Aksel Øijord: on the Ontology of Colours

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Aksel Øijord: on the Ontology of Colours Aksel Øijord: On the ontology of colours: Are colours heterogeneous or homogeneous? Are they two-dimensional or three-dimensional? Abstract My answer to the first question that is posed in the title of this thesis is that colours are homogeneous, which means each and every colour is only one in number. This means that colours are not heterogeneous, that is, they are not compounds or mixtures. For example: orange is often said to be red and yellow, and grey is often said to be white and black. In other words: orange and grey are both claimed to be heterogeneous. However, my conclusion that colours are homogeneous simply excludes that heterogeneity can be the case. My answer to the second question is that colours are two-dimensional, which means that colours stretch out in length and breadth, but not in depth. This conclusion gainsays naïve realistic conceptions about colours, for example that they can be objects like a piece of blue cobalt, or that there can be voluminous coloured light beams passing in three-dimensional space from a light source and, when they hit objects, mix with their colours. For example, one use to say that yellow and purple beams colour a landscape at sunset. The conclusion on two-dimensionality also gainsays the more sophisticated theory of identification of colours with brain events. That is, colours cannot be identified with brain events because the latter are three- dimensional while the former are two-dimensional. These two conclusions are drawn from three general propositions, which I call Basic Suppositions. The first says there is concomitance between colours and their extensions. This means that any colour has a certain extension and that this extension cannot be separated 1 from the colour itself. It follows that colours are homogeneous because if heterogeneous, like the contention on orange, the implication will be orange is twice its own extension, and this contradicts the first basic supposition. The second says that colours can only relate beside each other. This basic supposition gainsays naïve realistic conceptions which include that colours might exist behind each other and have different directions in three-dimensional space. The third says that only colours can limit colours, which means there can be no empty space or “clear air” between any two colours, i.e., it cannot be a blank or a gap between them, which is not a colour. In addition, my inquiry results in two other basic suppositions, namely that colours might be identical notwithstanding difference in figure, size or position, and that two or more different colours cannot be identical with one and the same colour. All these propositions will be clarified and defended in the discussion to follow. Method I proceed by naming colours. For example; this black is infield to which this white is outfield: X In this example, I denote the figure in question, the capital letter X, simply by naming it black. And also, I name its surroundings by the colour name white. That is, I am not trying to define particulars. And the common name colours refers to all particulars including white, black and grey. From there I detect their relational properties by using the substantival mode. The important thing to note is that when the substantival mode is used colour names function as subjects in sentences. This mode needs not only be used to characterizing colours by their positional relations, like the infield-outfield relation. Other characterizations are also possible. For example, a certain red in a white outfield may be a square. This means it has a certain relation to white, namely a square relation. But I can also characterize the same red by a sort of causal relation and for example contend it is a positive after image. The opposite is the adjectival mode which characterizes things or what is taken to be observable physical objects, by their colour properties, which is a naïve realistic approach. For example; this tomato is red, but that tomato is green. By this mode colour names function as predicates or adjectives in sentences. In some naïve realistic sciences, for example those of Goethe and Chevreul, whom I discuss in this thesis, the two modes are used together. For example, a contention is that red pigment mixes with yellow pigment into orange. Here red and yellow are used as adjectives respectively characterizing two different chemical materials, and orange characterizes the mix of those. However, a general contention might be that orange is a compound of red and yellow, and this latter contention conforms more clearly to the substantival mode. As I explain in General Introduction, section 1.5 below, the tradition from Hering to Hardin, does not, in the first place, bring causes into their determinations of particular 2 colours, and so, it is the substantival mode which is in use. However, determinations like orange is both red and yellow is some sort of defining colours by other colours, and gives reasons to believe in heterogeneity of colours. On the other hand, adherents of the tradition sometimes claim that such determination is purely psychological, i.e., without ontological implications. I discuss both options in the first section of General Introduction. In psychophysics, colours are characterized by their causes, and so also that discipline can be said to use the substantival mode. But these causes are theoretical entities, that is, they are in principle not observable. The talk is about differences of wavelengths of radiant energy, different purities of any one dominant wavelength, and differences of luminance which concerns intensity of radiant energy. These and purely neurophysiological causes are in themselves not colours and therefore not of concern to my exploration, though I give the principal explanatory structure considerable attention, especially in section 1 of General Introduction. My general contentions, i.e., the basic suppositions, are arrived at by observations and determinations of particular colour relations. And therefore, induction is fundamental to my method. From the basic suppositions I finally draw my conclusions. The structure of this thesis The text is divided in two, namely General Introduction and Chapters. General Introduction comprise the solutions to the problems discussed and the arguments for those solutions and is therefore not a short foreword, but a comprehensive text in where all the basic suppositions, except for number V, are formulated and defended. My reason for the divide is that the chapters relate to my findings and by that expand in orientation, addressing particular problems in colour philosophy. In section 1 of General Introduction, I address the contention that colours are heterogeneous, and argue that the terminologies both in naïve realistic sciences on colour, and in modern psychophysics, suggest that colours are judged heterogenous, and that this for apparent reasons can be a conviction about the ontology of colours. In section 2, I explain and defend my method and move into several themes related to the question if colours are homogeneous or heterogeneous, until I, in the last section, present and justify my argument in favour of colours’ homogeneity. In this run I also justify my general contention that colours can be identical notwithstanding difference in figure, size or position. In section 3.1, I first present my argument that colours are two-dimensional, and in sections 3.1.1 and 3.1.2, I give substantial justifications for the basic suppositions I use, respectively that colours can only relate beside each other and that only colours can limit colours. In this argumentation I address naïve realistic conceptions while paying them very much respect. In the last section 3.2, I consider most of psychologist Katz’s outlines of naïve realistic colour conceptions and conclude that those stand strong both in daily life and in science on colour, and that the belief in 3 colours’ two-dimensionality must be reserved for special colour conceptions, for example in psychophysics and eye-brain surgery. In the second part, which is divided into chapters, I try to show the relevance of my findings relating them to different themes. In chapter I, section 1, I present in brief all the basic suppositions. In section 2, I proceed to basic definitions, and in section 3, I present the main implications that can be drawn from the basic suppositions and definitions. I must confess that some definitions are not presented and defended in General introduction and that the implications are more than the two that answer the questions posed in the title of this thesis. However, I think the explanations I give in chapter I, are likely to be easily understood and accepted as sufficient justifications. In chapter II, I consider causal explanations in naïve realistic and realistic sciences. In relation to the former I gather conceptions from both Aristotle, Goethe and Chevreul. These are contrasted with the latter, represented by renaissance philosophy on colours, with focus on epiphenomenalism. I end the chapter by giving a brief outline of Eliminativism, a position defended both by Hardin and Arstila. In chapter III, I first discuss Hardin’s definition of unique colours, thereafter I repeat my critique on the lacking conceptual criteria for dividing colours into chromatic and achromatic. Then I present and discuss some theories of opponent colours within pigment colour systems. Further, I address some difficulties within colour systematics which arise from the detection of so-called forbidden colours. In the next sections I address the Swedish Natural Colour System and explain my reasons for not accepting that the use of the term natural is sufficiently counted for. In chapter IV, I discuss the problem of sorites series, which concerns degrees of likeness between colours. I refute the idea that two colours can be different while at the same time matching or being identical with a third colour.
Recommended publications
  • Neuro-Ophthalmology-Email.Pdf
    NEURO- OPHTHALMOLOGY Edited by DESMOND P. KIDD, MD, FRCP Consultant Neurologist Department of Clinical Neurosciences Royal Free Hospital and University College Medical School London, United Kingdom NANCY J. NEWMAN, MD LeoDelle Jolley Professor of Ophthalmology Professor of Ophthalmology and Neurology Instructor in Neurological Surgery Emory University School of Medicine Atlanta, Georgia VALE´RIE BIOUSSE, MD Cyrus H. Stoner Professor of Ophthalmology Professor of Ophthalmology and Neurology Emory University School of Medicine Atlanta, Georgia 1600 John F. Kennedy Blvd. Ste 1800 Philadelphia, PA 19103-2899 NEURO-OPHTHALMOLOGY ISBN: 978-0-7506-7548-2 Copyright # 2008 by Butterworth-Heinemann, an imprint of Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Permissions may be sought directly from Elsevier’s Rights Department: phone: (þ1) 215 239 3804 (US) or (þ44) 1865 843830 (UK); fax: (þ44) 1865 853333; e-mail: [email protected]. You may also complete your request on-line via the Elsevier website at http://www.elsevier.com/permissions. Notice Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experience broaden our knowledge, changes in practice, treatment and drug therapy may become necessary or appropriate. Readers are advised to check the most current information provided (i) on procedures featured or (ii) by the manufacturer of each product to be administered, to verify the recommended dose or formula, the method and duration of administration, and contraindications.
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring Sensory Neuroscience Through Experience and Experiment
    The Journal of Undergraduate Neuroscience Education (JUNE), Fall 2012, 11(1):A126-A131 ARTICLE Exploring Sensory Neuroscience Through Experience and Experiment Robert A. Wyttenbach Department of Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. Many phenomena that we take for granted are illusions — attempts to connect them to neuroscience, and shows how color and motion on a TV or computer monitor, for students can explore and experiment with them. Even example, or the impression of space in a stereo music when (as is often the case) there is no agreed-upon recording. Even the stable image that we perceive when mechanistic explanation for an illusion, students can form looking directly at the real world is illusory. One of the hypotheses and test them by manipulating stimuli and important lessons from sensory neuroscience is that our measuring their effects. In effect, students can experiment perception of the world is constructed rather than received. with illusions using themselves as subjects. Sensory illusions effectively capture student interest, but how do you then move on to substantive discussion of Key words: illusion, adaptation, aftereffect, receptive field, neuroscience? This article illustrates several illusions, motion, color, pitch, size, orientation In addition to their immediate aesthetic appeal, illusions adaptation, you move your head closer to or further from have historically been used to investigate mechanisms of the white screen or look at the sky? (6) If you adapt one perception. While the success of this approach has been eye with the other eye closed and then switch eyes, is mixed — many illusions do not have accepted explanations there an afterimage? or turn out to be more complex than initially thought — Interpretations: (1) Students should be able to predict illusions are still powerful teaching tools.
    [Show full text]
  • Processes in Biological Vision
    PROCESSES IN BIOLOGICAL VISION: including, ELECTROCHEMISTRY OF THE NEURON This material is excerpted from the full β-version of the text. The final printed version will be more concise due to further editing and economical constraints. A Table of Contents and an index are located at the end of this paper. James T. Fulton Vision Concepts [email protected]/vision April 30, 2017 Copyright 2000 James T. Fulton Environment & Coordinates 2- 1 2. Environment, Coordinate Reference System and First Order Operation 1 The beginning of wisdom is to call things by their right names. - Chinese Proverb The process of communication involves a mutual agreement on the meaning of words. - Charley Halsted, 1993 [xxx rationalize azure versus aqua before publishing ] The environment of the eye can be explored from several points of view; its radiation environment, its thermo- mechanical environment, its energy supply environment and its output signal environment. To discuss the operation of the eye in such environments, establishing certain coordinate systems, functional relationships and methods of notation is important. That is the purpose of this chapter. The reader is referred to the original source for more details on the environment and the coordinate systems. Extensive references are provided for this purpose. Details relative to the various functional operations in vision will be explored in the chapters to follow. Because of the unique integration of many processes by the photoreceptor cells of the retina, which are explored individually in different chapters, a brief section is included here to coordinate this range of material. 2.1 Physical Environment The study of the visual system requires exploring a great many parameters from a variety of perspectives.
    [Show full text]
  • Toward the Development of a Model to Estimate the Readability of Credentialing-Examination Materials
    UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 5-2010 Toward the development of a model to estimate the readability of credentialing-examination materials Barbara Anne Badgett University of Nevada Las Vegas Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations Part of the Educational Psychology Commons Repository Citation Badgett, Barbara Anne, "Toward the development of a model to estimate the readability of credentialing- examination materials" (2010). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 185. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/1436770 This Dissertation is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Dissertation in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/or on the work itself. This Dissertation has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected]. TOWARD THE DEVELOPMENT OF A MODEL TO ESTIMATE THE READABILITY OF CREDENTIALING-EXAMINATION MATERIALS by Barbara A. Badgett Bachelor of Science University of Nevada, Las Vegas 2000 Master of Science University
    [Show full text]
  • C:\Vision\17Performance Pt 1A.Wpd
    PROCESSES IN BIOLOGICAL VISION: including, ELECTROCHEMISTRY OF THE NEURON This material is excerpted from the full β-version of the text. The final printed version will be more concise due to further editing and economical constraints. A Table of Contents and an index are located at the end of this paper. James T. Fulton Vision Concepts [email protected] April 30, 2017 Copyright 2003 James T. Fulton Performance Descriptors 17- 1 [xxx reconfirm all Section references to or in 17.2.2, etc. ] [xxx reword references to constant quantum efficiency ] 17 Performance descriptors of Vision1 Probably more error has crept into the subject of colour vision from inexact description of experimental conditions and the nature of the stimuli employed than from any other cause. Sir John Parsons, 1915 Because of the amount of color artwork in this chapter, it has been necessary to divide it into three parts for distribution over the INTERNET. PART 1A: INTRO, LUMINANCE & NEW CHROMATICITY DIAGRAM PART 1B: EXTENSIONS TO THE NEW CHROMATICITY DIAGRAM PART 2: TEMPORAL AND SPATIAL DESCRIPTORS OF VISION PART 1A: INTRO. LUMINANCE & CHROMINANCE The press of work on other parts of the manuscript may delay the final cleanup of this PART but it is too valuable to delay its release for comment. Any comments are welcome at [email protected]. 17.1 Introduction This Chapter and Chapter 16 form a pair. While the last Chapter developed equations that are applicable to any animal, this Chapter will concentrate on the most highly developed performance descriptors, those applicable to the human. The visual system is considerably more capable, more flexible and more complex than reflected in even the scientific literature.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Illusions
    VISUAL ILLUSIONS: PERCEPTION OF LUMINANCE, COLOR, AND MOTION IN HUMANS Inaugural-Dissertation zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades Doctor rerum naturalium (Dr. rer. nat.) an der Justus-Liebig-Universität Giessen Fachbereich 06: Psychologie und Sportwissenschaften Otto-Behaghel-Strasse 10F 35394 Giessen vorgelegt am 21. Dezember 2006 von Dipl. Psych. Kai Hamburger geboren am 5. Juni 1977 in Gedern 1. Berichterstatter und Betreuer Prof. Karl R. Gegenfurtner, Ph.D. (Psychologie, Giessen) 2. Berichterstatter Prof. Dr. Hans Irtel (Psychologie, Mannheim) To my grandfather Heinrich, my parents Elke and Rainer, my brother Sven, and to my fiancée Sandra. Acknowledgement First of all, I would like to express my gratitude to my godfather in the graduate program, Professor Karl R. Gegenfurtner (Giessen, Germany), and my two other supervisors, Professor Lothar Spillmann (Freiburg, Germany) and Professor Arthur G. Shapiro (Lewisburg, PA, U.S.A.). Karl, on very short notice you gave me the opportunity to join the graduate program ‘Neural Representation and Action Control – Neuroact’ and by doing so one of the best departments in the field of Vision Sciences. I became a member of an extraordinary lab, which still excites me. You allowed me to finish projects which were already in progress when I started in Giessen and you gave me plenty of rope to pursue my own interests. Thus, I was able to publish efficiently and furthermore gained deep insights into the field of Vision Sciences and even beyond. This was the best mentoring a natural scientist could think of. Thank you. Professor Spillmann, you paved my way into the Vision Sciences. At the beginning of my scientific career you gave me the opportunity to join your famous ‘Freiburg Psychophysics Laboratory’.
    [Show full text]
  • Structure and Function of the Pigeon Visual System
    Physiological Psychology Copyright © 1978 by The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1978, Vol. 6 (4), 403·437 Structure and function of the pigeon visual system WILLIAM J. DONOVAN Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida 32306 This comprehensive. up· to-date account of vision in the pigeon begins with a description of the eye, its dimensions, and optical properties. The retina is discussed in considerable detail, with particular attention given to: the distribution of the various cell types within and across the retina, the number and the nature of the oil drops in the cones, the highly developed inner plexiform layer, the response characteristics of the ganglion cells, and the damage to cones caused by exposure to light. Lastly, the three major visual pathways are examined neuroanatomica11y and electrophysiologica11y. In the second section, several important issues have been emphasized: the putative differences between frontal and lateral vision, visual sensitivity to both discrete and periodic stimuli at various adaptation levels, polarotaxis in the pigeon, the bipartite spectral sensitivity of its retina, hue and saturation discrimination, sensitivity to ultraviolet light, panoramic acuity, and the detection of movement. With the tremendous growth of animal learning as modes of adaptation which are specific to each research during the past 30 years, the pigeon has organism's survival needs, then it seems advantageous emerged as the most frequently studied representative to select an animal for which the sensory system of the avian class. Quite often, an awareness of the being considered is highly developed and for which sensory abilities of an animal can enhance the quality the behavior and habitat are familiar.
    [Show full text]
  • British Chemical and Physiological Abstracts
    BRITISH CHEMICAL AND PHYSIOLOGICAL ABSTRACTS ISSUED BY THE Bureau of Chemical and Physiological Abstracts [Supported by the Chemical Society, the Society of Chemical Industry, the Physiological Society, the Biochemical Society, and the Anatomical Society of Great Britain and Ireland] JUNE, 1 9 4 3 BUREAU: C hairm an: L. H. LAMPITT, D.Sc., F.I.C. % Hon. Treasurer: F. P. DUNN, B.Sc., F.I.C. JULIAN L. BAKER, F.I.C. C. R. HARINGTON, M.A., P h .D., F.R.S. G. L. BROWN, M.Sc., M.B., Ch .B. L. A. JORDAN, D.Sc., F.I.C. H. W. CREMER, M.Sc., F.I.C., M.I.Chem.E. G. A. R. KON, M.A., D.Sc., F.R.S. C. W. DAVIES, D.Sc., F.I.C. H. McCOMBIE, D.S.O., M.C., P h .D ., D.Sc., F.I.C. H. J. T. ELLINGHAM, B.Sc., P h .D ., F.I.C. B. A. McSWINEY, B.A., M.B., Sc.D. E d ito r: T. F. BURTON, B.Sc. Assistant Editors: J. H. BIRKINSHAW, D.Sc., F.I.C* W. JEVONS, D.Sc., P h .D. H. BURTON, M.Sc., D.Sc., F.I.C. E‘ E’ TURNER, M.A., D.Sc., F.I.C., F.R.S. F. L. USHER, D.Sc. F. G. CROSSE, F.I.C. H WREN> m .A ., D -S c., P h .D . A. A. ELD RIDGE, B.Sc., F.I.C. SAMSON WRIGHT, M.D., F.R.C.P.* * Assisted by J. D. BOYD (Anatomy), A.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstracts from CIP 2007 Segundo Congreso Ibérico De Percepción ABSTRACTS from CIP 2007 (SEGUNDO CONGRESO IBÉRICO DE PERCEPCIÓN) 471
    Abstracts from CIP 2007 Segundo Congreso Ibérico de Percepción ABSTRACTS FROM CIP 2007 (SEGUNDO CONGRESO IBÉRICO DE PERCEPCIÓN) 471 KEY SESSION 1 Human Visual Motion Perception, why do we Make so many Errors? A. M. Derrington (University of Kent, United Kingdom). It is by charting the limits of perceptual performance that we are able to characterise the mechanisms that support different modalities of perception. In the case of motion perception, it has been possible to design experiments in which consistent patterns of errors reveal systematic shifts, distortions, or even reversals of motion perception that can be used to infer distinctive principles of operation. A long-standing example of this is the motion after-effect, which was first reported in the scientific literature in the early 19th century and which is still being used today to probe the mechanisms of motion perception. This talk will concentrate on two more recent examples: second-order motion and the reversals in motion perception that occur when coarse and fine features are combined in a briefly presented moving stimulus. The first indication that humans respond to second-order motion came from experiments that showed that when observers were required to discriminate the motion of a complex high spatial frequency moving pattern, their responses were consistent with second-order but not first-order sensitivity (Badcock & Derrington, 1985). Subsequent experiments showed that second-order features contribute to motion judgements in many situations, but measurements of the limits of second-order motion perception suggest that any purely second-order effects are exerted at a high level—there is no such thing as a dedicated second-order motion mechanism.
    [Show full text]
  • Diseases of the Nervous System: Clinical Neuroscience and Therapeutic Principles, Third Edition Edited by Arthur K
    Cambridge University Press 0521793513 - Diseases of the Nervous System: Clinical Neuroscience and Therapeutic Principles, Third Edition Edited by Arthur K. Asbury, Guy M. McKhann, W. Ian McDonald, Peter J. Goadsby and Justin C. McArthur Index More information Index Note: this is a complete two-volume index Note: page numbers in italics refer to figures and tables; ‘Fig.’ refers to illustrations in the plates section Abbreviations of conditions used in subheadings (without explanation): AD Alzheimer’s disease AIDS Acquired immune deficiency syndrome ALS Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis CJD Creutzfeldt–Jakob disease FTD Frontotemporal dementia HIV Human immunodeficiency virus HD Huntington’s disease PD Parkinson’s disease SIADH syndrome of inappropriate secretion of antidiuretic hormone A-fibres 873–4 abdominoperineal resection of carcinoma acetaminophen activation 875 846 migraine 1940 brain-derived nerve growth factor 884, abducens motoneurons 636 shingle pain 1678 885 abducens nerve acetazolamide sprouted 885, Fig. 58.12 fascicle lesions 650 acidification 1235 A2M genetic locus 5 middle ear disease 670 idiopathic intracranial hypertension A␣ fibres 874 palsy 649–51 2027 A␤ antibody passive transfer 1853–4 petrous bone infection 651 periodic paralysis 1201–2 A␤ fibres 874 abducens nucleus acetyl CoA 1210 AD 1844 abducens nerve palsy 649 acetylcholine formation in familial AD 1845 horizontal conjugate eye movements AD 256 see also amyloid 635, 636, 637 brain aging 198 A␤ immunization 1853–4 lesions 635 Lewy body dementia 273, 274 A␤ peptide 12, 215
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural and Electrophysiological Correlates of Lightness Contrast And
    Behavioural and electrophysiological correlates of lightness contrast and assimilation ACASTER, Steph Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA) at: http://shura.shu.ac.uk/24340/ This document is the author deposited version. You are advised to consult the publisher's version if you wish to cite from it. Published version ACASTER, Steph (2018). Behavioural and electrophysiological correlates of lightness contrast and assimilation. Doctoral, Sheffield Hallam University. Copyright and re-use policy See http://shura.shu.ac.uk/information.html Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive http://shura.shu.ac.uk Behavioural and Electrophysiological Correlates of Lightness Contrast and Assimilation Stephanie Louise Acaster A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of Sheffield Hallam University for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy October 2018 Abstract Lightness contrast and lightness assimilation are examples of the perception of a surface being influenced by surrounding areas. In contrast, a grey target is perceived lighter when neighbouring a dark surface, and darker when neighbouring a light surface. The reverse is true for assimilation. The general aims of this thesis were to investigate contrast and assimilation in parallel, using behavioural and electrophysiological methods to examine the responses. The first part of the project used a matching-chart method to assess the effect of depth separation on the perception of stimuli shown to elicit either contrast or assimilation, making a direct comparison between the effect on contrast and on assimilation. The second part of the project developed a forced-choice (lighter/darker) task to investigate the electrophysiological (ERP) responses associated with contrast and assimilation, thus investigating the time course of the associated neural processing, and whether contrast and assimilation result from different underlying processing.
    [Show full text]
  • CIE 2017 Abstract Booklet
    CIE Midterm Meeting 2017 – Abstract Booklet Contents PROGRAMME ......................................................................................................................................... 4 INVITED PRESENTATIONS ................................................................................................................. 12 ORAL PRESENTATIONS ..................................................................................................................... 19 Session OS1 Colour quality (1) .......................................................................................................... 20 Session OS2 Metrology for photometric and radiometric devices (1) .............................................. 31 Session OS3 Glare .............................................................................................................................. 42 Session OS4 Colour quality (2) .......................................................................................................... 53 Session OS5 Road lighting (1) ............................................................................................................ 60 Session OS6 Interior environment and lighting design (1) ................................................................ 67 Session OS7 Exterior lighting ............................................................................................................. 74 Session OS8 Metrology for photometric and radiometric devices (2) .............................................. 83 Session
    [Show full text]