Abridged Reader Vibration.Pdf

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Abridged Reader Vibration.Pdf A FOR- AY INTO WORLDS OF VI- BRATION In 1934, Estonian biophilosopher Jakob von Uex- of the spider are thus extended through the mate- küll penned a provocative text exploring animals' rial threads of its web, which acts as a network for perception of their environments, the translated conducting the vibrational semiotics through which title of which is A Foray Into the Worlds of Ani- the spider's Umwelt takes shape. In the paper by mals and Humans: With a Theory of Meaning. In this Introduction Hilton Japyassu and Kevin Laland (Extended spider Introduction work, Uexküll details his theory of Umwelten: that cognition), they take this relationship further, pro- all living creatures experience life in terms of their posing that the web not only extends the spider's unique, spatio-temporal sensory-perceptual world, senses, but also it's cognition: allowing the spider or Umwelt. In this context, animals are not simply to offload cognitive tasks to its web. The spider responding blindly to stimuli, but actively inter- and web are thus a deeply entangled and living as- preting the signs appropriate to them; actions that semblage; what Studio Saraceno has come to refer bring their world into being. For von Uexküll, the to by the neologism: spider/web. Umwelt (of human and nonhuman animals alike) is thus a species-specific subjective construction. The paper by Beth Mortimer et al (Tuning the instrument) explores how vibrations travel within The Umwelt or subjective sensory world partic- spider/webs, and examines the spider's ability to ular to spiders is very different from our own. tune this instrument. With its sensitive legs resting As explored in the papers by Friedrich Barth (The on the silken, trembling threads of this web, the Vibration Sense) and William Eberhard (Art Show) spider is thus already a musician; playing, tweak- , the spider's primary means of perceiving and un- ing and responding to the vibrations that travel derstanding the world is through vibration . Spiders through this floating material body. This observa- lack 'ears' for hearing, and despite having eight tion of spiders as vibratory musicians is not new eyes, web-building spiders have very poor vision. -as we can see in the 1904 paper by Fred Lahee Instead, the spider possess specialised organs for (The calls of spiders), the 1905 paper by James sensing airborne and substrate-borne vibrations: Porter (The Habits, Instincts, and Mental Powers of Spiders), or the 1915 paper by William Barrows trichobothria (hair-like extensions that cover the (The reactions of an orb-weaving spider), which spider's legs), and slit sensilla, which afford the notes the spider's reactions to the tremulations of spiders an acute sensitivity to the vibrations mov- a tuning fork, or a vibrating rubber band. The essay ing through their Umwelt. These are sense organs by David Toop (Filament Drums) further explores a for which we humans possess no recognisable tangled history of relationships between spider silk cognates. and musical instruments in other cultural traditions. A vibrational sensory capacity is not singular to Ultimately, what these instruments provocatively spiders. The study of substrate-borne vibrational explore is the possibility of interspeciecs com- communication in animals is a relatively new subset munication; of opening up a dialogue of sorts with of bioacoustics known as biotremology , introduced nonhuman animals, who have persistently been de- in this reader via the text by Peggy Hill and An- nied a meaningful place in our conversations. If our dreas Wessel (Biotremology), and explored spe- sensory worls truly are uncommunicating, how might cifically in relation to spider vibrational signalling we begin a dialogue? Is a conversation even possi- in Peggy's text (Quiet Listening). Matija Gogala's ble? The possibilities and problematics of communi- paper (Sound or Vibration: an Old Question of Insect cation with nonhuman animals is further explored in Communication) also explores this phenomenon of the text by Vinciane Despret's (in U is for Umwelt), vibrational communication, situating it in the con- which return us again to Uexkull's theories of Um- text of the history of studies of insect acoustic welten. In this text, Despret proposes that -rather communication. than seeing animals as beings in unique worlds, we might think of them as beings with worlds: worlds For web-building spiders (which number roughly that are multiple, porous, entangled. half of all known species), their web is an instru- ment for vibrational communication -a vibrant and We conclude with an excerpt from Stefan Helm- organic network of silk threads, whose architec- reich and Michele Friedner (When sound studies ture is acutely attuned to the vibrational tremors meet deaf studies), in which the authors raise the that pass through it: the signals the spider sends possibility that vibrations -which 'edge between through the web to gauge its tension and struc- tactilty and hearing'- are transgressive sensory tural properties, the signals sent by other spiders intercessors, and thus might be capable of trav- (potential mates, or predators), the vibrations elling between and interrupting different sensory produced by the spider's prey, or even the low worlds or Umwelten. Following this provocation frequency tremors produced by the wind. Without forward, we speculate that perhaps vibration might the web, the spider cannot eat, or gain an adequate be a way for us to begin a shared conversation with picture of her surroundings. The sensory capacities A reader on the vibrational sensory world of spider/webs 1 our nonhuman kin. A reader on the vibrational sensory world of spider/webs 2 The Vibration Sense 5 Barth, F. G. Index Art Show 10 Eberhard, W. G. Spider cognition: What is it like to be a spider? 12 Jackson, R. R. & Cross, F. R. Extended spider cognition 14 Japyassú, H. F. & Laland, K. N. Biotremology 15 Hill, P. S. M. & Wessel, A. Quiet Listening 20 Hill, P. S. M. Sound or Vibration, an Old Question of Insect 22 Communication Gogala, M. Tuning the instrument: sonic properties in the 23 spider's web Mortimer, B., Soler, A., Siviour, C. R., Zaera, R., & Vollrath, F. The Calls of Spiders 25 Lahee, F. H. The Habits, Instincts, and Mental Powers of 26 Spiders, Genera, Argiope and Epeira Porter, J. P. The reactions of an orb-weaving spider , Epei- 28 ra sclopetaria Clerck, to rhythmic vibrations of its web. Barrows, W. (1915). Filament Drums: the Endless Instrument 31 Toop, D. Spider and I 34 Ziporyn, E. Arachnid Instruments 36 Saraceno, S. T. U is for Umwelt: Do beasts know ways of being 37 in the world Despret, V. Sound studies meets deaf studies 41 Helmreich, S. & Friedner, M. 3 A reader on the vibrational sensory world of spider/webs Yes, but how do they do it? For many people some of the most intriguing questions regarding spider webs center on how a small, nearly blind animal with limited brainpower can build such precisely organized, complex Art Show webs that have such uniformly spaced lines and such finely balanced tensions. Perhaps it is easiest to appreciate an orb weaving spi- der's achievements if you put yourself in her place, and imagine what it would be like if you were attempting to build a new orb. To start, you must first blindfold your- self: the spider's eyesight is very poor, her eyes are on the wrong side of her body to see the web's lines as she hangs below them; and in any case she often works at night. Nor can she see any of the potential attach- ment sites, such as branches and leaves in her vicinity. The closest human analogy would be that of a blind man who must explore and climb through the tangled branches of trees on the basis of touch, leaving a silk line behind wherever he goes. And you will not be able to explore any of the spaces where you have been by swinging or flapping your lines, because the air is highly viscous at the scale of the spider; for a human, an analogous situation would be building a web of elastic ropes under water. Finding and recognizing an appropriate site in which to build your web will be one of the most challenging stages. The supports to which you might attach lines are arranged extremely irregularly in space, and each site is different (the smooth rectangular frames that spiders used in this exhibit are NOT fair approximations to natural conditions). You cannot even sense the pres- ence or absence of nearby attachment points or obsta- cles, much less their positions relative to each other until either you or your lines contact them. And even if you could have seen these objects, you will be unable fly to them, to shoot out your lines to reach them, or even, in many cases, to walk to them (imagine making a line between the widely spaced twigs on two differ- ent branches, or on different trees). You must instead depend on the vagarities of the wind; you will have to launch new lines, allowing their tips float away on ir- regular air currents, and hope that they will eventually snag on objects that can serve as supports for your web. As you discover attachment sites in this haphaz- ard way, you will need recognize whether they border an open site that is sufficiently large to accommodate orb that you are planning, and whether this space it is free of further protruding objects that could interfere with your orb. You will need to adjust the area of the open space that you are planning to occupy in accord with the amount of silk you that have available in your silk glands (this will vary with how much you ate and how much silk you have spun recently).
Recommended publications
  • Ecology and Behavior of the Giant Wood Spider in New Guinea
    Ecology and Behavior of the Giant Wood Spider Nephila maculata (Fabricius) in New Guinea MICHAEL H. ROBINSON and BARBARA ROBINSON SMITHSONIAN CONTRIBUTIONS TO ZOOLOGY • NUMBER 149 SERIAL PUBLICATIONS OF THE SMITHSONIAN INSTITUTION The emphasis upon publications as a means of diffusing knowledge was expressed by the first Secretary of the Smithsonian Institution. In his formal plan for the Insti- tution, Joseph Henry articulated a program that included the following statement: "It is proposed to publish a series of reports, giving an account of the new discoveries in science, and of the changes made from year to year in all branches of knowledge." This keynote of basic research has been adhered to over the years in the issuance of thousands of titles in serial publications under the Smithsonian imprint, com- mencing with Smithsonian Contributions to Knowledge in 1848 and continuing with the following active series: Smithsonian Annals of Flight Smithsonian Contributions to Anthropology Smithsonian Contributions to Astrophysics Smithsonian Contributions to Botany Smithsonian Contributions to the Earth Sciences Smithsonian Contributions to Paleobiology Smithsonian Contributions to Zoology Smithsonian Studies in History and Technology In these series, the Institution publishes original articles and monographs dealing with the research and collections of its several museums and offices and of professional colleagues at other institutions of learning. These papers report newly acquired facts, synoptic interpretations of data, or original theory in specialized fields. These pub- lications are distributed by mailing lists to libraries, laboratories, and other interested institutions and specialists throughout the world. Individual copies may be obtained from the Smithsonian Institution Press as long as stocks are available.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence for Nanocoulomb Charges on Spider Ballooning Silk
    PHYSICAL REVIEW E 102, 012403 (2020) Evidence for nanocoulomb charges on spider ballooning silk E. L. Morley1,* and P. W. Gorham 2,† 1School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, 24 Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, United Kingdom 2Department of Physics & Astronomy, University of Hawaii at Manoa, 2505 Correa Rd., Honolulu, Hawaii 96822, USA (Received 10 December 2019; revised 5 March 2020; accepted 6 March 2020; published 9 July 2020) We report on three launches of ballooning Erigone spiders observed in a 0.9m3 laboratory chamber, controlled under conditions where no significant air motion was possible. These launches were elicited by vertical, downward-oriented electric fields within the chamber, and the motions indicate clearly that negative electric charge on the ballooning silk, subject to the Coulomb force, produced the lift observed in each launch. We estimate the total charge required under plausible assumptions, and find that at least 1.15 nC is necessary in each case. The charge is likely to be nonuniformly distributed, favoring initial longitudinal mobility of electrons along the fresh silk during extrusion. These results demonstrate that spiders are able to utilize charge on their silk to attain electrostatic flight even in the absence of any aerodynamic lift. DOI: 10.1103/PhysRevE.102.012403 I. INTRODUCTION nificant upward components to the local wind velocity distri- bution; whether actual wind momentum spectra provide the The phenomenon of aerial dispersal of spiders using required distributions is still unproven, particularly for takeoff strands of silk often called gossamer was identified and stud- conditions. Even so, recent detailed observations of spider ied first with some precision by Martin Lister in the late 17th ballooning analyzed exclusively in terms of aerodynamic century [1], followed by Blackwall in 1827 [2], Darwin [3]on forces [12] provide plausible evidence that larger spiders can the Beagle voyage, and a variety of investigators since [4–7].
    [Show full text]
  • Spiders at the Cinema
    ACADEMIA Spider ecology, behavior, and learning Behavioral Ecology Spiders at the Cinema Focus on extremely receptive sensory organs allow MACIEJ BARTOS them to perceive those signals accurately, There are plenty Faculty of Biology and Environmental Protection helping them construct complex webs and of spiders that are University of Łódź remotely localize insects flying or scuttling as colorful as the [email protected] nearby. Eyesight and sensory organs used prettiest butterflies, Dr Maciej Bartos studies the behavior of jumping spiders to perceive chemical stimuli also play an have better eyesight important role in orientation and commu- than many birds or In spite of severe brain size limitations, nication between individuals. Most spiders’ mammals, and exhibit many spiders are able to solve simple eyes, like those of insects, are not more complex behavior complex problems, recognize their prey capable of perceiving complex images. They than many higher allow them to notice motion, changes in light animals. Pictured: with great accuracy, and learn new intensity, and to a lesser degree allow them American Cardinal hunting techniques to identify a partner or prey. As such the jumper (Phiddippus majority of spiders exhibit relatively simple cardinalis) To most people, spiders are hairy crea- behavior. However, not tures that lurk in dark corners behind the all spiders are limited by cupboard. They are widely disliked, and low acuity of vision. in some people even trigger panic fuelled There is in fact a large by tales of the creatures’ terrible venom. group of diurnal spiders However, arachnophobia can be cured: those whose world is domi- afflicted simply need to find out more about nated by visual stimuli.
    [Show full text]
  • Portia Perceptions: the Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping Spider
    Portia Perceptions: The Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping 1 Spider Duane P. Harland and Robert R. Jackson The Personality of Portia Spiders are traditionally portrayed as simple, instinct-driven animals (Savory, 1928; Drees, 1952; Bristowe, 1958). Small brain size is perhaps the most compelling reason for expecting so little flexibility from our eight-legged neighbors. Fitting comfortably on the head of a pin, a spider brain seems to vanish into insignificance. Common sense tells us that compared with large-brained mammals, spiders have so little to work with that they must be restricted to a circumscribed set of rigid behaviors, flexibility being a luxury afforded only to those with much larger central nervous systems. In this chapter we review recent findings on an unusual group of spiders that seem to be arachnid enigmas. In a number of ways the behavior of the araneophagic jumping spiders is more comparable to that of birds and mammals than conventional wisdom would lead us to expect of an arthropod. The term araneophagic refers to these spiders’ preference for other spiders as prey, and jumping spider is the common English name for members of the family Saltici- dae. Although both their common and the scientific Latin names acknowledge their jumping behavior, it is really their unique, complex eyes that set this family of spiders apart from all others. Among spiders (many of which have very poor vision), salticids have eyes that are by far the most specialized for resolving fine spatial detail. We focus here on the most extensively studied genus, Portia. Before we discuss the interrelationship between the salticids’ uniquely acute vision, their predatory strategies, and their apparent cognitive abilities, we need to offer some sense of what kind of animal a jumping spider is; to do this, we attempt to offer some insight into what we might call Portia’s personality.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Santa Barbara Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UC Santa Barbara UC Santa Barbara Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title How Collective Personality, Behavioral Plasticity, Information, and Fear Shape Collective Hunting in a Spider Society Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4pm302q6 Author Wright, Colin Morgan Publication Date 2018 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Santa Barbara How Collective Personality, Behavioral Plasticity, Information, and Fear Shape Collective Hunting in a Spider Society A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Ecology, Evolution and Marine Biology by Colin M. Wright Committee in charge: Professor Jonathan Pruitt, Chair Professor Erika Eliason Professor Thomas Turner June 2018 The dissertation of Colin M. Wright is approved. ____________________________________________ Erika Eliason ____________________________________________ Thomas Turner ____________________________________________ Jonathan Pruitt, Committee Chair April 2018 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to thank my Ph.D. advisor, Dr. Jonathan Pruitt, for supporting me and my research over the last 5 years. I could not imagine having a better, or more entertaining, mentor. I am very thankful to have had his unwavering support through academic as well as personal challenges. I am also extremely thankful for all the members of the Pruitt Lab (Nick Keiser, James Lichtenstein, and Andreas Modlmeier), as well as my cohorts at both the University of Pittsburgh and UCSB that have been my closest friends during graduate school. I appreciate Dr. Walter Carson at the University of Pittsburgh for being an unofficial second mentor to me during my time there. Most importantly, I would like to thank my family, Rill (mother), Tom (father), and Will (brother) Wright.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparative Analysis of Courtship in <I>Agelenopsis</I> Funnel-Web Spiders
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 5-2012 Comparative analysis of courtship in Agelenopsis funnel-web spiders (Araneae, Agelenidae) with an emphasis on potential isolating mechanisms Audra Blair Galasso [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons Recommended Citation Galasso, Audra Blair, "Comparative analysis of courtship in Agelenopsis funnel-web spiders (Araneae, Agelenidae) with an emphasis on potential isolating mechanisms. " PhD diss., University of Tennessee, 2012. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss/1377 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a dissertation written by Audra Blair Galasso entitled "Comparative analysis of courtship in Agelenopsis funnel-web spiders (Araneae, Agelenidae) with an emphasis on potential isolating mechanisms." I have examined the final electronic copy of this dissertation for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, with a major in Ecology and Evolutionary Biology. Susan E. Riechert, Major
    [Show full text]
  • Exploring the Relationship Between Behaviour and Neurochemistry in the Polyphenic Spider, Anelosimus Studiosus (Araneae: Theridiidae) Jennifer B
    East Tennessee State University Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University Electronic Theses and Dissertations Student Works 8-2016 Exploring the Relationship Between Behaviour and Neurochemistry in the Polyphenic Spider, Anelosimus studiosus (Araneae: Theridiidae) Jennifer B. Price East Tennessee State University Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.etsu.edu/etd Part of the Behavioral Neurobiology Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Comparative and Evolutionary Physiology Commons, Molecular Biology Commons, Other Animal Sciences Commons, and the Other Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Commons Recommended Citation Price, Jennifer B., "Exploring the Relationship Between Behaviour and Neurochemistry in the Polyphenic Spider, Anelosimus studiosus (Araneae: Theridiidae)" (2016). Electronic Theses and Dissertations. Paper 3114. https://dc.etsu.edu/etd/3114 This Dissertation - Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ East Tennessee State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Exploring the Relationship Between Behaviour and Neurochemistry in the Polyphenic Spider, Anelosimus studiosus (Araneae: Theridiidae) ______________________ A dissertation Presented to the faculty of the Department of Biomedical Sciences East Tennessee State University In partial fulfillment of the requirements
    [Show full text]
  • FIRST AID in ARACHNIDS a SUMMARY V1.2
    FIRST AID IN ARACHNIDS A SUMMARY v1.2 MORE ON WWW.THERAPHOSIDAE.BE www.theraphosidae.be © All rights reserved | 2017 1 ACCIDENTS MOLTING ISSUES Keep track of the spider's behavior. Predict an upcoming molt. Never remove the spider from her original spot during ecdysis, unless her life is in danger. Never spray water directly on the spider, esp. not during ecdysis. OLD EXOSKELETON GOT STUCK ON NEW SKIN • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Wait until the spider got on its feed to help. • Provide enough fresh water. • Remove parts of old carapace with tweezers. • Remove all living prey before ecdysis starts. • Difficult parts: Perform anaesthesia. • Don't disturb the spider. CURE PREVENTION DEFORMED LEGS • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Perform autotomy 2 weeks post-ecdysis. • Provide enough fresh water. • Remove all living prey before ecdysis starts. • Don't disturb the spider. CURE PREVENTION DISEASES OF THE CUTICULA • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Provide enough fresh water. • Remove all living prey before ecdysis starts. • Don't disturb the spider. CURE PREVENTION LOSS OF FANGS • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Step 1: Perform anaesthesia. • Provide enough fresh water. • Step 2: Cut the fangs from old exoskeleton. • Remove all living prey before ecdysis starts. • Step 3: Glue old fang in place. • Don't disturb the spider. Do nothing if the spider eats and lives well. CURE Contact a veterinarian, specializing in exotics. PREVENTION WET MOLT • Provide optimal environmental conditions. • Death is not inevitable. • Provide enough fresh water. • Remove all living prey before ecdysis starts. • Don't disturb the spider. CURE PREVENTION www.theraphosidae.be © All rights reserved | 2017 2 OPEN WOUNDS Mainly caused by use of bad terraria, sharp objects, collapsing structures and impulsive fear responses.
    [Show full text]
  • The Sublethal Effects of Neonicotinoids on Spiders Are Independent of Their
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN The sublethal efects of neonicotinoids on spiders are independent of their nutritional status Milan Řezáč1, Nela Gloríková1, Shawn M. Wilder2 & Petr Heneberg3* Spiders were recently shown to be adversely afected by feld-realistic concentrations of a broad scale of neonicotinoid insecticides. Among the reported efects of neonicotinoids on invertebrates were declines in lipid biosynthesis and upregulation of β-oxidation, while vertebrate models suggest increased adipogenesis following treatment with neonicotinoids. Therefore, we hypothesized that there exists synergy between the efects of diet and concurrent exposure to feld-realistic concentrations of neonicotinoid insecticides. To address this hypothesis, we fed frst instars of the large wolf spider Hogna antelucana with two types of diets and exposed them to feld-realistic concentrations of three formulations of neonicotinoids (thiamethoxam, thiacloprid and acetamiprid). We then measured the growth of the tested spiders; the lipid and protein content of their bodies; and their behavior, including ballooning, rappelling, and locomotor parameters. The two tested diets consisted of casein-treated and sucrose-treated Drosophila melanogaster. The dietary treatments afected the lipid and protein content of the spiders, their body weight and carapace length but did not afect any of the measured behavioral parameters. Surprisingly, we did not fnd any efects of acute exposure to neonicotinoid insecticides on the lipid or protein reserves of spiders. Exposure to neonicotinoids altered the behavior of the spiders as reported previously in other spider species; however, these efects were not afected by dietary treatments. Overall, the dietary treatments did not have any major synergy with acute exposure to feld-realistic concentrations of neonicotinoid insecticides.
    [Show full text]
  • Octopamine Levels Relate to Male Mating Tactic Expression in the Wolf Spider Rabidosa Punctulata Eileen A
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Eileen Hebets Publications Papers in the Biological Sciences 2015 Octopamine levels relate to male mating tactic expression in the wolf spider Rabidosa punctulata Eileen A. Hebets University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Matthew aH nsen University of Nebraska–Lincoln Thomas C. Jones East Tennessee State University, [email protected] Dustin J. Wilgers University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets Part of the Animal Sciences Commons, Behavior and Ethology Commons, Biology Commons, Entomology Commons, and the Genetics and Genomics Commons Hebets, Eileen A.; Hansen, Matthew; Jones, Thomas C.; and Wilgers, Dustin J., "Octopamine levels relate to male mating tactic expression in the wolf spider Rabidosa punctulata" (2015). Eileen Hebets Publications. 69. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/69 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eileen Hebets Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Animal Behaviour 100 (2015), pp. 136–142. doi 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.023 Copyright © 2014 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour. Published by Elsevier Ltd. Used by permission. Submitted 26 July 2014; accepted 20 August 2014 and again 13 November 2014; online 29 December 2014 digitalcommons.unl.edu Octopamine levels relate to male mating tactic expression in the wolf spider Rabidosa punctulata Eileen A. Hebets,1 Matthew Hansen,1 Thomas C. Jones,2 and Dustin J.
    [Show full text]
  • Predatory Behavior of the Zebra Spider, Salticus Scenicus (Araneae: Salticidae)
    Predatory behavior of the zebra spider, Salticus scenicus (Araneae: Salticidae) LAWRENCEM. DILL' Department of Biology, York University, Toronto, Ontario Received March 27, 1975 DILL, L. M. 1975. Predatory behavior of the zebra spider, Salticus scenicus (Araneae: Sal- ticidae). Can. J. Zool. 53: 1284-1289. The zebra spider (Salticus scenicus), a small jumping spider, orients towards prey detected by its lateral eyes whenever the angle subtended by such prey exceeds 5.5". The velocity of the prey is not involved in the determination of reactive distance, but only moving objects elicit orienta- tion. The probability that orientation is followed by stalking is a function of both prey size and velocity, but the effects of these parameters on reactive distance for stalking were not deter- mined. The zebra spider's stalk velocity declines progressively as it nears its (stationary) prey, and the probable optimality of this behavioral tactic is discussed. DILL,L. M. 1975. Predatory behavior of the zebra spider, Salticus scenicus (Araneae: Sal- ticidae). Can. J. Zool. 53: 1284-1289. Le saltique harlequin (Salticus scenicus) petite araignee sauteuse, s'oriente vers une proie repCree au moyen de ses yeux lateraux, lorsque I'angle soustendu par la proie depasse 5.5". La vitesse de la proie n'hfluence pas la distance ii laquelle I'araignke reagit, mais seuls les objets en mouvement entrainent I'orientation. La probabilite que I'orientation soit suivie d'une poursuite est fonction de la taille de la proie et de sa vitesse; cependant, les effets de ces parametres sur la distance a laquelle I'araignee commence sa poursuite n'ont pas kt6 determines.
    [Show full text]
  • Beautiful Big-Eyed Jumpers
    The American River Natural History Association Number 164 www.arnha.org Summer 2013 Beautiful Big-eyed Jumpers by Tim Manolis If you don’t know much about jumping spiders, be warned and are often more-or-less the same size. The vision these eyes – learning more about them can become an addictive pleasure! provide is often unremarkable to poor. Now, I know that a lot of folks more frequently associate the But two of the eight eyes of jumping spiders are strikingly phrase “creepy crawly” with spiders than the phrase “addictive different. The front row of eyes face forward, and the middle pleasure,” but jumping spiders are not your average spiders. two of this row (called the anterior median eyes) are much larger Among other amazing things, they stalk prey -- insects and than the other six eyes, giving these spiders a rather “cute” other spiders -- like cats, big-eyed gaze. More importantly, they give jumping spiders using vision that rivals that a surprisingly crisp, binocular view of the world in full color of many birds and mam- (they even see in the ultraviolet range). In short, the anterior mals. Male jumpers can median eyes of jumping spiders have much better visual acuity be brilliantly colored with than those of any other insects or spiders. Their other six eyes, courtship displays rivaling the outer two front eyes and the four in the back row (actually those of birds-of-paradise. arranged in two lateral rows behind the front row, facing up), Some species are remark- see Spiders on page 3 able mimics of ants, wasps, or beetles.
    [Show full text]