Implicit and Explicit Memory for New Associations in Normal and Amnesic Subjects Peter Graf and Daniel L

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Implicit and Explicit Memory for New Associations in Normal and Amnesic Subjects Peter Graf and Daniel L Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1985 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Learning. Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/85/$00.75 1985. Vol. II. No. 3. 501-518 Implicit and Explicit Memory for New Associations in Normal and Amnesic Subjects Peter Graf and Daniel L. Schacter University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada Two experiments examined whether repetition priming effects on a word completion task are influenced by new associations between unrelated word pairs that were established during a single study trial. On the word completion task, subjects were presented with the initial three letters of the response words from the study list pairs and they completed these fragments with the first words that came to mind. The fragments were shown either with the paired words from the study list (same context) or with other words (different context). Both experiments showed a larger priming effect in the same-context condition than in the different-context condition, but only with a study task that required elaborative processing of the word pairs. This effect was observed with college students and amnesic patients, suggesting that word completion performance is mediated by implicit memory for new associations that is independent of explicit recollection. Remembering is commonly assumed to fragments of recently presented words and of involve the conscious or explicit retrieval of new words, subjects succeed more often with information about particular experiences. presented words than with new words. This This assumption derives largely from tradi- facilitation in completion performance is re- tional memory tests, such as free recall, cued ferred to as a repetition or direct priming recall, and recognition; a denning character- effect (cf. Cofer, 1967; Cramer, 1966). istic of these tests is the explicit instruction Direct priming effects have also been dem- to retrieve information about a particular onstrated with other tasks, such as word experience. However, this assumption is not identification, which requires subjects to consistent with the results of studies that have identify words that are presented very briefly, assessed performance with tests that make and lexical decision, which requires subjects no reference to particular experiences. On to decide whether or not a presented letter these tests memory is expressed by a facili- string constitutes a word. A variety of terms tation in performance that need not be ac- has been used to distinguish between the type companied by conscious recollection. For ex- of memory that is tapped by priming tests ample, on a word completion test subjects on the one hand, and by recall and recognition are presented word fragments (e.g., BRE tests on the other hand (e.g., Baddeley, 1982; or B E D for BREAD) and are instructed Bruner, 1969; Cohen & Squire, 1980; Graf, to complete them with the first words that Squire, & Mandler, 1984; Jacoby & Wither- come to mind. When required to complete spoon, 1982; Mandler, 1980; Moscovitch, 1982; O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978; Schacter, in press-a; Schacter & Moscovitch, 1984; Tulv- This research was supported by Grant U0299 to Peter Graf and Grant UO361 to Daniel L. Schacter from the ing, 1983; Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1982; Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Wickelgren, 1979). For descriptive purposes, Canada and by a Special Research Program Grant from we use the terms implicit memory and explicit the Connaught Fund, University of Toronto. memory to distinguish between these forms We thank Reed Hunt, Larry Jacoby, Morris Moscov- of memory. Implicit memory is revealed when itch, Norm Slamecka, and Endel Tulving for their com- ments on earlier versions of this paper, Karen Raaflaub- performance on a task is facilitated in the Walsh for testing the amnesic patients, and Carol Mac- absence of conscious recollection; explicit donald for helping to prepare this manuscript. memory is revealed when performance on a Requests for reprints should be sent to Peter Graf, task requires conscious recollection of pre- Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5S 1A1. vious experiences. The term priming effect is 501 502 PETER GRAF AND DANIEL L. SCHACTER used in the present article to refer to the neurological dysfunctions. Amnesic patients faeilitative effects of a learning episode on show a pattern of cognitive functioning that performance of a word completion or similar is typically characterized by poor memory task. for recent events along with relatively normal Three converging lines of evidence suggest intellectual functions and premorbid knowl- that implicit memory differs fundamentally edge and skills (e.g., Moscovitch, 1982; from explicit memory. First, performance on Schacter, 1983; Squire, 1982; Squire, Cohen, implicit and explicit memory tasks is affected & Nadel, 1984; Warrington & Weiskrantz, differentially by a variety of experimental 1982). One of the hallmarks of anterograde manipulations (e.g., Graf & Mandler, 1984, amnesia is the patients' inability to acquire 1985; Graf, Mandler, & Haden, 1982; Graf, and remember new associations; failure to Squire, & Mandler, 1984; Jacoby & Dallas, remember a list of unrelated word pairs on 1981; Roediger & Blaxton, 1983; Scarbor- an immediate recall test is diagnostic of ough, Gerard, & Cortese, 1979; Tulving, amnesia (e.g., Meyer & Yates, 1955; Rozin, Schacter, & Stark, 1982). Second, perfor- 1976; Scoville & Milner, 1957; Starr & Phil- mance on tests of implicit retention can be lips, 1970). Nevertheless, amnesic patients statistically independent of recognition per- show relatively normal priming or implicit formance (Eich, 1984; Jacoby, & Wither- retention, when the study materials are fa- spoon, 1982; Schacter, McLachlan, Moscov- miliar items that have a preexisting memory itch, & Tulving, 1984; Tulving et al., 1982). representation, such as words (e.g., Graf, Third, patients with organic amnesia, who Squire, & Mandler, 1984; Rozin, 1976; War- are severely impaired on explicit memory rington & Weiskrantz, 1970), highly related tests, are less impaired, and frequently nor- paired associates (e.g., Shimamura & Squire, mal, on various implicit tests (e.g., Brooks & 1984; Winocur & Weiskrantz, 1976), or lin- Baddeley, 1976; Cohen, 1984; Cohen & guistic idioms (e.g., Schacter, in press-a). The Squire, 1980; Graf, Shimamura, & Squire, finding of normal priming effects, in con- 1984; Graf, Squire, & Mandler, 1984; Jacoby junction with the observation that amnesic & Witherspoon, 1982; Moscovitch, 1982, patients seem unable to acquire new associ- 1984; Schacter, in press-a; Schacter, Harbluk, ations, is consistent with the view that implicit & McLachlan, 1984; Warrington & Weis- memory is mediated by activation of preex- krantz, 1968, 1970, 1974; Weiskrantz & War- isting representations. The reasoning here is rington, 1979). straightforward. A new association has no preexisting memory representation that can A widespread interpretation of the forego- be activated and thus mediate implicit ing findings is that implicit memory is based memory. on the activation of preexisting memory rep- resentations (e.g., Graf et al., 1982; Graf, It is important to observe, however, that Squire, & Mandler, 1984; Mandler, 1980; the negative evidence for the retention of new Mortensen, 1980; Morton, 1969, 1979;Rozin, associations in amnesia has been obtained 1976; Warrington & Weiskrantz, 1982; Wick- on explicit memory tasks, such as paired elgren, 1979). The study materials used in associate learning; these findings thus do not most previous experiments were familiar preclude the possibility that there is implicit items, such as individual words, that are memory for new associations. Several recent represented in long-term or semantic memory studies are relevant to this issue, but the prior to their appearance in a study list. It findings are inconsistent. On the one hand, has been argued that these preexisting rep- there is some evidence for the implicit reten- resentations are activated as a result of pre- tion of newly acquired associations in amnesic senting the study items, and that activation patients who exhibit profound deficits on occurs automatically and thus independently tests of explicit memory (e.g., Moscovitch, of processes that mediate explicit remem- 1984; Schacter, Harbluk, & McLachlan, 1984; bering. Weiskrantz & Warrington, 1979). To illus- An activation interpretation receives sup- trate, Moscovitch required patients to mem- port from several studies of amnesic patients. orize word pairs and later to read these Amnesia occurs as a consequence of various words, either in the same or in different RETENTION OF NEW ASSOCIATIONS 503 pairings, from a degraded display. He found tation, we expected an increase in completion that amnesic patients were faster at reading performance even in the different-context the words in the same pairs than in different condition. The critical question is, however, pairs, suggesting that they had acquired new whether completion performance in the same- associations between the paired words in the context condition exceeds performance in the study list. On the other hand, several studies different-context condition. To the extent that with college students provide conflicting evi- information about the pairing of words in dence concerning the implicit retention of the study list contributes to priming, test new associations (Carroll & Kirsner, 1982; fragments should be completed with words Franks, Plybon, & Auble, 1982;
Recommended publications
  • VERBAL BEHAVIOR by B. F. Skinner William James Lectures Harvard
    VERBAL BEHAVIOR by B. F. Skinner William James Lectures Harvard University 1948 To be published by Harvard University Press. Reproduced by permission of B. F. Skinner† Preface In 1930, the Harvard departments of psychology and philosophy began sponsoring an endowed lecture series in honor of William James and continued to do so at irregular intervals for nearly 60 years. By the time Skinner was invited to give the lectures in 1947, the prestige of the engagement had been established by such illustrious speakers as John Dewey, Wolfgang Köhler, Edward Thorndike, and Bertrand Russell, and there can be no doubt that Skinner was aware that his reputation would rest upon his performance. His lectures were evidently effective, for he was soon invited to join the faculty at Harvard, where he was to remain for the rest of his career. The text of those lectures, possibly somewhat edited and modified by Skinner after their delivery, was preserved as an unpublished manuscript, dated 1948, and is reproduced here. Skinner worked on his analysis of verbal behavior for 23 years, from 1934, when Alfred North Whitehead announced his doubt that behaviorism could account for verbal behavior, to 1957, when the book Verbal Behavior was finally published, but there are two extant documents that reveal intermediate stages of his analysis. In the first decade of this period, Skinner taught several courses on language, literature, and behavior at Clark University, the University of Minnesota, and elsewhere. According to his autobiography, he used notes from these classes as the foundation for a class he taught on verbal behavior in the summer of 1947 at Columbia University.
    [Show full text]
  • Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory
    Syracuse University SURFACE Psychology College of Arts and Sciences 2008 Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory Michael J. Kahana University of Pennsylvania Marc W. Howard Syracuse University Sean M. Polyn University of Pennsylvania Follow this and additional works at: https://surface.syr.edu/psy Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons Recommended Citation Kahana, Michael J.; Howard, Marc W.; and Polyn, Sean M., "Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory" (2008). Psychology. 3. https://surface.syr.edu/psy/3 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Arts and Sciences at SURFACE. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology by an authorized administrator of SURFACE. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Associative Retrieval Processes in Episodic Memory Michael J. Kahana Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Marc W. Howard Department of Psychology Syracuse University Sean M. Polyn Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania Draft: Do not quote Abstract Association and context constitute two of the central ideas in the history of episodic memory research. Following a brief discussion of the history of these ideas, we review data that demonstrate the complementary roles of temporal contiguity and semantic relatedness in determining the order in which subjects recall lists of items and the timing of their successive recalls. These analyses reveal that temporal contiguity effects persist over very long time scales, a result that challenges traditional psychological and neuroscientific models of association. The form of the temporal contiguity effect is conserved across all of the major recall tasks and even appears in item recognition when subjects respond with high confidence.
    [Show full text]
  • How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory
    How Trauma Impacts Four Different Types of Memory EXPLICIT MEMORY IMPLICIT MEMORY SEMANTIC MEMORY EPISODIC MEMORY EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCEDURAL MEMORY What It Is What It Is What It Is What It Is The memory of general knowledge and The autobiographical memory of an event The memory of the emotions you felt The memory of how to perform a facts. or experience – including the who, what, during an experience. common task without actively thinking and where. Example Example Example Example You remember what a bicycle is. You remember who was there and what When a wave of shame or anxiety grabs You can ride a bicycle automatically, with- street you were on when you fell off your you the next time you see your bicycle out having to stop and recall how it’s bicycle in front of a crowd. after the big fall. done. How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It How Trauma Can Affect It Trauma can prevent information (like Trauma can shutdown episodic memory After trauma, a person may get triggered Trauma can change patterns of words, images, sounds, etc.) from differ- and fragment the sequence of events. and experience painful emotions, often procedural memory. For example, a ent parts of the brain from combining to without context. person might tense up and unconsciously make a semantic memory. alter their posture, which could lead to pain or even numbness. Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area Related Brain Area The temporal lobe and inferior parietal The hippocampus is responsible for The amygdala plays a key role in The striatum is associated with producing cortex collect information from different creating and recalling episodic memory.
    [Show full text]
  • Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses Graduate School 1994 Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders. Shannon Buckles Sebastian Louisiana State University and Agricultural & Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses Recommended Citation Sebastian, Shannon Buckles, "Explicit Memory Bias for Body-Related Stimuli in Eating Disorders." (1994). LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses. 5826. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_disstheses/5826 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Historical Dissertations and Theses by an authorized administrator of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of the Time-Of-Day of Training on Explicit Memory
    Time-of-dayBrazilian Journal of training of Medical and memory and Biological Research (2008) 41: 477-481 477 ISSN 0100-879X Effect of the time-of-day of training on explicit memory F.F. Barbosa and F.S. Albuquerque Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, RN, Brasil Correspondence to: F.F. Barbosa, Departamento de Fisiologia, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Lagoa Nova, Caixa Postal 1511, 59072-970 Natal, RN, Brasil Fax: +55-84-211-9206. E-mail: [email protected] Studies have shown a time-of-day of training effect on long-term explicit memory with a greater effect being shown in the afternoon than in the morning. However, these studies did not control the chronotype variable. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to assess if the time-of-day effect on explicit memory would continue if this variable were controlled, in addition to identifying the occurrence of a possible synchronic effect. A total of 68 undergraduates were classified as morning, intermediate, or afternoon types. The subjects listened to a list of 10 words during the training phase and immediately performed a recognition task, a procedure which they repeated twice. One week later, they underwent an unannounced recognition test. The target list and the distractor words were the same in all series. The subjects were allocated to two groups according to acquisition time: a morning group (N = 32), and an afternoon group (N = 36). One week later, some of the subjects in each of these groups were subjected to a test in the morning (N = 35) or in the afternoon (N = 33).
    [Show full text]
  • Behavior Systems, Associationism, and Pavlovian Conditioning
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1994, 1 (4), 405-420 Behavior systems, associationism, and Pavlovian conditioning WILLIAM TIMBERLAKE Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana Associative and behavior systems accounts of Pavlovian conditioning have different emphases. The traditional associative account has focused on the role of the unconditional stimulus (US) in strengthening stimulus associations according to a set of general laws. The behavior systems account has focused on the relation of conditional responding to the preorganized perceptual, motor, and mo­ tivational organization engaged by the US. Knowledge of a behavior system enables successful pre­ diction of the form and ease of conditioning as a function of the type of conditional stimulus (CS), US, and the CS-US relation. At the same time, Pavlovian manipulations act as a window on how a behavior system works. Both associative and behavior systems accounts can be criticized as in­ complete and idiosyncratic. A comprehensive account of Pavlovian conditioning could profit from their integration. Laboratory-associationist and adaptive-evolutionary zation and motivational processes underlying behavior is accounts of Pavlovian conditioning differ markedly in a critical step in understanding conditioning. Presenta­ their focus. The former has emphasized the association tion of the US is viewed as engaging and constraining between the conditional stimulus (CS) and the uncondi­ preorganized mechanisms underlying behavior, rather tional stimulus (US), and the role of the US in strength­
    [Show full text]
  • TRAUMATIC AMNESIA a Dissociative Survival Mechanism
    TRAUMATIC AMNESIA a dissociative survival mechanism Dr. Muriel Salmona, psychiatrist, Paris, January 19, 2018 I - Presentation Complete or fragmented traumatic amnesia is a common memory disorder found in victims of violence. Numerous clinical studies have described this well- known phenomenon since the beginning of the twentieth century, when the first systematic studies explored amnesia among traumatized combat soldiers, and later among victims of sexual violence. Studies found almost 40 % complete amnesia among these participants, and 60% partial amnesia when childhood sexual abuse was concerned (Brière 1993, Williams 1994, IVSEA 2015). These amnesias are psychotraumatic consequences of violence. They are neuro- psychological survival mechanisms based on dissociation (Van der Kolk, 1995, 2001). Since 2015, dissociative traumatic amnesias have been recognized as a defining criterium for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (DSM-5, 2015). They can last several decades and lead to amnesia for entire sections of childhood, with almost no retrievable memory, which results in a painful impression of having no past, nor landmarks. When amnesia fades away, traumatic memories most often return brutally, invasively, as uncontrolled, unintegrated, fragmented traumatic memories (flashbacks, nightmares) and trigger re-experiences of violence with identitical distress and sensations, and a vividness as if the trauma was happening all over again. Care professionals should be better acquainted with these phenomena in order to improve how they care for the victims, by treating such reminiscences without confusing them with hallucinations, identifying violence, and its psychotraumatic consequences for the victims. Similarly, justice professionals, when faced with sexual violence complaints after recovered memories, should be more aware of the reality of such frequent memory disorder.
    [Show full text]
  • Rethinking Trauma
    How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Daniel Siegel, MD - Main Session - pg. 1 Rethinking Trauma How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma the Main Session with Daniel Siegel, MD and Ruth Buczynski, PhD National Institute for the Clinical Application of Behavioral Medicine How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Daniel Siegel, MD - Main Session - pg. 2 Rethinking Trauma: Daniel Siegel, MD How to Use Brain Science to Help Patients Accelerate Healing after Trauma Table of Contents (click to go to a page) How To Define What Happens in the Brain During Trauma ................................... 3 The Optimally Functioning vs. Traumatized Brain as Defined by FACES ................. 4 How Age Affects the Impact of Trauma on the Brain .............................................. 7 Trauma Leads to the Chemical Release of Cortisol and Adrenaline ....................... 8 How Trauma Impairs the Brain’s Memory Systems ............................................... 10 Why the Brain’s Developmental Period Is Crucial to Working with Trauma ............ 11 The Layers of Memory and Encoding Implicit Memory .......................................... 13 The Retrieval of Implicit Memory and the Experience of Flashback ...................... 15 Putting the Science into Clinical Practice ............................................................... 17 What Happens in the Brain during Dissociation? ................................................... 18
    [Show full text]
  • Interactions Between Attention and Memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne
    Interactions between attention and memory Marvin M Chun and Nicholas B Turk-Browne Attention and memory cannot operate without each other. In form of selective attention to internal representations this review, we discuss two lines of recent evidence that [1,2]. support this interdependence. First, memory has a limited capacity, and thus attention determines what will be Classic psychologists such as William James stated long encoded. Division of attention during encoding prevents the ago that ‘we cannot deny that an object once attended to formation of conscious memories, although the role of attention will remain in the memory, while one inattentively in formation of unconscious memories is more complex. Such allowed to pass will leave no traces behind’ [3]. More memories can be encoded even when there is another recently, leading neuroscientists such as Eric Kandel have concurrent task, but the stimuli that are to be encoded must be stated that one of the most important problems for 21st selected from among other competing stimuli. Second, century neuroscience is to understand how attention memory from past experience guides what should be attended. regulates the processes that stabilize experiential mem- Brain areas that are important for memory, such as the ories [4]. Here, we review studies from the past two years hippocampus and medial temporal lobe structures, are that reveal progress towards understanding the inter- recruited in attention tasks, and memory directly affects frontal- actions between attention and memory in neural systems. parietal networks involved in spatial orienting. Thus, exploring the interactions between attention and memory can provide Attention at encoding new insights into these fundamental topics of cognitive A major question in many people’s minds is how to neuroscience.
    [Show full text]
  • How Do We Learn from Experience?
    TSI Graphics: Worth—Kolb/Whishaw: An Introduction to Brain and Behavior C H A P T E R 13 How Do We Learn from Experience? What Are Learning and Memory? The Structural Basis of Brain Studying Memory in the Laboratory Plasticity Two Types of Memory Measuring Synaptic Change What Makes Explicit and Implicit Enriched Experience and Plasticity Memory Different? Sensory or Motor Training and Plasticity Plasticity, Hormones, Trophic Factors, and Drugs Dissociating Memory Circuits Focus on Disorders: Patient Boswell’s Amnesia Recovery from Brain Injury The Three-Legged Cat Solution Neural Systems Underlying The New-Circuit Solution Explicit and Implicit Memories The Lost-Neuron-Replacement Solution A Neural Circuit for Explicit Memories Focus on Disorders: Alzheimer’s Disease A Neural Circuit for Implicit Memories Focus on Disorders: Korsakoff’s Syndrome A Neural Circuit for Emotional Memories Dimitri Messinis/AP Photo Micrograph: Oliver Meckes/Ottawa/Photo Researchers 488 ■ D TSI Graphics: Worth—Kolb/Whishaw: An Introduction to Brain and Behavior onna was born on June 14, 1933. Her memory of Over the ensuing 10 months, Donna regained most of the period from 1933 to 1937 is sketchy, but her motor abilities and language skills, and her spatial abil- D those who knew her as a baby report that she was ities improved significantly. Nonetheless, she was short- like most other infants. At first, she could not talk, walk, or tempered and easily frustrated by the slowness of her use a toilet. Indeed, she did not even seem to recognize her recovery, symptoms that are typical of people with closed father, although her mother seemed more familiar to her.
    [Show full text]
  • CHILDREN's MEMORY, TRAUMATIC MEMORIES and the CHILD WITNESS
    CHILD MEMORY, TRAUMATIC MEMORY and the CHILD WITNESS George E. Davis, MD 2017 Children’s Law Institute INTERPRETING THE RESEARCH • The complications of memory study • Test conditions and questions don’t always match real life situations and motivations • Divergent interests • Although broad rules can be proposed for different ages and conditions, reliability depends on the individual case REFERENCES 1. Memory and Suggestibility in the Forensic Interview (Eisen, Quas and Goodman, 2002) 2. Stress, Trauma and Children’s Memory Development (Howe, Goodman and Cicchetti, 2008) 3. Children as Victims, Witnesses, and Offenders (Bottoms, Najdowski and Goodman, 2009) MEMORY PROCESS • MEMORY COMPONENTS – Encoding • PerceptionConstruct • Child age and knowledge – Storage • Combination, sorting, comparison in the hippocampus • Short term, long term and working memory – Retrieval • Reconstruction from associations and neural networks that are frontal and temporal TYPES of MEMORY • TYPES OF MEMORY – Explicit / Declarative / Conscious • Episodic—events • Semantic—facts • Intentional, whether learned or recalled • Organized and encoded by hippocampus and medial temporal – Implicit / Contextual / Unconscious • Procedural—riding a bike, driving to work, tying shoes, etc • Encoded and stored in motor control and brain stem – Conditioned responses—eg, fear of certain people – Autobiographical—the continuous sense of self over time DISSOCIATION • Dissociation is a breakdown in memory, consciousness and sense of self provoked by extreme fear, pain or psychological
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: CROSS-PAIR ASSOCIATIONS 1 Are Associations Formed Across Word Pairs? a Test of Learning by Temporal Contiguity In
    Running head: CROSS-PAIR ASSOCIATIONS 1 Are associations formed across word pairs? A test of learning by temporal contiguity in associative recognition Adam F. Osth Julian Fox The University of Melbourne Word count: 3,663 Address correspondence to: Adam Osth (E-mail: [email protected]) Author Note We are extremely grateful to Vencislav Popov for sharing his data upon request and all authors who have publicly shared their data that we re-analyze in this work. We would also like to thank Nicola Singleton for assistance with data collection for Experiment 1. This work was supported by ARC DE170100106 awarded to Adam Osth. Data from this article can be found on our OSF page (https://osf.io/64qyf/). CROSS-PAIR ASSOCIATIONS 2 Abstract In models such as the search of associative memory (SAM: Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984) model, associations in paired associate tasks are only formed between the pair of to-be-remembered items. The temporal context model (TCM: Howard & Kahana, 2002) deviates from SAM by positing that long range associations are formed between the current item and all previously presented items, even in paired associate tasks, where cross-pair associations are formed in addition to within-pair associations (Davis, Geller, Rizzuto, & Kahana, 2002). We tested this proposal in an associative recognition task by constructing rearranged pairs where the distance in within-list serial position between the two pair members was manipulated between one and five pairs. Models such as TCM predict that FAR should be highest for rearranged pairs that are constructed from pair members that were adjacent to each other on the study list, while models such as SAM predict that FAR should be equal for rearranged pairs regardless of whether they’re constructed from adjacent or remote pairs.
    [Show full text]