What Is a Human Universal? Human Behavioral Ecology and Human Nature

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

What Is a Human Universal? Human Behavioral Ecology and Human Nature What is a Human Universal? Human Behavioral Ecology and Human Nature Elizabeth Cashdan, University of Utah In Arguing About Human Nature, S. M. Downes and E. Machery, eds. Routledge. 2013 In 1991 the cultural anthropologist Don a function of exposure to sunlight (tanning) is a Brown bucked anthropology's tabula rasa norm of reaction, as is the percentage of male and tradition by identifying over 300 \human female leopard geckos hatched at different incu- universals"|individual and sociocultural traits bation temperatures (see figure 1). Although that are found in every known human society gecko families vary in the the strength of their (Brown 1991; universals enumerated in Pinker response to temperature, the patterning of the 2003). The items he identified include both psy- response is similar: it is part of leopard gecko chological traits (e.g., wariness around snakes, nature for more males to be born when the tem- sweets preferred, sexual jealousy) and sociocul- perature is warmer, within this range of temper- tural ones (e.g., territoriality, females do more atures. Figure 2 shows another example, from a direct childcare, food sharing). Because they are brilliant study of soapberry bug mating behav- universal, it is plausible that these traits have ior by Carroll & Corneli (1999). A male soap- a biological basis and that they are evolved fea- berry bug may stay attached to his mate after tures of a universal human nature. mating in order to guard her against other suit- What, then, of the many domains where cul- ors. If there are many more males than females tures differ? The assumption is often made that (high sex ratio), this makes adaptive sense: he human nature is found solely in its universals { gains more by keeping other males away than in the traits found in every society. A trait that he loses in forgone mating opportunities. But is found in some societies and not others is then if eager suitors are less numerous (lower sex ra- assumed to be culturally constructed and with- tio), he gains little by mate-guarding and should out an evolutionary foundation. instead leave after mating and search for an- other mate. The upper figure shows the mate- Behavioral ecologists hold a different view: guarding response of male Oklahoma soapberry because human nature evolved to be flexible in bugs (each line is a family of half-sibs) to exper- predictable ways, the task of understanding hu- imental changes in the sex ratio. Mate-guarding man nature requires that we understand how increases as the sex ratio increases. Although evolution shaped that variation. The assump- each family has a slightly different reaction norm, tion is not just that we evolved to respond flex- they are similar enough to indicate a general ibly, but that selection shaped the nature and feature of what we might call Oklahoma soap- direction of that flexibility. To a behavioral ecol- berry bug nature. An observer will see differ- ogist, then, the predictable, patterned nature of ent behaviors in different environments, but the that response is the universal we must under- responsiveness|the shape of the reaction norm stand. In this view, we cannot understand our curve|is \universal." At least, it is univer- universal human nature without understanding sal in Oklahoma. The lower figure shows that the variability in its expression. soapberry bug families from Florida do not al- The concept is clarified by viewing variation ter their mate-guarding appreciably, irrespective as a norm of reaction|the pattern of expression of experimental changes in the sex ratio. Why of a genotype across a range of environments. not? In Florida, the climate, hence the sex ra- The increase in a person's skin pigmentation as 1 Figure 1: Sex ratio reaction norms as a function of incubation temperature in leopard geckos. Each line connects the sex ratio for a half-sib family (offspring sired by a single male) that was divided between two incubation temperatures. (Rhen et al., 2010) tio, is less variable than in Oklahoma. In Ok- This perspective on human nature derives lahoma, a facultative (plastic, flexible) response from the anthropologists' knowledge of human makes adaptive sense, because an individual bug variation and the ecological focus of the par- could find himself in a variety of environments ent discipline of behavioral ecology. Most hu- and he will reproduce better if he can respond man behavioral ecologists were trained as cul- to those changes. For the Florida bugs, living tural anthropologists, which gives them an un- in their equable environment, there is no advan- equaled knowledge of the breadth and regulari- tage to such flexibility, and in that population, ties of human variation. They know, better than the flexible response did not evolve (Carroll & anyone, that people living in developed, industri- Corneli, 1999). alized states (the usual subjects of human social science) represent only a very small part of the Soapberry bugs occupy a range of environ- range of human variation, and that those soci- ments, but the range is nothing compared to that eties are in many respects quite unusual. The na- of humans, who live in every part of the planet ture of the parent discipline of behavioral ecology and whose environments include novel ones that also shapes their perspective of human nature. they constructed for themselves. The principle Human behavioral ecology's modus operandi is illustrated in the previous example, therefore, is to model optimal outcomes by considering the even more important when discussing human na- costs and benefits of different strategies and how ture. To a human behavioral ecologist, then, hu- they trade off against one another. Doing this man nature is not limited to human universals, forces an explicit consideration of the ecological as that phrase is usually understood. The hu- factors that shape those costs and benefits, and man universal is the shape of the response, and how they vary across environments. the task is to understand how selection pressures shaped it. In what follows I will consider examples 2 Figure 2: Mate guarding reaction norms as a function of sex ratio in soapberry bugs in Oklahoma (top) and Florida (bottom). (Carroll & Corneli, 1999) 3 which illustrate the following implications of this havioral ecologists who work with humans also view of human nature: (1) human and non- typically hold the working assumption (not be- human animal behavior can be understood us- lief) that the human behavioral differences they ing the same evolutionary theoretical perspec- observe are the facultative expression of a largely tive, and, in some cases, models (hence human shared genotype. nature is seen as part of the evolved natural Finally, human behavioral ecologists as- world), (2) viewing behavioral variation as a re- sume that human nature is adaptive (fitness- action norm provides guidance on how policy can enhancing), and that selection will lead to op- address the darker side of human nature, and timal outcomes. These optimal outcomes are (3) the individual-maximizing process of natu- modeled as the best outcome possible given re- ral selection has created a remarkably altruis- source constraints and tradeoffs between com- tic, cooperative human nature. I will not, in peting demands. The assumption of optimality this essay, attempt to review the field of human is less a matter of belief about human nature behavioral ecology generally, which has been than it is a useful working assumption. Behav- done admirably elsewhere (Borgerhoff Mulder, ioral ecologists know, as well as anyone, that peo- 2003; Cronk, 1991; Laland & Brown, 2011; Smith ple sometimes do maladaptive things (although et al., 2001; Winterhalder & Smith, 2000). How- they do not expect maladaptive outcomes to be ever, several points should be made first about common), and they also know the reasons why the assumptions under which behavioral ecolo- evolution sometimes leads to sub-optimal out- gists operate. comes. But it is a reasonable and powerful work- Human nature, broadly speaking, encom- ing assumption in generating and testing hy- passes the ways in which people think, feel, and potheses about the functions of, and selection act. However, thoughts and feelings are them- pressures on, human behavior|hence how hu- selves \invisible" to natural selection, since they man nature came to be. This is best demon- can only affect survival or reproduction by mo- strated by example, so we will consider several, tivating behavior. An emotion, no matter how beginning with mating and marriage. strongly felt, is irrelevant to evolution if it does not cause an observable change. For this rea- Mating and Marriage: Biological mod- son, human behavioral ecologists are largely un- els can explain a lot about human cul- concerned with psychological mechanisms, and tural behavior focus instead on the behavioral outcomes that selection can act upon. Marriage is universal, but variable, across human For somewhat different reasons, human be- societies, and our understanding of what forms havioral ecologists also are largely unconcerned are natural or normal have both moral over- with genetic mechanisms. Although biologists tones and policy implications. The ethnographic studying the behavioral ecology of other species database makes it clear that human nature en- are increasingly interested in the genetic basis of compasses marriages that are both monogamous flexible responses, these are difficult to study in and polygynous, and even, under very rare and people, and we know very little about the ge- special circumstances, polyandrous. What be- netics underlying human nature and behavior. havioral ecology adds to this pluralistic view of Human behavioral ecologists therefore typically mating and marriage is the specification that adopt a \phenotypic gambit" that assumes there men and women will adjust their mating and has been sufficient genetic variation and time for marriage choices to environmental circumstances competing selection pressures to have resulted in in predictable ways, and that those choices will the evolution of better-adapted phenotypes.
Recommended publications
  • The Strength of Social Norms Across Human Groups
    PPSXXX10.1177/1745691617708631Gelfand et al.Strength of Social Norms 708631research-article2017 Perspectives on Psychological Science 2017, Vol. 12(5) 800 –809 The Strength of Social Norms Across © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: Human Groups sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617708631 10.1177/1745691617708631 www.psychologicalscience.org/PPS Michele J. Gelfand1, Jesse R. Harrington1, and Joshua Conrad Jackson2 1University of Maryland and 2University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Abstract Social norms are a defining feature of human psychology, yet our understanding of them is still underdeveloped. In this article, we present our own cross-cultural research program on tightness-looseness (TL)—which draws on field, experimental, computational, and neuroscience methods—to illustrate how going beyond Western borders is critical for understanding social norms’ functions and their multilevel consequences. Cross-cultural research enables us to account for the universal features of norm psychology but also explains the great cultural diversity we see in social norms around the globe. Keywords culture, diversity, neuroscience, individual differences, environment Imagine a world where everyone drives on both sides around town, to having conversations, to running an of the road. There are stoplights but no one pays atten- organization turn into an uncoordinated mess. tion to them. Trains, buses, and airplanes don’t operate Perhaps because of their important role in human with any fixed schedule. In conversations, people don’t behavior, social norms have been a central topic of greet each other, interrupt each other frequently, and study in psychology. The earliest studies of normative invade each other’s space.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Real Human Personality on Social Presence With
    International Conference on Artificial Reality and Telexistence Eurographics Symposium on Virtual Environments (2016) D. Reiners, D. Iwai, and F. Steinicke (Editors) The Influence of Real Human Personality on Social Presence with a Virtual Human in Augmented Reality Kangsoo Kim1 Gerd Bruder1 Divine Maloney2 Greg Welch1 1The University of Central Florida 2Sewanee: The University of the South Figure 1: Participant interacting with a virtual human in our experimental augmented reality environment. (A) The participant wears an optical see-through head-mounted display and sees the virtual human in either of two experimental conditions: (B) in the “Ignoring/Inconsistency” behavior condition, the virtual human passes through the physical chair (no occlusion) and does not ask for help from the participant, and (C) in the “Requesting/Consistency” behavior condition, the virtual human is naturally occluded by the physical objects and proactively asks help from the participant, to move the physical chair out of the way. Abstract Human responses to an interaction with a Virtual Human (VH) can be influenced by both external factors such as technology- related limitations, and internal factors such as individual differences in personality. While the impacts of external factors have been studied widely, and are typically controlled for in application scenarios, less attention has been devoted to the impacts of internal factors. We present the results of a human-subject experiment where we investigated a particular internal factor: the effects of extraversion–introversion traits of participants on the sense of social presence with a VH in an Augmented Reality (AR) setting. Our results indicate a positive association between a condition where the VH proactively requests help from the participant, and participants indicating higher social presence with the VH, regardless of their personality.
    [Show full text]
  • Reflections on Social Norms and Human Rights
    The Psychology of Social Norms and the Promotion of Human Rights Deborah A. Prentice Princeton University Chapter to appear in R. Goodman, D. Jinks, & A. K. Woods (Eds.), Understanding social action, promoting human rights. New York: Oxford University Press. This chapter was written while I was Visiting Faculty in the School of Social Sciences at the Institute for Advanced Study, Princeton, NJ. I would like to thank Jeremy Adelman, JoAnne Gowa, Bob Keohane, Eric Maskin, Dale Miller, Catherine Ross, Teemu Ruskola, Rick Shweder, and Eric Weitz for helpful discussions and comments on earlier drafts of the chapter. Please direct correspondence to: Deborah Prentice Department of Psychology Princeton University Green Hall Princeton, NJ 08540 [email protected] 1 Promoting human rights means changing behavior: Changing the behavior of governments that mistreat suspected criminals, opponents of their policies, supporters of their political rivals, and members of particular gender, ethnic, or religious groups; changing the behavior of corporations that mistreat their workers, damage the environment, and produce unsafe products; and changing the behavior of citizens who mistreat their spouses, children, and neighbors. In this chapter, I consider what an understanding of how social norms function psychologically has to contribute to this very worthy project. Social norms have proven to be an effective mechanism for changing health-related and environmental behaviors, so there is good reason to think that they might be helpful in the human-rights domain as well. In the social sciences, social norms are defined as socially shared and enforced attitudes specifying what to do and what not to do in a given situation (see Elster, 1990; Sunstein, 1997).
    [Show full text]
  • Identification and Prediction of Human Behavior Through Mining Of
    S S symmetry Review Identification and Prediction of Human Behavior through Mining of Unstructured Textual Data Mohammad Reza Davahli 1,*, Waldemar Karwowski 1 , Edgar Gutierrez 1,2 , Krzysztof Fiok 1 , Grzegorz Wróbel 3, Redha Taiar 4 and Tareq Ahram 1 1 Department of Industrial Engineering and Management Systems, University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL 32816, USA; [email protected] (W.K.); [email protected] (E.G.); fi[email protected] (K.F.); [email protected] (T.A.) 2 Center for Latin-American Logistics Innovation, LOGyCA, Bogota 110111, Colombia 3 Department of Logistics and Process Engineering, University of Information Technology and Management in Rzeszów, 35-225 Rzeszów, Poland; [email protected] 4 Sport Science Department, Université de Reims Champagne-Ardenne, 51100 Reims, France; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected] Received: 30 September 2020; Accepted: 17 November 2020; Published: 19 November 2020 Abstract: The identification of human behavior can provide useful information across multiple job spectra. Recent advances in applying data-based approaches to social sciences have increased the feasibility of modeling human behavior. In particular, studying human behavior by analyzing unstructured textual data has recently received considerable attention because of the abundance of textual data. The main objective of the present study was to discuss the primary methods for identifying and predicting human behavior through the mining of unstructured textual data. Of the 823 articles analyzed, 87 met the predefined inclusion criteria and were included in the literature review. Our results show that the included articles could be symmetrically classified into two groups. The first group of articles attempted to identify the leading indicators of human behavior in unstructured textual data.
    [Show full text]
  • Outline of Science
    Outline of science The following outline is provided as a topical overview of • Empirical method – science: • Experimental method – The steps involved in order Science – systematic effort of acquiring knowledge— to produce a reliable and logical conclusion include: through observation and experimentation coupled with logic and reasoning to find out what can be proved or 1. Asking a question about a natural phenomenon not proved—and the knowledge thus acquired. The word 2. Making observations of the phenomenon “science” comes from the Latin word “scientia” mean- 3. Forming a hypothesis – proposed explanation ing knowledge. A practitioner of science is called a for a phenomenon. For a hypothesis to be a "scientist". Modern science respects objective logical rea- scientific hypothesis, the scientific method re- soning, and follows a set of core procedures or rules in or- quires that one can test it. Scientists generally der to determine the nature and underlying natural laws of base scientific hypotheses on previous obser- the universe and everything in it. Some scientists do not vations that cannot satisfactorily be explained know of the rules themselves, but follow them through with the available scientific theories. research policies. These procedures are known as the 4. Predicting a logical consequence of the hy- scientific method. pothesis 5. Testing the hypothesis through an experiment – methodical procedure carried out with the 1 Essence of science goal of verifying, falsifying, or establishing the validity of a hypothesis. The 3 types of
    [Show full text]
  • Collective Action and the Evolution of Social Norm Internalization Sergey Gavriletsa,B,C,1 and Peter J
    Collective action and the evolution of social norm internalization Sergey Gavriletsa,b,c,1 and Peter J. Richersond aDepartment of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996; bDepartment of Mathematics, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996; cNational Institute for Mathematical and Biological Synthesis, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996; and dDepartment of Environmental Science and Policy, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 Edited by Simon A. Levin, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved May 4, 2017 (received for review March 7, 2017) Human behavior is strongly affected by culturally transmitted processing, and decision making (11) and the costs of monitor- norms and values. Certain norms are internalized (i.e., acting ing, punishments, or conditional rewards that would otherwise be according to a norm becomes an end in itself rather than merely necessary to ensure cooperation (9, 14). Internalization of norms a tool in achieving certain goals or avoiding social sanctions). allows individuals and groups to adjust their utility functions Humans’ capacity to internalize norms likely evolved in our ances- in situations with a rapidly changing environment when genetic tors to simplify solving certain challenges—including social ones. mechanisms would be too slow to react (9). A society’s values are Here we study theoretically the evolutionary origins of the capac- transmitted through the internalization of norms (15), with some ity to internalize norms. In our models, individuals can choose societies being more successful than others due to their norms to participate in collective actions as well as punish free rid- and institutions (16). The presence of both costs and benefits of ers.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology Defined ᮢ
    Psychology Defined ᮢ Gregg R. Henriques James Madison University A new form of knowledge technology is used to diagnose psychology’s epistemological woes and provide a solution to the difficulties. The argu- ment presented is that psychology has traditionally spanned two separate but intimately related problems: (a) the problem of animal behavior and (b) the problem of human behavior. Accordingly, the solution offered divides the field into two broad, logically consistent domains. The first domain is psychological formalism, which is defined as the science of mind, corre- sponds to animal behavior, and consists of the basic psychological sci- ences. The second domain is human psychology, which is defined as the science of human behavior at the individual level and is proposed as a hybrid that exists between psychological formalism and the social sciences. © 2004 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J Clin Psychol 60: 1207–1221, 2004. Keywords: Tree of Knowledge (ToK) System; psychological formalism; unified theory; mental behaviorism; Justification Hypothesis We persevere in looking at small questions instead of large ones and our view of the forest is forever obscured by the trees. (Bevan, 1991, p. 475) What is psychology? Is it a single, coherent scientific discipline awaiting transfor- mation from the current preparadigmatic state into a more mature unified one? Or, is it a heterogeneous federation of subdisciplines that will ultimately fragment into a multitude of smaller, more specialized fields? This is, in essence, the “to be or not to be” question of the field. Currently, psychology exists as an uneasy compromise between unification and fragmentation. On the one hand, the existence of numerous societal institutions sug- gests that psychology is a singular entity at some level.
    [Show full text]
  • Childhood Experiences of Introversion : an Exploration of Navigating Social and Academic Spaces and Ways of Coping
    Smith ScholarWorks Theses, Dissertations, and Projects 2015 Childhood experiences of introversion : an exploration of navigating social and academic spaces and ways of coping Leah S. Schwartz Smith College Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Schwartz, Leah S., "Childhood experiences of introversion : an exploration of navigating social and academic spaces and ways of coping" (2015). Masters Thesis, Smith College, Northampton, MA. https://scholarworks.smith.edu/theses/919 This Masters Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations, and Projects by an authorized administrator of Smith ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Leah Schwartz Childhood Experiences of Introversion: An Exploration of Navigating Social and Academic Spaces and Ways of Coping ABSTRACT The purpose of this study is to explore the experiences of introversion in childhood. This research seeks to understand how introverted children perceive themselves and how this impacts the way they interact with the world around them. The overarching research question is: how does the experience of being an introverted child impact one’s sense of self? Given the practical and ethical challenges of interviewing actual children, for this qualitative research I interviewed 12 adult self-identified introverts about their childhood experience of being introverted. The design for this research study is qualitative and exploratory. Findings included what being introverted meant to participants and how they coped with feelings of difference academically, behaviorally, and socially. This research fills an important gap in the literature and provides justification for paying closer attention to the unique needs of introverted children to help them feel supported, respected, and valued.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Cognitive Theory of Personality
    1 SOCIAL COGNITIVE THEORY OF PERSONALITY Albert Bandura Stanford University Bandura, A. (1999). A social cognitive theory of personality. In L. Pervin & O. John (Ed.), Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 154-196). New York: Guilford Publications. (Reprinted in D. Cervone & Y. Shoda [Eds.], The coherence of personality. New York: Guilford Press.) 2 Many psychological theories have been proposed over the years to explain human behavior. The view of human nature embodied in such theories and the causal processes they postulate have considerable import. What theorists believe people to be determines which aspects of human functioning they explore most thoroughly and which they leave unexamined. The conceptions of human nature in which psychological theories are rooted is more than a theoretical issue. As knowledge gained through inquiry is applied, the conceptions guiding the social practices have even vaster implications. They affect which human potentialities are cultivated, which are underdeveloped, and whether efforts at change are directed mainly at psychosocial, biological or sociostructural factors. This chapter addresses the personal determinants and mechanisms of human functioning from the perspective of social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986). The recent years have witnessed a resurgence of interest in self-referent phenomena. Self- processes have come to pervade diverse domains of psychology because most external influences affect human functioning through intermediary self processes rather than directly. The self system thus lies at the very heart of causal processes. To cite but a few examples, personal factors are very much involved in regulating attentional processes, schematic processing of experiences, memory representation and reconstruction, cognitively-based motivation, emotion activation, psychobiologic functioning and the efficacy with which cognitive and behavioral competencies are executed in the transactions of everyday life.
    [Show full text]
  • Measuring Social Norms
    Measuring Social Norms Cristina Bicchieri University of Pennsylvania Running Agenda • What is the theory of social norms? • What are social norms? • How do we measure whether there is a social norm? • How do we measure sustainability? © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 2 (Penn SONG) Explaining norms (Bicchieri-Muldoon, 2011) • Macro view • Micro view • Functions • Reasons to conform • Evolution – Cost-benefit • Emergence – Emotions – Expectations The content and behavioral effects of social norms remain a black box © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 3 (Penn SONG) Opening the Black Box • Intervention goal needs micro-analysis – For successful interventions to curb harmful behaviors, we must figure out people’s reasons for doing what they do • To develop diagnostic tools – Must be guided by specific models of behavior • Effective tools good measures © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 4 (Penn SONG) Guidance: Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen 1991) Attitude Subjective Intention Behaviour Norms Perceived Behavioural Control 5 Guidance: Modified Belief/Preference Model (Bicchieri 2006) Personal normative beliefs Social expectations Factual beliefs: action X leads to outcome… Empirical and/or normative Preference Choice © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 6 (Penn SONG) The Theory of Social Norms • The theory of social norms is a theory of what motivates collective patterns of behavior. • It tries to answer a very basic question – Why do people do what they do? • We use very simple, measurable concepts to answer that question. © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 7 (Penn SONG) Our simple (measurable) concepts (Bicchieri 2006, 2016) • Preference (conditional) • Personal Normative Belief • Social Expectations • Reference Network © C. Bicchieri, Penn Social Norms Group 8 (Penn SONG) So Why Do People Do What They Do? • People do what they do because they prefer to act that way.
    [Show full text]
  • HUMAN BEHAVIOR in the SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT (3 Credits)
    MASTER SYLLABUS Social Work 3510 - HUMAN BEHAVIOR IN THE SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT (3 credits) I. Course Domain and Boundaries The purpose of this course is to introduce the social work student to knowledge of human development as a base for social work practice. Human problems are viewed within their environmental context. Individuals, families, groups, organizations and communities are in constant transaction with each other in the physical and social environmental context. Thus, people and environments reciprocally affect one another, and adequate assessments of human problems and plans of intervention must consider the reciprocal impact of people and a range of environmental systems. Life span development from conception to very old age is examined. The course focuses on theories and knowledge related to biological, sociological, psychological, spiritual and cultural processes and development across the life span. It addresses the environmental conditions that support or inhibit individual and family growth; and variations arising form ethnicity, class, cohort, gender, race, sexual orientation, religion, and other differences. Particular attention is given to factors that contribute to persons being at risk and the issues and concerns of multi-cultural, urban populations. COMPETENCIES 7: Apply knowledge of human behavior and the social environment II. A Benchmarks: Adolescent paper: students address significant personal life events during adolescence and integrate three theories *construct a genogram to assess interrelationships II. Learning Outcomes By the end of course, students will be able to: Describe the unique roles and Services that social workers provide (Competency 7-A) Apply theories and knowledge of biological, sociological, cultural, psychological and spiritual development across the life span, including attachment theory, role theory, cognitive theory, learning theory, and theories of moral development.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Targeting As an Effective Approach to Digital Mass Persuasion
    Psychological targeting as an effective approach to digital mass persuasion S. C. Matza,1, M. Kosinskib,2, G. Navec, and D. J. Stillwelld,2 aColumbia Business School, Columbia University, New York City, NY 10027; bGraduate School of Business, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; cWharton School of Business, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA 19104; and dCambridge Judge Business School, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, CB2 3EB, United Kingdom Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved October 17, 2017 (received for review June 17, 2017) People are exposed to persuasive communication across many from that displayed in the laboratory (7). Consequently, it is different contexts: Governments, companies, and political parties questionable whether—andtowhatextent—these findings can be use persuasive appeals to encourage people to eat healthier, generalized to the application of psychological persuasion in real- purchase a particular product, or vote for a specific candidate. world mass persuasion (see ref. 8 for initial evidence). Laboratory studies show that such persuasive appeals are more A likely explanation for the lack of ecologically valid research effective in influencing behavior when they are tailored to individ- in the context of psychological persuasion is the questionnaire- uals’ unique psychological characteristics. However, the investiga- based nature of psychological assessment. Whereas researchers tion of large-scale psychological persuasion in the real world has can ask participants to complete a psychological questionnaire in been hindered by the questionnaire-based nature of psychological the laboratory, it is unrealistic to expect millions of people to do assessment. Recent research, however, shows that people’spsycho- so before sending them persuasive messages online.
    [Show full text]