Topics in Syllable Geometry Jstuart Davis

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Topics in Syllable Geometry Jstuart Davis c o "'0 oC --l ~ ..:::L ... o >­ ~ cu (6)l~ c :c '" ~"~ Nl.WYt'l~~~!~~" 11·••••••••• _ DEDICATION Copyright © 1988 Stuart Davis All Rights Reserved To my parents and brothers. Library of Congress Cataloging-in.publication Data Davis, Stuart. Topics in syllable geometry JStuart Davis. p. em. - (Outstanding dissertations in linguistics) Originally presented as the author's thesis (ph. D. -University of Arizona, 1985) 8ibliography: p. ISBN 0-8140-5181-5 1. Grammar, Comparative and general-Syllable. I. Rhyme. 3. Accents and accentuation. I. Title. II. Series. PI36.D38 1988 414-dc19 88--16509 Printed on acid-free, ISO-year.life paper Manufactured in the United States of America ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank several people for making the completion of this dissertation possible. I thank the three members of my cOmMittee, Dick Demers, Dick Oehrle, and Deirdre Wheeler, as well as Richard Janda and Natsuko Tsujimura for their valuable comments and encouragement. Much of the dissertation was shaped through discussion with them. I would also like to thank Susan Steele and Ann Farmer for their input on earlier versions of Chapter two. Furthermore, I offer thanks to the Graduate College of the Univer­ sity of Arizona and to Sigma xi for financial support. r~ NllNY;;k~~!!~D" Ill•••••••••••••••• TABLE OF CONTENTS Page CHAPTER 1. OVERVIEW 1 1.0. Introduction 1 1.1. Brief Background of the Syllable in Phonological Theory . • . 1 1.2. Background on Syllable-Internal Constituency . 8 2. ON THE ARGUMENTS FOR THE RHYME AS A SUB SYLLABIC CONSTITUENT 17 2.0. Introduction 17 2.1. Phonotactics 19 2.1.0. Introduction 19 2.1.1. Constraints between Onset and Other Syllable Parts for English . • . 23 2.1.2. Constraints between Onset and Nucleus for Other Languages . 27 2.1.3. Constraints between Onset and Coda for Other Languages 29 2.1.4. Conclusion . 30 2.2. Rule Types Referring to the Rhyme 31 2.2.1. Stress Rules .....• 32 2.2.2. Phonetic-Spreading Rules 46 2.2.3. Compensatory Lengthening Rules . 48 2.2.4. Conclusion . 55 2.3. The Rhyme as a Durational Unit 56 2.3.1. Background on Durational Experiments of VC-Sequences 58 2.3.2. English •.••.• 64 2.3.3. Korean •..... 68 2.3.4. French and Russian 70 2.3.5. Other Languages 72 2.3.6. Surrnnary 74 2.4. Conclusion . 75 ............- N!W 'i'n.... ••':;-:'::::.""" ,,---------­ TABLE OF CONTENTS--Continued CHAPTER 1 Page OVERVIEW S~LABLE STRUCTURE AND STRESS RULES FROM DIFFERENT LANGUAGES 79 1.0. Introduction The goal of this cha~ter is to furnish back­ 3 • 0 • Introduction . 79 3.1. Standard Stress Systems 82 3.1.0. Introduction •. 82 ground information on the role of the syllable in recent 3.1.1. Same Syllab:e Stress S3 3.1.2. Long Vowel Stress B5 phonologlcal theory, and, then. to put my thesis in proper 3.1.3. Closed Syllable stresS 87 3.1.4. Heavy syllable Stress B9 perspective by presentin9 vdxious views on the nature of Oncet-Sensi~ive Stress Languages 97 3. 2. ~yllable-internal con~tituency. 3.2.0. IntrOductio~. 97 In this dissertation, 3.2.1. Western Aranda 97 3.2.2. Alyawarra 107 : arque ~hat none of these various Views provides an 3.2.3. Madimadi :16 3.2.4. Piraha' 127 adequate representation of the syllable. 1 prOpose that 3.3. Conclusion •• 132 the syllable universally consists ot three (sister) "MOVEMENT" PHENOMENA AS EVIDENCE FOR SYLLABLE-INTBRNAL CONSTITUENCY 13' components: an onset (o~tional), a nucleus, and a coda 4.0 • Introduction • • • • 138 (optional) • 4 • 1 • Speech Errors .,. 139 4.1.0. Introduction 139 4.1.1. Some Problems With the Use of 1.1. Brief Background of the Syllable speech Errors as E~idence 143 in Phonolosical Theory 4.1.2. Speech Error Evidence and the Rhyme • • • • • • • • • • 146 Much recent work in phonology has focused on the 4.:.3. Speech Error E~idence and sUbsy11abic Constituency 151 ntudy Of the syllable. In the early years of generative 4.1.4. Conclusion. 161 4.2. Lan9uage Games • • • • • • • • • 162 phonology, though, the role of the syllable in phonolo­ 4.2.0. Introduction •.•••• 162 4.2.1. Typology of Language Games 163 qiC~l description was largely ignored. one reason for 4.2.1.1. Expunsion 163 4.2.1.2. Contraction 167 this was that the structuralists never l>ucceeded il1 4.2_1.3. substitution 168 4.2.1.4. Rearrangement 169 ~aoqudtely defil1ing the syllable as a physical ~eality. 4.2.1.5. Combinations •• 173 4.2.2. Language Games and subsy11abic '!/(ttnon 11951:21. for e~ample, attempted to define the Constituency 176 4 • 3 • Concl\:sion 184 !lVl J.'lblo Physically by associating it with a chest pulse 1ST OF REFERENCES . 185 1 2 3 induced by muscular activity. "careful experimentation consonant clusters do not break down into a combination with all types of syllables and consonants makes it cer­ of a ~ossible word-final cluster followed by a word­ l tain that every syllable has its chest pulse delimited initial cluster. by chest muscles (intercostals) or by the constriction Even though the syllable cannot be defined (complete or partial) of the consonant or by both_" physically, it can still function as a relevant phonolo­ Contrary to Stetson's experiments, Ladefoged (1967) has gical unit. And, in fact, significant phonological shown that chest pulses are not always an accurate generalizations are missed when phonological rules are diagnostic for the syllable: some syllables are produced not allowed to refer to syllables. Por example, it has without a chest pulse, and some chest pulses do not been noted by Kahn (1976) and others that many rules correspond to syllables. Jespersen, as cited in Pulgram that are written with the environment {i} miss the (1970) defined the syllable in terms of vocalic sonority; generalization that the environment for the rules is in other words, te believed that sounds grouped themselves actually a syllable boundary. Another case of a missed into syllables by their sonority. However, this view has qeneralization, discussed both by Vennemann (1972) and also been criticized (e.g., in Ladefoged 1975) since Wheeler (1981) among others, relates to Chomsky and sonority can either increase or decrease before reaching llJlle's (1968) formulation of the main stress rule in the syllabic peak - e.g., in the word ££l there is an ~nglish. Main stress for nouns and certain adjectives in increase in sonority between the Ikl and the /r/, but J<;nglish is usually assigned to a penultimate vowel if it in the word ~ there is a decrease in sonority between tn tensed or followed by at least two consonants; the lsi and the /p/. Yet both words constitute a single syllable. Haugen (1956) and others defined the syllable 1. Not all structuralists, however, accepted Ilw oyllable. For example, Malmberg (1955) cites a number in terms of distribution of phonemes. In other words, fI(1 ,1.inguists who view the syllable as not being signifi­ I~nt since no physical correlate of it could be found. they viewed the syllable as a unit over which phonotactic !ilmJ,larly, Kohler (1966) conclUdes that the syllable is "1mpQusible" and "harmful. II He argues this on the basis constraints hold. This view, though, has been criticized 1/1' the claim that syllable division is often not discer­ lIn)J10 and is arbitrary. For further discussion of the by Bell (1976) for several reasons. One reason is that (I I ff'oring views on the phonetic reality of the syllable rH1f1 1)u19ram (1970) and Vogel (1977). such a view is unable to handle languages in which medial ~ '':~ 5 4 Another example of a generalization that is missed utherwise, the antepenultimate vowel is stressed. The wlthout reference to the syllable is found in Hooper's oxnmples in (1) illustrate this: / (1972) discussion of Harris' (1969) analysis of Spanish (1) Ariz6na ag~nda vigilant nasalization. Harris observes that nasals assimilate llowever, note the following exceptions: before adjacent obstruents in the same word or across (2) discipline IGdicrous ~loquent word boundaries as is shown below (examples from Hooper Exceptions, like those in (2), led Chomsky and Halle to formulate the concept of "weak cluster" which is essen­ 1972:525) : [umbeso] 'a kiss' tially defined as in (3): (4) un beso [uncarko] 'a pool' (3) Weak cluster un charco [u'jgato] 'a cat' v Cl un gato o ['" voc ] [-tense] rA cons Harris also observes that, before glides, nasal assimi­ They then refer to the notion of weak cluster in their lation takes place only across word boundaries and not main stress rule (as well as in four other rules ­ word internally, as the two examples in (5) illustrate: Auxiliary Reduction, Pre-Cluster Laxing, u-Tensing, and (5) a. miel [myel] 'honey' Tensing before CIV). But by not recognizing the syllable b. un hielo [unyelo] 'an ice' Chomsky and Halle miss a generalization. Weak clusters Hooper notes that in Harris' system the rules for the are just those clusters that are possible syllable-initial two cases of nasal assimilation (in (4) and (5b) above) clusters. That is, both their members are syllabified cannot be collapsed, although they describe the same with the following (ultimate) vowel, so that the penulti­ process. Hooper argues, however, that once the syllable mate vowel is in an open syllable. Thus, in the data is recognized (and it is formally recognized by syllable above, stress is on the antepenultimate vowel when the boundary insertion rules), an obvious generalization penultimate vowel is both lax and in an open syllable. follows: Nasal assimilation occurs before a consonant This generalization was missed by Chomsky and Halle or a glide if a syllable boundary intervenes.
Recommended publications
  • Chapter One Phonetic Change
    CHAPTERONE PHONETICCHANGE The investigation of the nature and the types of changes that affect the sounds of a language is the most highly developed area of the study of language change. The term sound change is used to refer, in the broadest sense, to alterations in the phonetic shape of segments and suprasegmental features that result from the operation of phonological process es. The pho- netic makeup of given morphemes or words or sets of morphemes or words also may undergo change as a by-product of alterations in the grammatical patterns of a language. Sound change is used generally to refer only to those phonetic changes that affect all occurrences of a given sound or class of sounds (like the class of voiceless stops) under specifiable phonetic conditions . It is important to distinguish between the use of the term sound change as it refers tophonetic process es in a historical context , on the one hand, and as it refers to phonetic corre- spondences on the other. By phonetic process es we refer to the replacement of a sound or a sequenceof sounds presenting some articulatory difficulty by another sound or sequence lacking that difficulty . A phonetic correspondence can be said to exist between a sound at one point in the history of a language and the sound that is its direct descendent at any subsequent point in the history of that language. A phonetic correspondence often reflects the results of several phonetic process es that have affected a segment serially . Although phonetic process es are synchronic phenomena, they often have diachronic consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonology, Phonetics, and Diachrony of Sturtevant's
    Indo-European Linguistics 7 (2019) 241–307 brill.com/ieul The phonology, phonetics, and diachrony of Sturtevant’s Law Anthony D. Yates University of California, Los Angeles [email protected] Abstract This paper presents a systematic reassessment of Sturtevant’s Law (Sturtevant 1932), which governs the differing outcomes of Proto-Indo-European voiced and voice- less obstruents in Hittite (Anatolian). I argue that Sturtevant’s Law was a con- ditioned pre-Hittite sound change whereby (i) contrastively voiceless word-medial obstruents regularly underwent gemination (cf. Melchert 1994), but gemination was blocked for stops in pre-stop position; and (ii) the inherited [±voice] contrast was then lost, replaced by the [±long] opposition observed in Hittite (cf. Blevins 2004). I pro- vide empirical and typological support for this novel restriction, which is shown not only to account straightforwardly for data that is problematic under previous analy- ses, but also to be phonetically motivated, a natural consequence of the poorly cued durational contrast between voiceless and voiced stops in pre-stop environments. I develop an optimality-theoretic analysis of this gemination pattern in pre-Hittite, and discuss how this grammar gave rise to synchronic Hittite via “transphonologization” (Hyman 1976, 2013). Finally, it is argued that this analysis supports deriving the Hittite stop system from the Proto-Indo-European system as traditionally reconstructed with an opposition between voiceless, voiced, and breathy voiced stops (contra Kloekhorst 2016, Jäntti 2017). Keywords Hittite – Indo-European – diachronic phonology – language change – phonological typology © anthony d. yates, 2019 | doi:10.1163/22125892-00701006 This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY-NCDownloaded4.0 License.
    [Show full text]
  • Information to Users
    INFORMATION TO USERS This manuscript has been reproduced from the microfilm master. UM l films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type o f computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely afreet reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UME a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are reproduced by sectioning the original, b^inning at the upper left-hand comer and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back o f the book. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy, ffigher quality 6” x 9” black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UM l directly to order. UMl A Bell & Howell Infoimation Company 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor MI 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600 Velar-Initial Etyma and Issues in Comparative Pama-Nyungan by Susan Ann Fitzgerald B.A.. University of V ictoria. 1989 VI.A.
    [Show full text]
  • What Drives Compensatory Lengthening? Beyond Moraic Conservation Rebeka CamposAstorkiza the Ohio State University Department of Spanish and Portuguese
    1 What drives compensatory lengthening? Beyond moraic conservation Rebeka Campos-Astorkiza The Ohio State University Department of Spanish and Portuguese Abstract Compensatory Lengthening (CL) refers to processes where deletion of a segment leads to lengthening of another segment. The deleted segment (trigger) and the lengthened segment (target) can be either a vowel or a consonant. Previous research has focused mainly on cases of vowel lengthening, paying less attention to consonant lengthening. The goal of this paper is to argue for two different motivations driving consonant lengthening in different languages, based on the typological comparison between vowel and consonant lengthening developed in Campos-Astorkiza (2005). Traditionally, CL has been explained through moraic conservation (Hayes 1995), according to which the trigger is always a moraic segment and upon its deletion, the stranded mora is attached to an adjacent element resulting in lengthening. However, moraic conservation has been challenged and, based on typological considerations, the current study argues that not all instances of consonant CL have the same motivation. I propose that phonologization and perceived similarity play a role in different cases. According to the phonologization account (see Kavistskaya 2002 for vowel lengthening), the phonetic consonant duration is reanalyzed as phonological upon loss of the conditioning environment. This analysis is illustrated with data from the Greek dialects of Lesbian and Thessalian. The perceived similarity approach, based on work by Steriade (2001), argues that the result of compensatory lengthening is more similar to the original sequence than the result of only deleting. Hungarian presents a case of compensatory lengthening where perceived similarity offers an explanation to the observed pattern.
    [Show full text]
  • L Vocalisation As a Natural Phenomenon
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of Essex Research Repository L Vocalisation as a Natural Phenomenon Wyn Johnson and David Britain Essex University [email protected] [email protected] 1. Introduction The sound /l/ is generally characterised in the literature as a coronal lateral approximant. This standard description holds that the sounds involves contact between the tip of the tongue and the alveolar ridge, but instead of the air being blocked at the sides of the tongue, it is also allowed to pass down the sides. In many (but not all) dialects of English /l/ has two allophones – clear /l/ ([l]), roughly as described, and dark, or velarised, /l/ ([…]) involving a secondary articulation – the retraction of the back of the tongue towards the velum. In dialects which exhibit this allophony, the clear /l/ occurs in syllable onsets and the dark /l/ in syllable rhymes (leaf [li˘f] vs. feel [fi˘…] and table [te˘b…]). The focus of this paper is the phenomenon of l-vocalisation, that is to say the vocalisation of dark /l/ in syllable rhymes 1. feel [fi˘w] table [te˘bu] but leaf [li˘f] 1 This process is widespread in the varieties of English spoken in the South-Eastern part of Britain (Bower 1973; Hardcastle & Barry 1989; Hudson and Holloway 1977; Meuter 2002, Przedlacka 2001; Spero 1996; Tollfree 1999, Trudgill 1986; Wells 1982) (indeed, it appears to be categorical in some varieties there) and which extends to many other dialects including American English (Ash 1982; Hubbell 1950; Pederson 2001); Australian English (Borowsky 2001, Borowsky and Horvath 1997, Horvath and Horvath 1997, 2001, 2002), New Zealand English (Bauer 1986, 1994; Horvath and Horvath 2001, 2002) and Falkland Island English (Sudbury 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • The Diachrony of Initial Consonant Loss in Cape York Peninsula (Australia)
    The diachrony of initial consonant loss in Cape York Peninsula (Australia) Jean-Christophe Verstraete (University of Leuven) This paper revisits the diachrony of initial consonant loss, a type of sound change that is found in several areas in Australia (Hale 1976a, Alpher 1976, Blevins 2001), but is rare from a world-wide perspective (Blevins 2007). So far, the literature has mainly analysed initial loss as the outcome of a gradual process of initial weaking, caused by a shift of stress away from the initial syllable (Alpher 1976, Blevins & Marmion 1994). In this paper, I use data from a set of eight Paman (Pama-Nyungan) languages of Cape York Peninsula (Australia), which illustrate not just loss of initial consonants, but also initial consonant lenition and the loss of entire initial syllables. Using these data, I argue that (i) the classic model of gradual initial weakening needs to be supplemented with more abrupt mechanisms, specifically analogy-driven loss based on synchronic alternations, and contact-induced loss, and (ii) the causal link with stress shift needs to be refined, and in some languages loss of initial consonants in part of the lexicon may itself cause changes in the stress system. Cape York Peninsula, in Australia’s northeast, has several ‘hotspots’ of initial loss (Alpher 1976, Sutton 1976), e.g. in the north and on the central east coast. This paper focuses on the eastern hotspot in the Princess Charlotte Bay area, specifically eight languages from three different subgroups of Paman, viz. Middle Paman (Umpithamu, Yintyingka, Umpila), Lamalamic (Lamalama, Umbuygamu, Rimanggudinhma) and Thaypanic (Kuku Thaypan, Aghu Tharrnggala).
    [Show full text]
  • Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990
    Studies in African Linguistics Volume 21, Number 3, December 1990 CONTEXTUAL LABIALIZATION IN NA WURI* Roderic F. Casali Ghana Institute of Linguistics Literacy and Bible Translation and UCLA A spectrographic investigation into the non-contrastive labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri (a Kwa language of Ghana) sup­ ports the notion that this labialization is the result of a phonological, feature­ spreading rule and not simply an automatic transitional process. This as­ sumption is further warranted in that it allows for a more natural treatment of some other phonological processes in the language. The fact that labial­ ization before round vowels is generally not very audible is explained in terms of a principle of speech perception. A final topic addressed is the question of why (both in Nawuri and apparently in a number of other Ghanaian languages as well) contextual labialization does tend to be more perceptible in certain restricted environments. o. Introduction This paper deals with the allophonic labialization of consonants before round vowels in Nawuri, a Kwa language of Ghana.! While such labialization is gener­ ally not very audible, spectrographic evidence suggests that it is strongly present, * The spectrograms in this study were produced at the phonetics lab of the University of Texas at Arlington using equipment provided through a grant of the Permanent University Fund of the University of Texas system. I would like to thank the following people for their valuable comments and suggestions: Joan Baart, Don Burquest, Mike Cahill, Jerry Edmondson, Norris McKinney, Bob Mugele, Tony Naden, and Keith Snider. I would also like to express my appreciation to Russell Schuh and an anonymous referee for this journal for their helpful criticism of an earlier version, and to Mary Steele for some helpful discussion concerning labialization in Konkomba.
    [Show full text]
  • Yorùbá Vowel Deletion Involves Compensatory Lengthening: Evidence from Phonetics
    Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus, Vol. 60, 2020, 1-12 doi: 10.5842/60-0-753 Yorùbá vowel deletion involves compensatory lengthening: Evidence from phonetics Nick Danis Linguistics Program, Washington University in St. Louis, USA E-mail: [email protected] Abstract A phonetic pilot study of Yorùbá vowel deletion shows that the vowel that remains after an adjacent vowel deletes is slightly but significantly longer than a short vowel in non-deletional contexts (p < 0.001). In the configuration studied here, deletion occurs in the vowel of a CV verb when occurring before a V-initial direct object (/CV1 +V2 / → [CV2]). However, instead of full vowel deletion as it is previously analysed (e.g. Akinlabi and Oyebade 1987, Ola Orie and Pulleyblank 2002), a compensatory lengthening analysis is proposed based on this new phonetic evidence. The experiment for this study controlled for inherent vowel duration, voicing, and manner of articulation of the surrounding consonants. These results are in line with a similar result regarding Yorùbá tone (Ajíbóyè et al. 2011) in the same syntactic (verb + direct object) configuration. Keywords: phonology, Yorùbá, deletion, compensatory lengthening, vowel duration 1. Introduction This work examines the phonetics of a vowel deletion process in Yorùbá (Atlantic-Congo, Nigeria). Specifically, in a pilot study, the duration of an underived short vowel is compared with the duration of a vowel that remains after deletion of an adjacent vowel (called here the “remnant vowel”) in a VV sequence. (1) a. /V1V2/ → [V2] /ta+ata/ → [tata] ‘sell pepper’ b. /V3/ → [V3] /tata/ → [tata] ‘grasshopper’ The duration of the remnant vowel (V2 above) is slightly but significantly longer than the duration of the underived short vowel (V3 above).
    [Show full text]
  • Between Natural and Unnatural Phonology: the Case of Cluster-Splitting Epenthesis Juliette Blevins the Graduate Center, CUNY
    Chapter 1 Between natural and unnatural phonology: The case of cluster-splitting epenthesis Juliette Blevins The Graduate Center, CUNY A widely recognized feature of loan-word phonology is the resolution of clusters by vowel epenthesis. When a language lacking word-initial clusters borrows words from a language with initial #TRV sequences, T an oral stop and R a liquid, it is common to find vowel epenthesis, most typically vowel-copy, as in, for example: Basque <gurutze> ‘cross’ from Latin <cruce(m)>; Q’eqchi’ <kurus> ‘cross’ from Spanish <cruz> ‘cross’, or Fijian <kolosi> ‘cross’ from English <cross>. The phonological rule or sound change responsible for this pat- tern is sometimes called “cluster-splitting epenthesis”: #TRVi > #TV(i)RVi. The most widely accepted explanation for this pattern is that vowel epenthesis between the oral stop andthe following sonorant is due to the vowel-like nature of the TR transition, since #TRVi is per- ceptually similar to #TV(i)RVi. A fact not often appreciated, however, is that cluster-splitting epenthesis is extremely rare as a language-internal development. The central premise of this chapter is that #TRVi in a non-native language is heard or perceived as #TV(i)RVi when phonotactics of the native language demand TV transitions. Without this cognitive compo- nent, cluster-splitting epenthesis is rare and, as argued here, decidedly unnatural. 1 Introduction Diachronic explanations have been offered for both natural and unnatural sound pat- terns in human spoken languages. Building on the Neogrammarian tradition, as well as the experimental research program of Ohala (e.g. 1971; 1974; 1993), it is argued that natural sound patterns, like final obstruent devoicing, nasal place assimilation, vowel harmony, consonant lenition, and many others, arise from regular sound changes with clear phonetic bases (Blevins 2004, 2006, 2008, 2015; Anderson 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • 3. Historical Phonology
    -38- 3. HISTORICAL PHONOLOGY Because this thesis is primarily concerned with morphological reconstruction, detailed phonological reconstructions fall outside the scope of investigation. Nonetheless, it is important to make a few remarks regarding the sound changes which may be traced, since many of these changes affect morphemes as well as whole words. Some phonological changes will simply be noted and very briefly described; others, such as the voicing contrasts (section 3.2.4), are important for the reconstruction of forms in this thesis and will be discussed at greater length. 3.1 LITERATURE REVIEW The first and only reconstruction of the phonology of Proto-Karnic was undertaken by Professor Peter Austin and published in 1990; this is Austin (1990a). Austin reconstructs over 300 lexical items for the Lake Eyre Basin and draws a family tree based on lexical retentions and common sound changes.30 In addition, there are several sound changes which do not fit the family tree, and are assumed to have diffused. Austin also evaluates the evidence for the inclusion of Arabana-Wangkangurru as Karnic, and concludes that this language is not part of the Karnic subgroup. Austin also includes a few of grammatical forms, including pronouns and the pronominal accusative marker -nha. Useful as this paper is, there are a number of faults. First there is a matter of internal grouping of languages. Yandruwandha and Yawarrawarka are clearly more similar to each other than either are to Mithaka (since they are regarded by a number of people as dialects of the same language31), yet Austin does not group them together (recall figure 1.2 on page 10).
    [Show full text]
  • Compensatory Lengthening in Harmonic Serialism∗
    COMPENSATORY LENGTHENING IN ∗ HARMONIC SERIALISM BERN SAMKO BSAMKO @ UCSC . E D U June 2011 1 Introduction Informally, compensatory lengthening (CL) is a phenomenon that consists of two parts: the deletion of a weight-bearing segment, and the concomitant lengthening of another, usually adjacent segment. Unsurpris- ingly, analyses of CL are usually derivational in that lengthening only occurs after it is triggered by deletion of a neighboring segment. Less discussed is the fact that, because CL is triggered only by the loss of moraic segments, both of these steps must refer to a syllabified and moraically specified form. Only coda conso- nants can be associated with moras, and a particular segment’s status as a coda can only be determined after the form has been syllabified. It is crucial, therefore, that syllabification and assignment of moras happen before segmental deletion. In constraint-based phonology, this requirement for prosodic structure means that faithfulness vio- lations must be evaluated with respect to a form that contains prosodic structure at least up to the level of the syllable. Doing so is not possible in classic Optimality Theory (OT, Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004), a framework that disallows intermediate levels of representation. Moreover, it is not immediately obvious that Harmonic Serialism (HS; McCarthy 2000, et seq.), an iterative version of OT, allows for this particular intermediate stage. Even in HS, there is no guarantee that the correct syllabic and moraic structure will be built before segmental changes occur. I will show that a ranking paradox prevents the building of prosodic structure from intrinsically preceding deletion of segments.
    [Show full text]
  • Quantitative Metathesis in Ancient Greek
    Quantitative Metathesis in Ancient Greek Anita Brown Bryn Mawr College December 2017 Abstract This paper explores the phenomenon known as 'quantitative metathesis' in Ancient Greek. Historically this change, an apparent metathesis of vowel length, has been considered to be true metathesis by classicists, but recent scholarship has cast suspicion on this notion, not least because metathesis of vowel length is not a known change in any other language. In this paper, I present a review of previous scholarship on Greek quantitative metathesis, in addition to a cross-linguistic survey of general metathesis, with special attention to autosegmental theory. I conclude that Greek quantitative metathesis is not true metathesis, but rather a retention and reassociation of abstract timing units through the two individual (and well-attested) processes of antevocalic shortening and compensatory lengthening. 1 Contents 1 Introduction 3 1.1 Introduction to Ancient Greek 5 1.2 Introduction to Quantitative Metathesis 6 1.2.1 A-stem 6 1.2.2 Athematic stems 7 1.3 Introduction to Autosegmental Phonology 8 2 Greek meter and accent 9 2.1 Meter 9 2.2 Accent 11 3 Overview of metathesis 12 3.1 CV metathesis in Rotuman 13 3.1.1 As deletion and reattachment (Besnier 1987) 13 3.1.2 As compensatory metathesis (Blevins & Garrett 1998) 13 3.2 CV metathesis in Kwara'ae ............... 14 3.3 Compensatory lengthening from CV metathesis in Leti . 15 3.4 VV metathesis .. 16 3.5 Syllabic metathesis 17 4 Analyses of quantitative metathesis 18 4.1 QM as timing-slot transfer ..... 18 4.2 QM as CL/preservation of quantity.
    [Show full text]