy in a picture should take into ac- imental manipulation of jealousy. The present count the possibility that such tasks contain study followed the same paradigm that suc- | DPP16~1! 445 18021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

174 N. Bauminger

cessfully provoked jealousy in typically de- difficulties these children exhibit in under- veloping children, altering none of its standing affective jealousy. components. However, in order to claim un- In conclusion, high-functioning children equivocally that the presence of the other peer with autism manifested jealousy in similar sit- or the mother was necessary to provoke jeal- uations as did their typically developing coun- ousy, future research should compare these terparts; yet, their understanding of the feeling results with nontriadic scenarios comprising was less coherent compared with their typi- only the mother and child while the mother cal age mates. These outcomes call for a solely praises her own drawing and0or with developmental–integral model to explicate the two triadic scenarios ~one including the mother affective deficit of these children and take and the other including an unfamiliar adult!. into consideration the better social–emotional Previous research on prosocial behaviors re- performance of individuals with autism who vealed that children with autism were rela- have higher cognitive capabilities. In partic- tively indifferent to mothers’ behaviors ~e.g., ular, the major contribution of the present pretending to be sick; Sigman, Kasari, Kwon, study revealed a gap between a more intact & Yirmiya, 1990!, but researchers have not capability to experience jealousy ~a self- tested this issue for jealousy. In addition, to reflective, socially mediated emotion! and a better tease out the differentiation of jealousy deficit in the capacity to fully reflect on the from envy, another experimental condition experience of such an emotion. This gap be- could include only two peers, for example, tween expression and understanding of the with one receiving what the other one de- more affective–interpersonal roots of emo- sires. Fourth, because the present study em- tions should be studied further. Along these ployed only a single picture to tap affective lines, the current study’s outcomes empha- jealousy, future studies would do well to in- size the need to promote emotional under- clude control pictures ~e.g., including other standing capabilities in high-functioning emotions! in order to claim specificity for the children with autism.

References

American Psychiatric Association. ~1994!. Diagnostic and Blumberg, S., & Izard, C. ~1991!. Patterns of emotion statistical manual of mental disorders ~4th ed., Re- experiences as predictors of facial expressions of emo- vised!. Washington, DC: Author. tion. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 37, 97–183. American Psychiatric Association. ~2000!. Diagnostic and Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., & Sigman, M. ~1992!. Under- statistical manual of mental disorders: Text revision standing of simple and complex emotions in non- ~4th ed., Revised!. Washington, DC: Author. retarded children with autism. Journal of Child Arnold, M. B. ~1960!. Emotion and personality. New York: Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, Columbia University Press. 33, 1169–1182. Bacon, A. L., Fein, D., Morris, R., Waterhouse, L., & Dawson, G., & McKissick, F. C. ~1984!. Self-recognition Allen, D. ~1998!. The responses of autistic children to in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Develop- the distress of others. Journal of Autism and Develop- mental Disorders, 14, 383–394. mental Disorders, 28, 129–142. Dennis, M., Lockyer, L., & Lazenby, A. L. ~2000!. How Baron–Cohen, S., & Swettenham, J. ~1997!. Theory high-functioning children with autism understand real of mind in autism: Its relations to executive func- and deceptive emotion. Autism, 4, 370–381. tion and central coherence. In D. J. Cohen & F. R. Dissanayake, C., & Sigman, M. ~2001!. Attachment and Volkmar ~Eds.!, Handbook of autism and devel- emotional responsiveness in children with autism. In- opmental disorders ~pp. 880–893!. New York: ternational Review of Research in Mental Retarda- Wiley. tion, 23, 239–266. Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. ~2000!. Loneliness and friend- Hansen, G. L. ~1991!. Jealousy: Its conceptualization, mea- ship in high-functioning children with autism. Child surement, and integration with family stress theory. In Development, 71, 447–456. P. Salovey ~Ed.!, The psychology of jealousy and envy Bauminger, N., & Shulman, C. ~2003!. The development ~pp. 211–230!. New York: Guilford Press. and maintenance of in high-functioning chil- Happé, F. G. E. ~1994!. Annotation: Current psychologi- dren with autism: Maternal perceptions. Autism, 7, cal theories of autism: The “theory of mind” account 81–97. and rival theories. Journal of Child Psychology and Bers, S. A., & Rodin, J. ~1984!. Social comparison jeal- Psychiatry, 35, 215–229. ousy: A developmental and motivational study. Jour- Happé, F. G. E. ~1995!. The role of age and verbal ability nal of Personality and , 47, in the theory of mind task performance of subjects 766–779. with autism. Child Development, 66, 843–855. | DPP16~1! 445 19021 02013004 11:40 am REVISED PROOF |

Jealousy in autism 175

Harris, P. L. ~1989!. Children and emotion: The develop- Parrott, W. G. ~1991!. The emotional experience of envy ment of psychological understanding. Oxford: Basil and jealousy. In P. Salovey ~Ed.!, The psychology of Blackwell. jealousy and envy ~pp. 3–31!. New York: Guilford Hermelin, B., & O’Connor, N. ~1985!. The logico-affective Press. disorder in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov Parrott, W. G., & Smith, R. H. ~1993!. Distinguishing the ~Eds.!, Communication problems in autism ~pp. 283– experiences of envy and jealousy. Journal of Person- 310!. New York: Plenum Press. ality and Social Psychology, 64, 906–920. Hobson, R. P. ~1990!. On the origins of self and the case Rogers, S. R., & Bennetto, L. ~2001!. Intersubjectivity in of autism. Development and Psychopathology, 2, autism: The role of imitation and executive function. 163–181. In A. M. Wetherby & B. M. Prizant ~Eds.!, Autism Hobson, R. P. ~1993a!. Autism and the development of spectrum disorders: A transactional developmental per- mind. Hove, UK: Erlbaum. spective ~Vol. 9, pp. 79–107!. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Hobson, R. P. ~1993b!. The emotional origins of interper- Brooks. sonal understanding. Philosophical Psychology, 6, Rogers, S. R., & Pennington, B. F. ~1991!. A theoretical 227–249. approach to the deficits in infantile autism. Develop- Hupka, R. B. ~1984!. Jealousy: Compound emotion or ment and Psychopathology, 3, 137–162. label for a particular situation. Motivation and Emo- Saarni, C. ~1999!. The development of emotional compe- tion, 8, 141–155. tence. New York: Guilford Press. Izard, C. E. ~1991!. The psychology of emotions. New Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. ~1984!. Some antecedents and York: Plenum Press. consequences of social-comparison jealousy. Journal Kasari, C., Chamberlain, B., & Bauminger, N. ~2001!. of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 780–792. Social emotions and social relationships in autism: Salovey, P., & Rothman, A. ~1991!. Envy and jealousy: Can children with autism compensate? In J. Burack, Self and society. In P. Salovey ~Ed.!, The psychology T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo ~Eds.!, Devel- of jealousy and envy ~pp. 271–286!. New York: Guil- opment and autism: Perspectives from theory and re- ford Press. search ~pp. 309–323! Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Seidner, L. B., Stipek, D. J., & Fesbach, N. D. ~1988!.A Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Baumgartner, P., & Stipek, D. developmental analysis of elementary school-aged chil- ~1993!. Pride and mastery in children with autism. dren’s concept of pride and embarrassment. Child De- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, velopment, 59, 367–377. 353–362. Silver, M., & Sabini, J. ~1978!. The perception of envy. Kasari, C., Sigman, M., Yirmiya, N., & Mundy, P. ~1993!. Social Psychology, 41, 105–117. Affective development and communication in chil- Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. ~1990!. dren with autism. In A. P. Kaiser & D. B. Gray ~Eds.!, Responses to negative emotion of others by autistic, Enhancing children’s communication: Research foun- mentally retarded, and normal children. Child Devel- dations for intervention ~pp. 201–222!. New York: opment, 63, 796–807. Brookes. Spiker, D., & Ricks, M. ~1984!. Visual self-recognition in Lee, A., & Hobson, R. P. ~1998!. On developing self- autistic children: Developmental relationships. Child concepts: A controlled study of children and adoles- Development, 55, 214–225. cents with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Tager–Flusberg, H. ~2001!. A reexamination of the theory Psychiatry, 39, 1131–1144. of mind hypothsis of autism. In J. Burack, T. Char- Lewis, M. ~1993!. The emergence of human emotions. In man, N. Yirmiya, & P. Zelazo ~Eds.!, Development M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland ~Eds.!, Handbook of emo- and autism: Perspectives from theory and research tions ~pp. 223–235!. New York: Guilford Press. ~pp. 173–193!. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Lord, C., Rutter, M., & LeCouteur, A. ~1994!. Autism Tov–Ruach, L. ~1980!. Jealousy, attention, and loss. In A. Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of O. Rorty ~Ed.!, Explaining emotions ~pp. 465–488!. a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Jour- Travis, L. L., & Sigman, M. ~1998!. Social deficit and nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, interpersonal relationship in autism. Mental Retarda- 185–212. tion and Developmental Disabilities, 4, 65–72. Masciuch, S., & Kienapple, K. ~1993!. The emergence of Volling, B. L., McElwain, N. L., & Miller, A. ~2002!. jealousy in children 4 months to 7 years of age. Jour- Emotion regulation in context: The jealousy complex nal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 421–435. between young siblings and its relations with child Miller, A. L., Volling, B. L., & McElwain, N. L. ~2000!. and family characteristics. Child Development, 73, Sibling jealousy in a triadic context with mothers and 581–600. fathers. Social Development, 9, 433–457. Wechsler, D. ~1974!. WISC-R manual: Wechsler Intelli- Neisser, U. ~1988!. Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philo- gence Scale for Children—Revised. San Antonio, TX: sophical Psychology, 1, 35–59. Psychological Corporation. Neu, J. ~1980!. Jealous thoughts. In A. O. Rorty ~Ed.!, Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasari, C., & Mundy, P. ~1992!. Explaining emotions ~pp. 425–463!. Berkeley, CA: Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children University of California Press. with autism. Child Development, 63, 150–165. | DPP16 ~ 1

Appendix A ! 4 20 445 Examples of children’s manifestations of jealousy on the explicitness scale for the drawing and playing scenarios

Autism Typical Development 0 Level of Explicitness 02 21 of Jealousy Draw Play Draw Play

Level 4 Stands in close proximity to “Mom, I love you . . .” Starts again after stopping own Stops own game, grabs objects 0 Behaviors or verbalizations mother and caresses her hair work and says “Just a minute, from the other child’s 13 0

that indirectly intervene Makes repeated loud comments I did not finish yet, Mom . . . construction game, and starts PROOF REVISED am 11:40 04 into the parent—rival child Makes loud comment “I am the about own game such as @plus eye gaze# to build own model interaction champ,” with eye gaze toward “Wow, what a beautiful slide the father ...” Stops own work, looks at mom and other child, grabs colors and stamps from the other child’s color box, and uses them in her picture

176 Level 5 “Now look at my picture,” while “Look what I did . . .” Says “Mom, look” and very “Yeah! This is something I Direct behaviors0 pushing picture toward gently places own picture could never do before with verbalizations aimed at mother’s face several times “Mom, look,” while pushing closer to mom’s sight my own game . . . Wow! focusing parent’s attention game toward the mother Mom, look . . .” to the drawing0playing of “Look at this.” the child in the “Mom, look . . .” experimental group “See mine.”

Level 6 “I did not think of that idea, so “Mom, why don’t you play with “Mine is prettier. Here, look.” “Mom @in sad tone of voice#, Direct declaration of now I will make the same me too?” why can’t I also play with comparison and lack of picture as his, so you “Is mine pretty too? Do you both of you?” equality, and0or any @mother# will say mine is “Mom . . . there is something know what I am painting? I negative affect pretty too” that is really making me am painting a doubledecker “. . . And what about me? Am I angry: Why do you only help bus.” alone?” with facial expression “I put a lot of effort into her?...Iwon’t be your of frustration drawing this, so why aren’t friend if you keep playing you saying anything about my only with her” ~whining! picture?” ~whining! “Mom, see what I am doing. “But I drew more . . .” Why don’t you ask me what I am doing? We used to play this together” | | DPP16 ~ 1 ! 4 21 445 Appendix B

Children’s examples for the experience of jealousy 0 102 21

Type of Example Autism Typical Development 0

Social–relations jealousy • When someone walks with his , his other friends • I was jealous when my little sister was born and everyone 13

feel jealous. They also want to be friends with the kid who paid attention only to her. 0 is my friend. • When my father hugged my brother, mom was not home, PROOF REVISED am 11:40 04 • When my best friend gets more attention, then I don’t. and I also wanted my dad. • When a kid from class is going to play with another kid • When my friend meets another friend of hers, I feel jealous, from class, and not with me, I feel sad. because I feel left alone. • When you are insulted by a friend and your other friends are on the side of your rival Social–cognitive jealousy • When somebody gets something and the other one does not • I’m jealous of my friend because she’s prettier than me. get anything, then it is possible to be very jealous. • When someone’s good at sports and I’m not • When kids in school can buy whatever they want whenever • In school when someone gets good grades and I don’t they want

177 • I feel jealous of my brother. He has more fun than I do, he goes to parties, and I help with the housework, like I do the laundry.

Appendix C Examples of children’s suggested solutions for coping with jealousy

Type of Solution Autism Typical Development

Situation activity To ask for the desired game as a present for Passover To save money and buy the desired object To go and play with another child To ask your friend if you can try his bicycle To join the child who has what I want To go see the movie at a different time Cognitive mentalistic Get out for a walk and wait for the feeling to pass Use self-talk to encourage yourself, such as “I’m also pretty” Ignore the event Think about other things and forget about it Say to yourself that you don’t want the other thing Convince yourself that it is not so important for you Get out of that place so you won’t see it or have to think Think about the other person’s disadvantages and remember the about it things that you like in yourself | Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.