Scientific Integrity Issues in Environmental Toxicology and Chemistry: Improving Research Reproducibility, Credibility, and Transparency
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Case Studies in Research Misconduct and Human Subjects
Rules of the Road for those Conducting Federally Funded or Regulated Research: Case Studies in Research Misconduct and Human Subjects April 2, 2019 University of Maryland School of Medicine Baltimore, MD Robert P. Charrow General Counsel United States Department of Health and Human Services Introduction The materials that follow address three areas-misconduct in science, human subjects and privacy, and financial conflicts of interest. To help illustrate the application of the rules that pertain in each area, a number of hypothetical case studies are presented. You should read and be prepared to discuss each case study during the April 2 lecture. The government regulates research under two authorities. First, it regulates research that it funds. How much does the federal government spend annually on research? See Table 2. Second, a federal agency can regulate research on products that fall within its jurisdiction. Thus, by way of example, the Food and Drug Administration asserts jurisdiction over clinical drug trials even though those trials are privately funded. 1. Misconduct A. What is Research Misconduct? The Public Health Service ("PHS"), which includes NIH, defines "research misconduct" as follows: Research misconduct means fabrication, falsification, or plagiarism in proposing, performing, or reviewing research, or in reporting research results. (a) Fabrication is making up data or results and recording or reporting them. (b) Falsification is manipulating research materials, equipment, or processes, or changing or omitting data or results such that the research is not accurately represented in the research record. (c) Plagiarism is the appropriation of another person's ideas, processes, results, or words without giving appropriate credit. -
Integrating the History of Science Into Broader Discussions of Research Integrity and Fraud
HOS0010.1177/0073275320952268History of ScienceRoberts et al. 952268research-article2020 Introduction HOS History of Science 2020, Vol. 58(4) 354 –368 Integrating the history © The Author(s) 2020 of science into broader https://doi.org/10.1177/0073275320952268Article reuse guidelines: sagepub.com/journals-permissions discussions of research DOI: 10.1177/0073275320952268 integrity and fraud journals.sagepub.com/home/hos Lissa L. Roberts University of Twente, Netherlands H. Otto Sibum Uppsala University, Sweden Cyrus C. M. Mody Maastricht University, Netherlands Abstract This introductory article frames our special issue in terms of how historicizing research integrity and fraud can benefit current discussions of scientific conduct and the need to improve public trust in science. Keywords Research integrity, fraud, scientific conduct, scientific values, public trust Writing about research integrity and fraud in the midst of the Covid-19 crisis carries an extra sense of urgency. Voices of reason tell us to trust the experts, echoing both the (idealized) scientific method and Ronald Reagan’s response to Soviet promises regard- ing nuclear disarmament: “trust, but verify.” Great advice, but how easy is it really to identify trustworthy experts? Is the boundary between healthy skepticism and manufac- tured doubt a simple matter of distinguishing between valid laboratory and publishing protocols on one side and political, economic, or personal interests on the other? Should we follow those who rely on the Mertonian distinction between norm-bound -
Using the Proposed Effect of Breakfast on Obesity to Show 2 Practices That Distort Scientific Evidence1–4
Belief beyond the evidence: using the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity to show 2 practices that distort scientific evidence1–4 Andrew W Brown, Michelle M Bohan Brown, and David B Allison Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ajcn/article-abstract/98/5/1298/4577332 by guest on 06 September 2019 ABSTRACT there are not insurmountable barriers to moving beyond obser- Background: Various intentional and unintentional factors influ- vational evidence, and 2) it seems like a less politically charged ence beliefs beyond what scientific evidence justifies. Two such topic than the topics of some previous publications on biased factors are research lacking probative value (RLPV) and biased reporting [eg, sugar-sweetened beverages (2, 3) and breastfeeding research reporting (BRR). (4)] to better facilitate the discussion of extrascientific factors that Objective: We investigated the prevalence of RLPV and BRR in affect the fidelity of research reporting. research about the proposition that skipping breakfast causes weight In this article, we first establish that the PEBO is only a pre- gain, which is called the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity sumption so that we can examine 2 factors that may influence the (PEBO) in this article. propensity to believe in a presumption beyond the available evi- Design: Studies related to the PEBO were synthesized by using dence. The first factor we identified is research lacking probative a cumulative meta-analysis. Abstracts from these studies were also value (RLPV), which we defined as experiments or analyses that rated for the improper use of causal language and biased interpre- are 1) about questions that have already been sufficiently an- tations. -
Achieving a High Level of Protection from Pesticides in Europe: Problems with the Current Risk Assessment Procedure and Solutions
https://doi.org/10.1017/err.2020.18 . Achieving a High Level of Protection from Pesticides in Europe: Problems with the Current Risk Assessment Procedure and Solutions Claire ROBINSON* , Christopher J. PORTIER**, Aleksandra ČAVOŠKI***, Robin MESNAGE****, Apolline ROGER*****, Peter CLAUSING******, Paul WHALEY*******, Hans MUILERMAN******** and https://www.cambridge.org/core/terms Angeliki LYSSIMACHOU********* The regulation of pesticides in the European Union (EU) relies on a network of hard law (legislation and implementing acts) and soft law (non-legally binding guidance documents and administrative and scientific practices). Both hard and soft laws govern how risk assessments are conducted, but a significant role is left to the latter. Europe’s pesticide regulation is one of the most stringent in the world. Its stated objectives are to ensure an independent, objective and transparent assessment of pesticides and achieve a high level of protection for health and environment. However, a growing body of evidence shows that pesticides that have passed through this process and are authorised for use may harm , subject to the Cambridge Core terms of use, available at humans, animals and the environment. The authors of the current paper – experts in toxicology, law and policy – identified shortcomings in the authorisation process, focusing on the EU assessment of the pesticide active substance glyphosate. The shortcomings mostly consist of failures to implement the hard or soft laws. But in some instances the law itself is responsible, as some provisions can only fail to achieve its objectives. Ways to improve the system are proposed, requiring changes in hard and soft laws as well as in administrative 30 Sep 2021 at 13:44:58 and scientific practices. -
Managing Allegations of Scientific Misconduct: A
MANAGING ALLEGATIONS OF SCIENTIFIC MISCONDUCT: A Guidance Document for Editors January 2000 Office of Research Integrity Office of Public Health and Science U.S. Department of Health and Human Services http://ori.dhhs.gov 1 TABLE OF CONTENTS I. Role of Editors in Responding to Scientific Misconduct .............. 1 II. Purpose of Guidelines ................................................................... 2 III. Definitions ..................................................................................... 2 IV. Procedure for Handling Suspect Manuscripts .............................. 3 V. Responding to ORI Requests for Assistance ............................... 4 VI. Correcting the Literature ............................................................... 5 VII. Helpful Editorial Policies ............................................................... 5 VIII. Conclusion .................................................................................... 8 References ................................................................................................. 9 Appendix A: ORI Case Examples of Suspect Manuscripts ..................... 11 ORI Information and Technical Assistance . Inside Back Cover 2 MANAGING ALLEGATIONS OF SCIENTIFIC MISCONDUCT: A Guidance Document for Editors* I. Role of Editors in Responding to Scientific Misconduct The role editors should play in responding to scientific misconduct has been articulated by their colleagues, asserted by two national reports, and demonstrated by the scientific misconduct allegations -
How Scientific Misconduct Harms Prior Collaborators
A Service of Leibniz-Informationszentrum econstor Wirtschaft Leibniz Information Centre Make Your Publications Visible. zbw for Economics Hussinger, Katrin; Pellens, Maikel Working Paper Guilt by association: How scientific misconduct harms prior collaborators ZEW Discussion Papers, No. 17-051 Provided in Cooperation with: ZEW - Leibniz Centre for European Economic Research Suggested Citation: Hussinger, Katrin; Pellens, Maikel (2017) : Guilt by association: How scientific misconduct harms prior collaborators, ZEW Discussion Papers, No. 17-051, Zentrum für Europäische Wirtschaftsforschung (ZEW), Mannheim, http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:bsz:180-madoc-436888 This Version is available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10419/171327 Standard-Nutzungsbedingungen: Terms of use: Die Dokumente auf EconStor dürfen zu eigenen wissenschaftlichen Documents in EconStor may be saved and copied for your Zwecken und zum Privatgebrauch gespeichert und kopiert werden. personal and scholarly purposes. Sie dürfen die Dokumente nicht für öffentliche oder kommerzielle You are not to copy documents for public or commercial Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, öffentlich zugänglich purposes, to exhibit the documents publicly, to make them machen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. publicly available on the internet, or to distribute or otherwise use the documents in public. Sofern die Verfasser die Dokumente unter Open-Content-Lizenzen (insbesondere CC-Lizenzen) zur Verfügung gestellt haben sollten, If the documents have been made available under an Open gelten abweichend von diesen Nutzungsbedingungen die in der dort Content Licence (especially Creative Commons Licences), you genannten Lizenz gewährten Nutzungsrechte. may exercise further usage rights as specified in the indicated licence. www.econstor.eu Dis cus si on Paper No. 17-051 Guilt by Association: How Scientific Misconduct Harms Prior Collaborators Katrin Hussinger and Maikel Pellens Dis cus si on Paper No. -
Thinking and Reasoning
Thinking and Reasoning Thinking and Reasoning ■ An introduction to the psychology of reason, judgment and decision making Ken Manktelow First published 2012 British Library Cataloguing in Publication by Psychology Press Data 27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication by Psychology Press Data 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017 Manktelow, K. I., 1952– Thinking and reasoning : an introduction [www.psypress.com] to the psychology of reason, Psychology Press is an imprint of the Taylor & judgment and decision making / Ken Francis Group, an informa business Manktelow. p. cm. © 2012 Psychology Press Includes bibliographical references and Typeset in Century Old Style and Futura by index. Refi neCatch Ltd, Bungay, Suffolk 1. Reasoning (Psychology) Cover design by Andrew Ward 2. Thought and thinking. 3. Cognition. 4. Decision making. All rights reserved. No part of this book may I. Title. be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any BF442.M354 2012 form or by any electronic, mechanical, or 153.4'2--dc23 other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and 2011031284 recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing ISBN: 978-1-84169-740-6 (hbk) from the publishers. ISBN: 978-1-84169-741-3 (pbk) Trademark notice : Product or corporate ISBN: 978-0-203-11546-6 (ebk) names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used -
Disclosures FINAL Not Blind
Testimonial injustice and speakers’ duties1 Kristin Voigt ([email protected]) Final version forthcoming in Journal of Social Philosophy. 1 Introduction Miranda Fricker’s work on testimonial justice focuses on how individuals are perceived as ‘knowers’. She describes the injustice inflicted on speakers when their testimony is given less credibility due to stereotypes or prejudice against a social group of which they are a member. While much of the debate has focused on how we can counteract testimonial injustice in our capacity as hearers, I argue in this paper that our actions as speakers can also contribute to, or reduce, testimonial injustice. So as to be able capture this concern, I suggest that we broaden Fricker’s framework in three related respects. First, while Fricker focuses on epistemic virtues in responding to testimonial injustice, testimonial justice may also give rise to duties. Second, while Fricker thinks of testimonial injustice as involving the wronging of particular speakers, I suggest here that we can fail in our duties of testimonial injustice in ways that do not wrong particular individuals. Finally, while Fricker’s account excludes excess credibility as a form of testimonial injustice, the argument presented here strengthens the case for including not only credibility deficits but also excess credibility in our account of testimonial injustice. 1 For helpful comments and suggestions, I am grateful to audiences at the Ethox Centre, Southampton University, Lancaster University, and the ‘Understanding Epistemic Injustice’ conference, Bristol University, where versions of this paper were presented, as well as the anonymous reviewers of this journal. Part of this work was undertaken while I was a Caroline Miles Visiting Scholar at the Ethox Centre, Oxford University; I am grateful to the Ethox Centre for its support. -
Parallels of Unintentional Plagiarism and Predatory Publishing: Understanding Root Causes and Solutions
City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Publications and Research New York City College of Technology 2017 Parallels of Unintentional Plagiarism and Predatory Publishing: Understanding Root Causes and Solutions Monica Berger CUNY New York City College of Technology How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/ny_pubs/352 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] Monica Berger, Associate Professor, Library, NYC College of Technology, CUNY Parallels of Unintentional Plagiarism and Predatory Publishing: Understanding Root Causes and Solutions CUNY-Wide Conference on Academic Integrity Eugenio María de Hostos Community College Sept. 29, 2017 Abstract Plagiarism and predatory publishing share common attributes. Although students do not publish in predatory journals, both plagiarism and predatory publishing fall under the umbrella of academic integrity and scholarly ethics. Academic misconduct has many faces, ranging from student cheating on exams to purchasing a doctoral thesis and claiming it as one’s own work. Some forms of academic misconduct, such as the examples above are always intentional. However, many manifestations of academic misconduct are less clearly intentional. Students often plagiarize unintentionally because they lack writing skills including paraphrasing and citing. Faculty sometimes publish with predatory journals when they lack scholarly publishing knowledge. Weak information literacy underpins both behaviors. However, other factors drive both plagiarism and predatory publishing. Three broad areas are cultural considerations, generational differences, and local academic values. The discourse related to cultural considerations is especially fruitful to unpack. -
A Narrative on the Fabrication of Results in Science
GENERAL ARTICLES A narrative on the fabrication of results in science João Freitas*, Dulce Esteves and Henrique Neiva Data fabrication is an act of scientific misconduct that affects its transparency and veracity. In this study we identify the main data fabrication problems in scientific research and suggest prevention methodologies. This article is a narrative review of the major articles in fabrication of results in science. The results of this review provide the knowledge that fabrication cases come from most diverse scientific fields. Currently, there is more pressure on scientists to publish. As a result, several studies now report on data fabrication in research. State laws and research ethics committees strengthen relationships to criminalize this type of misconduct that usurps state funds. This study dis- cusses the controversies and complexities of data fabrication in scientific research. Ethics commit- tees must continue their efforts to prevent data fabrication, thus contributing to scientific transparency. Keywords: Criminalization, data fabrication, scientific misconduct, transparency. SCIENTIFIC misconduct does not stop with a decline in Current data show that, the success rate of eligible pro- morality and increase in competition in obtaining posals in the Universities for the year 2015 was only research funds. Instead, very well structured, accusations 14%; as governments decrease funding, the pressure on of scientific misconduct, stand in the history of universities to obtain funding from other sources increas- science itself from ancient Greece to the present day1. es, including the UE and the private sector13. Although the respective federal agencies nuclearize the Data fraud can have four types of external effects14. It concept of scientific conduct to review and disseminate can damage the career of colleagues and students, who research, experts have a more popular view2. -
Childhood Obesity Intervention Studies: A
Page 1 of 45 Obesity Reviews 1 Childhood Obesity Intervention Studies: A 2 Narrative Review and Guide for Investigators, 3 Authors, Editors, Reviewers, Journalists, and 4 Readers to Guard Against Exaggerated 5 Effectiveness Claims 6 Authors and affiliations: 7 Andrew W. Brown*, Douglas G. Altman+, Tom Baranowski, J. Martin Bland, John A. Dawson, Nikhil V. 8 Dhurandhar, Shima Dowla, Kevin R. Fontaine, Andrew Gelman, Steven B. Heymsfield, Wasantha 9 Jayawardene, Scott W. Keith, Theodore K. Kyle, Eric Loken, J. Michael Oakes, June Stevens, Diana M. 10 Thomas, & David B. Allison* 11 Brown: Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health-Bloomington, 12 Bloomington, IN, 47405, USA 13 Altman: Centre for Statistics in Medicine, Nuffield Department of Orthopaedics, Rheumatology, and 14 Musculoskeletal Sciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom 15 Baranowski: Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research 16 Center, Houston, TX, 77030 17 Bland: Department of Health Sciences, University of York, York, United Kingdom 18 Dawson: Department of Nutritional Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX, 79409 19 Dhurandhar: Department of Nutritional Sciences, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas 79409 20 Dowla: School of Medicine, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, 35294, USA 21 Fontaine: Department of Health Behavior, School of Public Health, University of Alabama at Birmingham, 22 Birmingham, AL 35294, USA 1 of 41 Obesity Reviews Page 2 of 45 -
Making Matters of Fraud: Sociomaterial Technology in the Case of Hwang
HOS0010.1177/0073275320921687History of SciencePark 921687research-article2020 Article HOS History of Science 2020, Vol. 58(4) 393 –416 Making matters of fraud: © The Author(s) 2020 Sociomaterial technology Article reuse guidelines: sagepub.com/journals-permissions in the case of Hwang and https://doi.org/10.1177/0073275320921687DOI: 10.1177/0073275320921687 Schatten journals.sagepub.com/home/hos Buhm Soon Park Graduate School of Science, Technology, and Policy, Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology (KAIST), Republic of Korea Abstract This paper revisits the “Hwang case,” which shook Korean society and the world of stem cell research in 2005 with the fraudulent claim of creating patient-specific embryonic stem cells. My goal is to overcome a human-centered, Korea-oriented narrative, by illustrating how materials can have an integral role in the construction and judgment of fraud. To this end, I pay attention to Woo Suk Hwang’s lab at Seoul National University as a whole, including human and nonhuman agents, that functioned as what I call sociomaterial technology, and Gerald P. Schatten at the University of Pittsburgh, Hwang’s collaborator, who played a crucial role in demonstrating the potency of this technology to the members of the scientific community. By recasting the whole event as the “case of Hwang and Schatten,” I argue that fraud is, like all knowledge claims, a sociotechnical construct, and that matters of fraud are locally judged. Fraud leaves its mark on materials, but I show that material evidence alone never