State-Dependent Control of Neural Activity in the Olfactory Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

State-Dependent Control of Neural Activity in the Olfactory Cortex STATE-DEPENDENT CONTROL OF NEURAL ACTIVITY IN THE OLFACTORY CORTEX by KAITLIN S. CARLSON Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Thesis Advisor: Dr. Daniel Wesson Department of Neurosciences CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY August, 2018 CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES We hereby approve the dissertation of Kaitlin S. Carlson candidate for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy* Committee Chair……………………………………………... Ben W. Strowbridge, Ph.D. Member………………………………………………………… Daniel W. Wesson, Ph.D. Member…………………………………………………………… Evan S. Deneris, Ph.D. Member……………………………………………………… Brian M. McDermott, Ph.D. Date of Defense May 30th, 2018 *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained herein. i Table of Contents List of Figures…………………………………………………………………….......... iv Acknowledgments…………………………………………………………………….. viii Abstract………………………………………………………………………………...... 1 CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION……………………………………………………. 3 1.1 Sensory Systems………………………………………………………………… 3 1.1.1 Why we need sensory systems……………………………………………… 3 1.1.2 The mammalian olfactory system………………………………………….. 6 1.2 State-Dependent Influences on Sensory Processing…………………………….. 8 1.2.1 Arousal, sleep, anesthesia, and the olfactory system………………………. 8 1.3 Cognitive Influences on Sensory Systems………………………………………. 10 1.3.1 Historical context…………………………………………………….......... 10 1.3.2 Attention…………………………………………………………………… 12 1.3.3 Modulation in vision and audition…………………………………………. 14 1.3.4 Modulation in chemosensory systems……………………………………… 16 1.4 Network vs Single-Unit Activity………………………………………………... 17 1.5 Olfactory Cortex Anatomy……………………………………………………… 19 1.5.1 The olfactory tubercle……………………………………………………… 19 1.6 Studying Attention in Non-Human Animal Models……………………….......... 21 1.6.1 Types of attention…………………………………………………………... 21 1.6.2 Psychophysics and attention in olfaction…………………………………... 21 1.6.3 Techniques to study attention in rodents…………………………………… 22 1.7 Questions and Hypotheses: State Influences on Network and SUA…………….. 24 1.7.1 State-dependent local network level modulation…………………………… 24 1.7.2 Attentional influences on behavior………………………………………… 26 1.7.3 Attention-dependent changes in neural coding……………………………. 26 CHAPTER 2: ODOR- AND STATE-DEPENDENT OLFACTORY TUBERCLE LOCAL FIELD POTENTIAL DYNAMICS IN AWAKE RATS…………………... 33 2.1 Abstract……………………………………………………………………....... 33 2.2 Introduction……………………………………………………………………... 34 2.3 Results…………………………………………………………………………... 37 2.3.1 Theta-band power dominates spontaneous network activity………………. 37 2.3.2 Odor stimulation evokes beta- and gamma-band increases in the OT……………………………………………………………………........... 38 2.3.3 Spontaneous and odor-evoked LFP activity within the OT is similar to the upstream OB……………………………………………………………. 40 2.3.4 Both spontaneous and odor-evoked activity correspond with functional coherence across all spectral bands……………………………………….. 42 2.3.5 Inhalation-triggered OT theta cycles lag behind those of the OB……….. 42 2.3.6 Sleep-like and anesthesia-dependent modulation of spontaneous activity……………………………………………………………………… 43 2.3.7 Limited influences of sleep-like states and anesthesia on odor-evoked activity……………………………………………………………………… 43 2.4 Discussion………………………………………………………………………. 47 2.5 Conclusions……………………………………………………………………. 53 2.6 Methods……………………………………………………………………......... 54 ii 2.7 Acknowledgments……………………………………………………………... 62 CHAPTER 3: SELECTIVE ATTENTION CONTROLS OLFACTORY DECISIONS AND THE NEURAL ENCODING OF ODORS…………………….. 83 3.1 Abstract………………………………………………………………………... 83 3.2 Introduction……………………………………………………………………... 84 3.3 Results…………………………………………………………………………. 86 3.3.1 Individual rat behavioral shaping on the CAT…………………………….. 87 3.3.2 Rats can selectively attend to odors, which dictates discrimination accuracy………………………………………………………………........ 87 3.3.3 Rats improve their ability to shift attention to odors with experience……… 88 3.3.4 Increased perceptual difficulty delays odor-directed shifts in attention…. 90 3.3.5 Additional influences of enhanced cognitive demand, trial congruency, and multisensory input on behavioral responses…………………………… 90 3.3.6 Odor-directed selective attention bi-directionally sculpts the encoding of odors in the OT………………………………………………………… 93 3.3.7 Odor-excited neurons increase their FRs, while odor-inhibited neurons further decrease their FRs with odor-directed attention…………………… 93 3.3.8 Odor-directed attention enhances the signal-to-noise ratio of odor- evoked units……………………………………………………………….. 96 3.3.9 FRs of units unmodulated by odor are not significantly different with attention……………………………………………………………………. 97 3.4 Discussion……………………………………………………………………... 98 3.5 Methods………………………………………………………………………... 102 3.6 Acknowledgments............................................................................................. 114 CHAPTER 4: DISCUSSION………………………………………………………... 139 4.1 Major Conclusions……………………………………………………………. 139 4.2 Influences of Selective Attention on Odor Coding…………………………….. 140 4.3 Caveats……………………………………………………………………........ 143 4.3.1 CAT design………………………………………………………………. 143 4.3.2 Attention and reward……………………………………………………... 147 4.3.3 Limits of extracellular techniques……………………………………….. 148 4.4 Future Directions………………………………………………………………. 150 4.4.1 Influences of increased attentional demand……………………………... 150 4.4.2 Multimodal influences……………………………………………………. 152 4.4.3 Active sampling………………………………………………………...... 152 4.4.4 Neuromodulatory influences……………………………………………. 153 4.4.6 Goal cells………………………………………………………………… 156 Bibliography………………………………………………………………………….. 169 iii List of Figures Figure 1-1. Hypotheses: Odor- and state-dependent modulation of local field potential activity within the OB and OT…………………………………………………29 Figure 1-2. Hypotheses: Odor-directed selective attention modulates performance accuracy, shapes sampling durations, and sculpts the underlying odor coding………….31 Figure 2-1. Experimental timeline and electrode tip locations for olfactory tubercle (OT) and olfactory bulb (OB) recordings...………...……………………………………63 Figure 2-2. Physiological classification of sleep state for state-dependent analysis...….65 Figure 2-3. OT LFP activity in awake rats……………………………………………...67 Figure 2-4. Odor-evoked modulation of OT LFP activity in awake rats…..…………...69 Figure 2-5. Spontaneous LFP activity within the OT compared with the upstream OB……………………………………………………………………………………….71 Figure 2-6. Similar odor-evoked power in the OT and OB………………………….....73 Figure 2-7. Spectral coherence of OT and OB LFP activity………………………...…75 iv Figure 2-8. Temporal dynamics of theta cycles in the OT and OB relative to respiration…………………………………………………………………………..…..77 Figure 2-9. Behavioral and anesthetic state impact the spontaneous LFP activity in the OT………….………………………………………………………………..……...79 Figure 2-10. Impact of anesthetic state on odor-evoked LFP in the OT….………...…81 Figure 3-1. Odor-directed attention dictates discrimination accuracy….………...…..115 Figure 3-2. Rats make fewer incongruent errors as they shift their attention, which leads to increased performance accuracy………………………………………..……117 Figure 3-3. OT units are bidirectionally modulated by odor-directed attention….….119 Figure 3-4. Odor-directed attention controls odor coding…………………….....…..121 Figure 3-5. Attention yields enhanced signal-to-noise among odor coding neurons…………………………………………………………………………….....123 Figure S3-1. Detailed structure of CAT shaping phases………………………….....125 Figure S3-2. Behavioral performance during task shaping…………………….........127 v Figure S3-3. Shifting to odor-directed attention is delayed when perceptual demand is enhanced…..…………………………………...…………………..……129 Figure S3-4. Selective attention influences subtle, yet critical aspects of olfactory behavior…………………………………….…………………..…….…..131 Figure S3-5. The majority of odor-unmodulated units have firing rate and signal-to-noise ratios that remain unchanged by attention…………………..….….133 Table S3-1. Total number of blocks and sessions to reach criterion across shaping phases 1-4…………………………………………………………..……..135 Table S3-2. Total number of blocks and sessions to reach criterion across multimodal and attention phases………………..…………………………..……..136 Table S3-3. Descriptive summary of single-neuron data used for analyses…...….137 Table S3-4. Body weights (bwts) during shaping and performance………..…….138 Figure 4-1. Future experiment: Task structure for elucidating neural effects of increased attentional demand……………………………………………...………159 vi Figure 4-2. The frequency of theta cycles increases during anticipatory hold and odor across task types………………………………….…..…………...…….161 Figure 4-3. Proposed experiment: Inhibiting cholinergic input from the HDB to the OT during performance of the CAT…………………………...…………..163 Figure 4-4. Example of a goal-directed unit that increases its FR with leftward movement toward the reward port……...……………………………….…..……165 Figure 4-5. Goal-directed units within the OT encode leftward movement…..…167 vii Acknowledgments I am deeply grateful to my advisor, Dr. Daniel Wesson, for his guidance, advice, and assistance throughout my doctoral studies. I was fortunate to have been able to train under his supervision and to have shared his enthusiasm for science. In the first few years, Dan offered an enriching laboratory environment, wherein he eagerly provided me with all of the tools and training I needed to succeed in his lab. This hands-on instruction was crucial to my development as a scientist, and no doubt, allowed
Recommended publications
  • Chemoreception
    Senses 5 SENSES live version • discussion • edit lesson • comment • report an error enses are the physiological methods of perception. The senses and their operation, classification, Sand theory are overlapping topics studied by a variety of fields. Sense is a faculty by which outside stimuli are perceived. We experience reality through our senses. A sense is a faculty by which outside stimuli are perceived. Many neurologists disagree about how many senses there actually are due to a broad interpretation of the definition of a sense. Our senses are split into two different groups. Our Exteroceptors detect stimulation from the outsides of our body. For example smell,taste,and equilibrium. The Interoceptors receive stimulation from the inside of our bodies. For instance, blood pressure dropping, changes in the gluclose and Ph levels. Children are generally taught that there are five senses (sight, hearing, touch, smell, taste). However, it is generally agreed that there are at least seven different senses in humans, and a minimum of two more observed in other organisms. Sense can also differ from one person to the next. Take taste for an example, what may taste great to me will taste awful to someone else. This all has to do with how our brains interpret the stimuli that is given. Chemoreception The senses of Gustation (taste) and Olfaction (smell) fall under the category of Chemoreception. Specialized cells act as receptors for certain chemical compounds. As these compounds react with the receptors, an impulse is sent to the brain and is registered as a certain taste or smell. Gustation and Olfaction are chemical senses because the receptors they contain are sensitive to the molecules in the food we eat, along with the air we breath.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children's Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing
    Understanding Sensory Processing: Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Communities of Practice in Autism September 24, 2009 Charlottesville, VA Dianne Koontz Lowman, Ed.D. Early Childhood Coordinator Region 5 T/TAC James Madison University MSC 9002 Harrisonburg, VA 22807 [email protected] ______________________________________________________________________________ Dianne Koontz Lowman/[email protected]/2008 Page 1 Looking at Children’s Behavior Through the Lens of Sensory Processing Do you know a child like this? Travis is constantly moving, pushing, or chewing on things. The collar of his shirt and coat are always wet from chewing. When talking to people, he tends to push up against you. Or do you know another child? Sierra does not like to be hugged or kissed by anyone. She gets upset with other children bump up against her. She doesn’t like socks with a heel or toe seam or any tags on clothes. Why is Travis always chewing? Why doesn’t Sierra liked to be touched? Why do children react differently to things around them? These children have different ways of reacting to the things around them, to sensations. Over the years, different terms (such as sensory integration) have been used to describe how children deal with the information they receive through their senses. Currently, the term being used to describe children who have difficulty dealing with input from their senses is sensory processing disorder. _____________________________________________________________________ Sensory Processing Disorder
    [Show full text]
  • The Olfactory Bulb Theta Rhythm Follows All Frequencies of Diaphragmatic Respiration in the Freely Behaving Rat
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Frontiers - Publisher Connector ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 11 June 2014 BEHAVIORAL NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00214 The olfactory bulb theta rhythm follows all frequencies of diaphragmatic respiration in the freely behaving rat Daniel Rojas-Líbano 1,2†, Donald E. Frederick 2,3, José I. Egaña 4 and Leslie M. Kay 1,2,3* 1 Committee on Neurobiology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA 2 Institute for Mind and Biology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA 3 Department of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA 4 Departamento de Anestesiología y Reanimación, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile Edited by: Sensory-motor relationships are part of the normal operation of sensory systems. Sensing Donald A. Wilson, New York occurs in the context of active sensor movement, which in turn influences sensory University School of Medicine, USA processing. We address such a process in the rat olfactory system. Through recordings of Reviewed by: the diaphragm electromyogram (EMG), we monitored the motor output of the respiratory Thomas A. Cleland, Cornell University, USA circuit involved in sniffing behavior, simultaneously with the local field potential (LFP) of Emmanuelle Courtiol, New York the olfactory bulb (OB) in rats moving freely in a familiar environment, where they display University Langone Medical Center, a wide range of respiratory frequencies. We show that the OB LFP represents the sniff USA cycle with high reliability at every sniff frequency and can therefore be used to study the *Correspondence: neural representation of motor drive in a sensory cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • Special Issue “Olfaction: from Genes to Behavior”
    G C A T T A C G G C A T genes Editorial Special Issue “Olfaction: From Genes to Behavior” Edgar Soria-Gómez 1,2,3 1 Department of Neurosciences, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, 48940 Leioa, Spain; [email protected] or [email protected] 2 Achucarro Basque Center for Neuroscience, Science Park of the UPV/EHU, 48940 Leioa, Spain 3 IKERBASQUE, Basque Foundation for Science, 48013 Bilbao, Spain Received: 12 June 2020; Accepted: 15 June 2020; Published: 15 June 2020 The senses dictate how the brain represents the environment, and this representation is the basis of how we act in the world. Among the five senses, olfaction is maybe the most mysterious and underestimated one, probably because a large part of the olfactory information is processed at the unconscious level in humans [1–4]. However, it is undeniable the influence of olfaction in the control of behavior and cognitive processes. Indeed, many studies demonstrate a tight relationship between olfactory perception and behavior [5]. For example, olfactory cues are determinant for partner selection [6,7], parental care [8,9], and feeding behavior [10–13], and the sense of smell can even contribute to emotional responses, cognition and mood regulation [14,15]. Accordingly, it has been shown that a malfunctioning of the olfactory system could be causally associated with the occurrence of important diseases, such as neuropsychiatric depression or feeding-related disorders [16,17]. Thus, a clear identification of the biological mechanisms involved in olfaction is key in the understanding of animal behavior in physiological and pathological conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Visual System: Higher Visual Processing
    The Visual System: Higher Visual Processing Primary visual cortex The primary visual cortex is located in the occipital cortex. It receives visual information exclusively from the contralateral hemifield, which is topographically represented and wherein the fovea is granted an extended representation. Like most cortical areas, primary visual cortex consists of six layers. It also contains, however, a prominent stripe of white matter in its layer 4 - the stripe of Gennari - consisting of the myelinated axons of the lateral geniculate nucleus neurons. For this reason, the primary visual cortex is also referred to as the striate cortex. The LGN projections to the primary visual cortex are segregated. The axons of the cells from the magnocellular layers terminate principally within sublamina 4Ca, and those from the parvocellular layers terminate principally within sublamina 4Cb. Ocular dominance columns The inputs from the two eyes also are segregated within layer 4 of primary visual cortex and form alternating ocular dominance columns. Alternating ocular dominance columns can be visualized with autoradiography after injecting radiolabeled amino acids into one eye that are transported transynaptically from the retina. Although the neurons in layer 4 are monocular, neurons in the other layers of the same column combine signals from the two eyes, but their activation has the same ocular preference. Bringing together the inputs from the two eyes at the level of the striate cortex provide a basis for stereopsis, the sensation of depth perception provided by binocular disparity, i.e., when an image falls on non-corresponding parts of the two retinas. Some neurons respond to disparities beyond the plane of fixation (far cells), while others respond to disparities in front of the plane of the fixation (near cells).
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perceptual Skills
    Super Duper® Handy Handouts!® Number 168 Guidelines for Identifying Visual Perceptual Problems in School-Age Children by Ann Stensaas, M.S., OTR/L We use our sense of sight to help us function in the world around us. It helps us figure out if something is near or far away, safe or threatening. Eyesight, also know as visual perception, refers to our ability to accurately identify and locate objects in our environment. Visual perception is an important component of vision that contributes to the way we see and interpret the world. What Is Visual Perception? Visual perception is our ability to process and organize visual information from the environment. This requires the integration of all of the body’s sensory experiences including sight, sound, touch, smell, balance, and movement. Most children are able to integrate these senses by the time they start school. This is important because approximately 75% of all classroom learning is visual. A child with even mild visual-perceptual difficulties will struggle with learning in the classroom and often in other areas of life. How Are Visual Perceptual Problems Diagnosed? A child with visual perceptual problems may be diagnosed with a visual processing disorder. He/she may be able to easily read an eye chart (acuity) but have difficulty organizing and making sense of visual information. In fact, many children with visual processing disorders have good acuity (i.e., 20/20 vision). A child with a visual-processing disorder may demonstrate difficulty discriminating between certain letters or numbers, putting together age-appropriate puzzles, or finding matching socks in a drawer.
    [Show full text]
  • AN OT's Toolbox : Making the Most out of Visual Processing and Motor Processing Skills
    AN OT’s Toolbox : Making the Most out of Visual Processing and Motor Processing Skills Presented by Beth Kelley, OTR/L, MIMC FOTA 2012 By Definition Visual Processing Motor Processing is is the sequence of steps synonymous with Motor that information takes as Skills Disorder which is it flows from visual any disorder characterized sensors to cognitive by inadequate development processing.1 of motor coordination severe enough to restrict locomotion or the ability to perform tasks, schoolwork, or other activities.2 1. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Visual_ processing 2. http://medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/Motor+skills+disorder Visual Processing What is Visual Processing? What are systems involved with Visual Processing? Is Visual Processing the same thing as vision? Review general anatomy of the eye. Review general functions of the eye. -Visual perception and the OT’s role. -Visual-Motor skills and why they are needed in OT treatment. What is Visual Processing “Visual processing is the sequence of steps that information takes as it flows from visual sensors to cognitive processing1” 1. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Visual_Processing What are the systems involved with Visual Processing? 12 Basic Processes are as follows: 1. Vision 2. Visual Motor Processing 3. Visual Discrimination 4. Visual Memory 5. Visual Sequential Memory 6. Visual Spatial Processing 7. Visual Figure Ground 8. Visual Form Constancy 9. Visual Closure 10. Binocularity 11.Visual Accommodation 12.Visual Saccades 12 Basic Processes are: 1. Vision The faculty or state of being able to see. The act or power of sensing with the eyes; sight. The Anatomy of Vision 6 stages in Development of the Vision system Birth to 4 months 4-6 months 6-8 months 8-12 months 1-2 years 2-3 years At birth babies can see patterns of light and dark.
    [Show full text]
  • Identification & Intervention with Sensory Processing
    An Introduction to Identification & Intervention for Children with Sensory Processing Difficulties EARLY CHILDHOOD MENTAL HEALTH INSTITUTE Location: University of Alaska Anchorage Date: May 13, 2009 Time: 8:30am to 12:00pm Jackie Brown, OTR/L Occupational Therapist Registered, Licensed Sensory Integration Certified Therapist #1602 Modulated and Advanced Therapeutic Listening Training All For Kids Pediatric Therapy Occupational Therapy/Physical Therapy Supervisor 8200 Homer Drive, Unit F Anchorage, AK 99518 Phone: (907) 345-0050 Email: [email protected] Website: www.allforkidsalaska.com Presentation Objectives WHAT IS IT? Define terms related to Sensory Processing Disorder (SPD). WHY IS IT IMPORTANT? Identify behaviors (signs and symptoms) associated with sensory processing difficulties. WHO DISCOVERED IT? WHERE HAVE WE BEEN? WHERE ARE WE GOING? Understand a brief history and look into current research. HOW DOES IT WORK? Identify the various sensory systems and their functions. WHAT DOES IT LOOK LIKE? Identify model of understand and describing Sensory Processing Disorders. WHEN DO I ACT and WHERE DO I GO FROM HERE? Identify when to refer a client to a specialist for a screening or evaluation. WHAT DO I DO NOW? Become familiar with simple intervention techniques to put into practice. What is Occupational Therapy? Occupational therapy is the scientifically based use of purposeful activity (or occupation) with individuals who are affected by physical injury or illness, psychosocial dysfunction, developmental or learning disabilities, or the aging process, in order to maximize independence, prevent disability, and promote health. WHAT IS SPD? What is Sensory Processing or Sensory Integration (SI)? Definitions The neurological process that organizes sensation form ones body and the environment and makes it possible to use the body effectively within the environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Anatomy and Physiology of the Afferent Visual System
    Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 102 (3rd series) Neuro-ophthalmology C. Kennard and R.J. Leigh, Editors # 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved Chapter 1 Anatomy and physiology of the afferent visual system SASHANK PRASAD 1* AND STEVEN L. GALETTA 2 1Division of Neuro-ophthalmology, Department of Neurology, Brigham and Womens Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA 2Neuro-ophthalmology Division, Department of Neurology, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA INTRODUCTION light without distortion (Maurice, 1970). The tear–air interface and cornea contribute more to the focusing Visual processing poses an enormous computational of light than the lens does; unlike the lens, however, the challenge for the brain, which has evolved highly focusing power of the cornea is fixed. The ciliary mus- organized and efficient neural systems to meet these cles dynamically adjust the shape of the lens in order demands. In primates, approximately 55% of the cortex to focus light optimally from varying distances upon is specialized for visual processing (compared to 3% for the retina (accommodation). The total amount of light auditory processing and 11% for somatosensory pro- reaching the retina is controlled by regulation of the cessing) (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). Over the past pupil aperture. Ultimately, the visual image becomes several decades there has been an explosion in scientific projected upside-down and backwards on to the retina understanding of these complex pathways and net- (Fishman, 1973). works. Detailed knowledge of the anatomy of the visual The majority of the blood supply to structures of the system, in combination with skilled examination, allows eye arrives via the ophthalmic artery, which is the first precise localization of neuropathological processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Sensory Processing Disorder with Strategies for Learning Behavior
    Sensory Processing Disorder with Strategies for learning Behavior and Social Skills Description: Understanding and learning to recognize the sensory needs of the children who have spectrum processing disorder. Bonnie Vos, MS, O.T.R./L History of Diagnosis Autism criterion Autism not it’s better defined, # Subtype own diagnostic of diagnosis s are category increased rapidly DSM I DSM II DSM II DSM DSM IV DSM V 1952 1968 1980 III-R 1992 2013 1987 Autism not it’s Autism New subtypes are own diagnostic becomes its added, now 16 category own diagnostic behaviors are category characteristic (must have 6 to qualify) History of Diagnosis 1. New name (Autism Spectrum Disorder) 2. One diagnosis-no subcategories 3. Now 2 domains (social and repetitive) 4. Symptom list is consolidated 5. Severity rating added 6. Co-morbid diagnosis is now permitted 7. Age onset criterion removed 8. New diagnosis: Social (pragmatic) Communication disorder What is Autism • http://www.parents.com/health/autism/histo ry-of-autism/ Coding of the brain • One theory of how the brain codes is using a library metaphor. • Books=experiences • Subjects areas are representations of the main neuro- cognitive domains: • The subjects are divided into 3 sections: 1. Sensory: visual, auditory, ect. 2. Integrated skills: motor planning (praxis), language 3. Abilities: social and cognitive • Designed as a model for ideal development The Brain Library • Each new experience creates a book associated with the subject area that are active during the experience • Books are symbolic of whole or parts of experiences • The librarian in our brain – Analyzes – Organizes – Stores – Retrieves Brain Library • The earliest experiences or sensory inputs begin writing the books that will eventually fill the foundational sections of our brain Libraries – Sensory integration begins in-utero – Example: vestibular=14 weeks in-utero – Example: Auditory=20 weeks in-utero Brain Library 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Language on Visual Perception
    Effects of Language on Visual Perception Gary Lupyan1a, Rasha Abdel Rahmanb, Lera Boroditskyc, Andy Clarkd aUniversity of Wisconsin-Madison bHumboldt-Universität zu Berlin cUniversity of California San Diego dUniversity of Sussex Abstract Does language change what we perceive? Does speaking different languages cause us to perceive things differently? We review the behavioral and elec- trophysiological evidence for the influence of language on perception, with an emphasis on the visual modality. Effects of language on perception can be observed both in higher-level processes such as recognition, and in lower-level processes such as discrimination and detection. A consistent finding is that language causes us to perceive in a more categorical way. Rather than being fringe or exotic, as they are sometimes portrayed, we discuss how effects of language on perception naturally arise from the interactive and predictive nature of perception. Keywords: language; perception; vision; categorization; top-down effects; prediction “Even comparatively simple acts of perception are very much more at the mercy of the social patterns called words than we might suppose.” [1]. “No matter how influential language might be, it would seem preposter- ous to a physiologist that it could reach down into the retina and rewire the ganglion cells” [2]. 1Correspondence: [email protected] Preprint submitted to Trends in Cognitive Sciences August 22, 2020 Language as a form of experience that affects perception What factors influence how we perceive the world? For example, what makes it possible to recognize the object in Fig. 1a? Or to locate the ‘target’ in Fig. 1b? Where is the head of the bird in Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 17: the Special Senses
    Chapter 17: The Special Senses I. An Introduction to the Special Senses, p. 550 • The state of our nervous systems determines what we perceive. 1. For example, during sympathetic activation, we experience a heightened awareness of sensory information and hear sounds that would normally escape our notice. 2. Yet, when concentrating on a difficult problem, we may remain unaware of relatively loud noises. • The five special senses are: olfaction, gustation, vision, equilibrium, and hearing. II. Olfaction, p. 550 Objectives 1. Describe the sensory organs of smell and trace the olfactory pathways to their destinations in the brain. 2. Explain what is meant by olfactory discrimination and briefly describe the physiology involved. • The olfactory organs are located in the nasal cavity on either side of the nasal septum. Figure 17-1a • The olfactory organs are made up of two layers: the olfactory epithelium and the lamina propria. • The olfactory epithelium contains the olfactory receptors, supporting cells, and basal (stem) cells. Figure 17–1b • The lamina propria consists of areolar tissue, numerous blood vessels, nerves, and olfactory glands. • The surfaces of the olfactory organs are coated with the secretions of the olfactory glands. Olfactory Receptors, p. 551 • The olfactory receptors are highly modified neurons. • Olfactory reception involves detecting dissolved chemicals as they interact with odorant-binding proteins. Olfactory Pathways, p. 551 • Axons leaving the olfactory epithelium collect into 20 or more bundles that penetrate the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone to reach the olfactory bulbs of the cerebrum where the first synapse occurs. • Axons leaving the olfactory bulb travel along the olfactory tract to reach the olfactory cortex, the hypothalamus, and portions of the limbic system.
    [Show full text]